SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  16
What is PERCEPTUAL LEARNING?
learning to understand the unions between stimulants and items in the
surroundings or the variations among stimulants.
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING: "Perceptual learning is acquired by all individuals
over time."
Psychology Dictionary: http://psychologydictionary.org/perceptual-learning/#ixzz2d4652bwh
Perceptual learning
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Psychology
Outline
History
Subfields
Basic types
Abnormal
Biological
Cognitive
Comparative
Cultural
Differential
Developmental
Evolutionary
Experimental
Mathematical
Personality
Positive
Quantitative
Social
Applied psychology
Applied behavior analysis
Clinical
Community
Consumer
Educational
Environmental
Forensic
Health
Industrial and organizational
Legal
Military
Occupational health
Political
Religion
School
Sport
Lists
Disciplines
Organizations
Psychologists
Psychotherapies
Publications
Research methods
Theories
Timeline
Topics
Psychology portal
V
T
E
Perceptual learning is the process of learning improved skills of perception. These improvements range from
simple sensory discriminations (e.g., distinguishing two musical tones from one another) to complex
categorizations of spatial and temporal patterns relevant to real-world expertise (e.g., reading, seeing relations
among chess pieces, knowing whether or not an X-ray image shows a tumor). Sensory modalities may include
visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory, and taste. Perceptual learning forms important foundations of complex
cognitive processes (i.e., language) and interacts with other kinds of learning to produce perceptual
expertise.[1][2]
Underlying perceptual learning are changes in the neural circuitry. The ability for perceptual
learning is retained throughout life.[3]
Contents
[hide]
1 Examples
o 1.1 Basic sensory discriminations
o 1.2 In the natural world
2 Brief history
3 Characteristics
o 3.1 Discovery and fluency effects
o 3.2 The role of attention
o 3.3 Time course of perceptual learning
4 Explanations and Models
o 4.1 Receptive field modification
o 4.2 Reverse hierarchy theory
o 4.3 Enrichment versus Differentiation
o 4.4 Selective reweighting
5 The impact of training protocol and the dynamics of learning
o 5.1 Consolidation and sleep
o 5.2 Comparison and contrast
o 5.3 Task difficulty
o 5.4 Active classification and attention
o 5.5 Feedback
o 5.6 Limits
6 Relations to other forms of learning
o 6.1 Declarative & procedural learning
o 6.2 Implicit learning
7 Applications
o 7.1 Improving perceptual skills
o 7.2 Technologies for perceptual learning
8 See also
9 References
Examples[edit source | editbeta]
Basic sensory discriminations[edit source | editbeta]
Laboratory studies reported many examples of dramatic improvements in sensitivities from appropriately
structured perceptual learning tasks. In visual Vernier acuity tasks, observers judge whether one line is
displaced above or below a second line. Untrained observers are often already very good with this task, but
after training, observers' threshold has been shown to improve as much as 6 fold.[4][5][6]
Similar improvements
have been found for visual motion discrimination [7]
and orientation sensitivity.[8][9]
In visual search tasks,
observers are asked to find a target object hidden among distractors or in noise. Studies of perceptual learning
with visual search show that experience lead to great gains in sensitivity and speed. In one study by Karni and
Sagi,[10]
the amount of time it took for subjects to search for an oblique line among a field of horizontal lines was
found to also improve dramatically, from about 200ms in one session to about 50ms in a later session. With
appropriate practice, visual search can become automatic and very efficient, such that observers do not need
more time to search when there are more items present on the search field.[11]
Tactile perceptual learning has
been demonstrated on spatial acuity tasks such as tactile grating orientation discrimination, and on vibrotactile
perceptual tasks such as frequency discrimination; tactile learning on these tasks has been found to transfer
from trained to untrained fingers.[12][13][14][15]
In the natural world[edit source | editbeta]
Perceptual learning is prevalent and occurs continuously in everyday life. As our perceptual system adapts to
the natural world, we become better at discriminating between different stimuli when they belong to different
categories than when they belong to the same category. We also tend to become less sensitive to the
differences between two instances of the same category.[16]
These effects are described as the result
of categorical perception. Categorical perception effects do not transfer across domains.
Young infants, when different sounds belong to the same phonetic category in their native language, tend to
lose sensitivity to differences between speech sounds by 10 months of age.[17]
They learn to pay attention to
salient differences between native phonetic categories, and ignore the less language-relevant ones. In chess,
expert chess players encode larger chunks of positions and relations on the board and require fewer exposures
to fully recreate a chess board. This is not due to their possessing superior visual skill, but rather to their
advanced extraction of structural patterns specific to chess.[18][19]
Extensive practice reading in English leads to extraction and rapid processing of the structural regularities of
English spelling patterns. The word superiority effect demonstrates this—people are often much faster at
recognizing words than individual letters.[20][21]
In speech phonemes, observers who listen to a continuum of equally spaced consonant-vowel syllables going
from /be/ to /de/ are much quicker to indicate that two syllables are different when they belonged to different
phonemic categories than when they were two variants of the same phoneme, even when physical differences
were equated between each pair of syllables.[22]
Other examples of perceptual learning in the natural world include the ability to distinguish between relative
pitches in music,[23]
identify tumors in x-rays,[24]
sort day-old chicks by gender,[25]
taste the subtle differences
between beers or wines,[26]
identify faces as belonging to different races,[27]
detect the features that distinguish
familiar faces,[28]
discriminate between two bird species ("great blue crown heron" and "chipping
sparrow"),[29]
and attend selectively to the hue, saturation and brightness values that comprise a color
definition.[30]
Brief history[edit source | editbeta]
The fact that with huge amounts of practice, individuals can reach impressive perceptual expertise, whether in
wine tasting, fabric evaluation or musical preference, has been well acknowledged for centuries, along with the
prevalent idiom that "practice makes perfect". The first documented report, dating to the mid-19th century, is
the earliest example of tactile training aimed at decreasing the minimal distance at which individuals can
discriminate whether one or two points on their skin have been touched. It was found that this distance (JND,
Just Noticeable Difference) decreases dramatically with practice, and that this improvement is at least partially
retained on subsequent days. Moreover, this improvement is at least partially specific to the trained skin area.
A particularly dramatic improvement was found for skin positions at which initial discrimination was very crude
(e.g. on the back), though training could not bring the JND of initially crude areas down to that of initially
accurate ones (e.g. finger tips).[31]
William James devoted a section in his Principles of Psychology (1890/1950)
to "the improvement in discrimination by practice.[32]
He noted examples and emphasized the importance of
perceptual learning for expertise. In 1918, Clark L. Hull, a noted learning theorist, trained human participants to
learn to categorize deformed Chinese characters into categories. For each category, he used 6 instances that
shared some invariant structural property. People learned to associate a sound as the name of each category,
and more importantly, they were able to classify novel characters accurately.[33]
This ability to extract
invariances from instances and apply them to classify new instances marked this study as a perceptual
learning experiment. It was not until 1969, however, that Eleanor Gibson published her seminal book The
Principles of Perceptual learning and Development and defined the modern field of perceptual learning. She
established the study of perceptual learning as an inquiry into the behavior and mechanism of perceptual
change. By the mid-1970s, however, this area was in a state of dormancy due to a shift in focus to perceptual
and cognitive development in infancy. Much of the scientific community tended to underestimate the impact of
learning compared with innate mechanisms. Thus, most of this research focused on characterizing basic
perceptual capacities of young infants rather than on perceptual learning processes.
Since the mid-1980s, there has been a new wave of interest in perceptual learning due to findings of cortical
plasticity at the lowest sensory levels of sensory systems. Our increased understanding of the physiology and
anatomy of our cortical systems has been used to connect the behavioral improvement to the underlying
cortical areas. This trend began with earlier findings of Hubel and Wiesel that perceptual representations at
sensory areas of the cortex are substantially modified during a short ("critical") period immediately following
birth. Merzenich, Kaas and colleagues showed that though neuroplasticity is diminished, it is not eliminated
when the critical period ends.[34]
Thus, when the external pattern of stimulation is substantially modified,
neuronal representations in lower-level (e.g. primary) sensory areas are also modified. Research in this period
centered on basic sensory discriminations, where remarkable improvements were found on almost any sensory
task through discrimination practice. Following training, subjects were tested with novel conditions and learning
transfer was assessed. This work departed from earlier work on perceptual learning, which spanned different
tasks and levels.
A question still debated today is to what extent improvements from perceptual learning stems from peripheral
modifications compared with improvement in higher-level readout stages. Early interpretations, such as that
suggested by William James, attributed it to higher-level categorization mechanisms whereby initially blurred
differences are gradually associated with distinctively different labels. The work focused on basic sensory
discrimination, however, suggests that the effects of perceptual learning are specific to changes in low-levels of
the sensory nervous system (i.e., primary sensory cortices).[35]
More recently, research suggest that perceptual
learning processes are multilevel and flexible.[36]
This cycles back to the earlier Gibsonian view that low-level
learning effects are modulated by high-level factors, and suggests that improvement in information extraction
may not involve only low-level sensory coding but also apprehension of relatively abstract structure and
relations in time and space.
Within the past decade, researchers have sought a more unified understanding of perceptual learning and
worked to apply these principles to improve perceptual learning in applied domains.
Characteristics[edit source | editbeta]
Discovery and fluency effects[edit source | editbeta]
Perceptual learning effects can be organized into two broad categories: discovery effects and fluency
effects.[1]
Discovery effects involve some change in the bases of response such as in selecting new information
relevant for the task, amplifying relevant information or suppressing irrelevant information. Experts extract
larger "chunks" of information and discover high-order relations and structures in their domains of expertise that
are invisible to novices. Fluency effects involve changes in the ease of extraction. Not only can experts process
high-order information, they do so with great speed and low attentional load. Discovery and fluency effects
work together so that as the discovery structures becomes more automatic, attentional resources are
conserved for discovery of new relations and for high-level thinking and problem-solving.
The role of attention[edit source | editbeta]
William James (Principles of Psychology, 1890) asserted that "My experience is what I agree to attend to. Only
those items which I notice shape my mind - without selective interest, experience is an utter chaos.".[32]
His
view was extreme, yet its gist was largely supported by subsequent behavioral and physiological studies. Mere
exposure does not seem to suffice for acquiring expertise.
Indeed, a relevant signal in a given behavioral condition may be considered noise in another. For example,
when presented with two similar stimuli, one might endeavor to study the differences between their
representations in order to improve one's ability to discriminate between them, or one may instead concentrate
on the similarities to improve one's ability to identify both as belonging to the same category. A specific
difference between them could be considered 'signal' in the first case and 'noise' in the second case. Thus, as
we adapt to tasks and environments, we pay increasingly more attention to the perceptual features that are
relevant and important for the task at hand, and at the same time, less attention to the irrelevant features. This
mechanism is called attentional weighting.[36]
However, recent studies suggest that perceptual learning occurs without selective attention.[37]
Studies of such
task-irrelevant perceptual learning (TIPL) show that the degree of TIPL is similar to that found through direct
training procedures.[38]
TIPL for a stimulus depends on the relationship between that stimulus and important
task events[39]
or upon stimulus reward contingencies.[40]
It has thus been suggested that learning (of task
irrelevant stimuli) is contingent upon spatially diffusive learning signals.[41]
Similar effects, but upon a shorter
time scale, have been found for memory processes and in some cases is called attentional boosting.[42]
Thus,
when an important (alerting) event occurs, learning may also affect concurrent, non-attended and non-salient
stimuli.[43]
Time course of perceptual learning[edit source | editbeta]
The time course of perceptual learning varies from one participant to another.[12]
Perceptual learning occurs not
only within the first training session but also between sessions.[44]
Fast learning (i.e., within-first-session
learning) and slow learning (i.e., between-session learning) involves different changes in the human adult brain.
While the fast learning effects can only be retained for a short term of several days, the slow learning effects
can be preserved for a long term over several months.[45]
Explanations and Models[edit source | editbeta]
Receptive field modification[edit source | editbeta]
Research on basic sensory discriminations often show that perceptual learning effects are specific to the
trained task or stimulus [46]
Many researchers take this to suggest that perceptual learning may work by
modifying the receptive fields of the cells (e.g., V1 and V2 cells) that initially encode the stimulus. For example,
individual cells could adapt to become more sensitive to important features, effectively recruiting more cells for
a particular purpose, making some cells more specifically tuned for the task at hand.[47]
Evidence for receptive
field change has been found using single-cell recording techniques in primates in both tactile and auditory
domains.[48]
However, not all perceptual learning tasks are specific to the trained stimuli or tasks. Sireteanu and
Rettenback [49]
discussed discrimination learning effects that generalize across eyes, retinal locations and
tasks. Ahissar and Hochstein [50]
used visual search to show that learning to detect a single line element hidden
in an array of differently-oriented line segments could generalize to positions at which the target was never
presented. In human vision, not enough receptive field modification has been found in early visual areas to
explain perceptual learning.[51]
Training that produces large behavioral changes such as improvements in
discrimination does not produce changes in receptive fields. In studies where changes have been found, the
changes are too small to explain changes in behavior.[52]
Reverse hierarchy theory[edit source | editbeta]
The Reverse Hierarchy Theory (RHT), proposed by Ahissar & Hochstein, aims to link between learning
dynamics and specificity and the underlying neuronal sites.[53]
RHT proposes that naïve performance is based
on responses at high-level cortical areas, where crude, categorical level representations of the environment are
represented. Hence initial learning stages involve understanding global aspects of the task. Subsequent
practice may yield better perceptual resolution as a consequence of accessing lower-level information via the
feedback connections going from high to low levels. Accessing the relevant low-level representations requires a
backward search during which informative input populations of neurons in the low level are allocated. Hence,
subsequent learning and its specificity reflect the resolution of lower levels. RHT thus proposes that initial
performance is limited by the high-level resolution whereas post-training performance is limited by the
resolution at low levels. Since high-level representations of different individuals differ due to their prior
experience, their initial learning patterns may differ. Several imaging studies are in line with this interpretation,
finding that initial performance is correlated with average (BOLD) responses at higher-level areas whereas
subsequent performance is more correlated with activity at lower-level areas[citation needed]
. RHT proposes that
modifications at low levels will occur only when the backward search (from high to low levels of processing) is
successful. Such success requires that the backward search will "know" which neurons in the lower level are
informative. This "knowledge" is gained by training repeatedly on a limited set of stimuli, such that the same
lower-level neuronal populations are informative during several trials. Recent studies found that mixing a broad
range of stimuli may also yield effective learning if these stimuli are clearly perceived as different, or are
explicitly tagged as different. These findings further support the requirement for top-down guidance in order to
obtain effective learning.
Enrichment versus Differentiation[edit source | editbeta]
In some complex perceptual tasks, all humans are experts. We are all experts at scene identification, face
identification and speech perception. Traditional explanations attribute this expertise to some holistic,
somewhat specialized, mechanisms. Perhaps such quick identifications are achieved by more specific and
complex perceptual detectors which gradually "chunk" (i.e., unitize) features that tend to concur, making it
easier to pull a whole set of information. Whether any concurrence of features can gradually be chunked with
practice or chunking can only be obtained with some pre-disposition (e.g. faces, phonological categories) is an
open question. Current findings suggest that such expertise is correlated with a significant increase in the
cortical volume involved in these processes. Thus, we all have somewhat specialized face areas, which may
reveal an innate property, but we also develop somewhat specialized areas for written words as opposed to
single letters or strings of letter-like symbols. Moreover, special experts in a given domain have larger cortical
areas involved in that domain. Thus, expert musicians have larger auditory areas.[54]
These observations are in
line with traditional theories of enrichment proposing that improved performance involves an increase in cortical
representation. For this expertise, basic categorical identification may be based on enriched and detailed
representations, located to some extent in specialized brain areas. Physiological evidence suggests that
training for refined discrimination along basic dimensions (e.g. frequency in the auditory modality) also
increases the representation of the trained parameters, though in these cases the increase may mainly involve
lower-level sensory areas.[55]
Selective reweighting[edit source | editbeta]
In 2005, Petrov, Dosher and Lu pointed out that perceptual learning may be explained in terms of the selection
of which analyzers best perform the classification, even in simple discrimination tasks. They explain that the
some part of the neural system responsible for particular decisions have specificity, while low-level perceptual
units do not.[36]
In their model, encodings at the lowest level do not change. Rather, changes that occur in
perceptual learning arise from changes in higher-level, abstract representations of the relevant stimuli. Because
specificity can come from differentially selecting information, this "selective reweighting theory" allows for
learning of complex, abstract representation. This corresponds to Gibson’s earlier account of perceptual
learning as selection and learning of distinguishing features. Selection may be the unifying principles of
perceptual learning at all levels.[56]
The impact of training protocol and the dynamics of learning[edit
source | editbeta]
Ivan Pavlov discovered conditioning. He found that when a stimulus (e.g. sound) is immediately followed by
food several times, the mere presentation of this stimulus would subsequently elicit saliva in a dog's mouth. He
further found that when he used a differential protocol, by consistently presenting food after one stimulus while
not presenting food after another stimulus, dogs were quickly conditioned to selectively salivate in response to
the rewarded one. He then asked whether this protocol could be used to increase perceptual discrimination, by
differentially rewarding two very similar stimuli (e.g. tones with similar frequency). However, he found that
differential conditioning was not effective.
Pavlov's studies were followed by many training studies which found that an effective way to increase
perceptual resolution is to begin with a large difference along the required dimension and gradually proceed to
small differences along this dimension. This easy-to-difficult transfer was termed "transfer along a continuum".
These studies showed that the dynamics of learning depend on the training protocol, rather than on the total
amount of practice. Moreover, it seems that the strategy implicitly chosen for learning is highly sensitive to the
choice of the first few trials during which the system tries to identify the relevant cues.
Consolidation and sleep[edit source | editbeta]
Several studies asked whether learning takes place during practice sessions or in between, for example, during
subsequent sleep. The dynamics of learning are hard to evaluate since the directly measured parameter is
performance, which is affected by both learning, inducing improvement, and fatigue, which hampers
performance. Current studies suggest that sleep contributes to improved and durable learning effects, by
further strengthening connections in the absence of continued practice.[44][57][58]
Both slow-wave and REM (rapid
eye movement) stages of sleep may contribute to this process, via not-yet-understood mechanisms.
Comparison and contrast[edit source | editbeta]
Practice with comparison and contrast of instances that belong to the same or different categories allow for the
pick-up of the distinguishing features—features that are important for the classification task—and the filter of
the irrelevant features [59]
Task difficulty[edit source | editbeta]
Learning easy examples first may lead to better transfer and better learning of more difficult cases.[60]
By
recording ERPs from human adults, Ding and Colleagues investigated the influence of task difficulty on the
brain mechanisms of visual perceptual learning. Results showed that difficult task training affected earlier visual
processing stage and broader visual cortical regions than easy task training.[61]
Active classification and attention[edit source | editbeta]
Active classification effort and attention are often necessary to produce perceptual learning effects.[58]
However,
in some cases, mere exposure to certain stimulus variations can produce improved discriminations.
Feedback[edit source | editbeta]
In many cases, perceptual learning does not require feedback (whether or not the classification is
correct).[55]
Other studies suggest that block feedback (feedback only after a block of trials) produces more
learning effects than no feedback at all.[62]
Limits[edit source | editbeta]
Despite the marked perceptual learning demonstrated in different sensory systems and under varied training
paradigms, it is clear that perceptual learning must face certain unsurpassable limits imposed by the physical
characteristics of the sensory system. For instance, in tactile spatial acuity tasks, experiments suggest that the
extent of learning is limited by fingertip surface area, which may constrain the underlying density
of mechanoreceptors.[12]
Relations to other forms of learning[edit source | editbeta]
Declarative & procedural learning[edit source | editbeta]
In many domains of expertise in the real world, perceptual learning interacts with other forms of
learning. Declarative knowledge tends to occur with perceptual learning. As we learn to distinguish between an
array of wine flavors, we also develop a wide range of vocabularies to describe the intricacy of each flavor.
Similarly, perceptual learning also interacts flexibly with procedural knowledge. For example, the perceptual
expertise of a baseball player at bat can detect early in the ball's flight whether the pitcher threw a curveball.
However, the perceptual differentiation of the feel of swinging the bat in various ways may also have been
involved in learning the motor commands that produce the required swing.[1]
Implicit learning[edit source | editbeta]
Perceptual learning is often said to be implicit, such that learning occurs without awareness. It is not at all clear
whether perceptual learning is always implicit. Changes in sensitivity that arise are often not conscious and do
not involve conscious procedures, but perceptual information can be mapped onto various responses.[1]
In complex perceptual learning tasks (e.g., sorting of newborn chicks by gender, playing chess), experts are
often unable to explain what stimulus relationships they are using in classification. However, in less complex
perceptual learning tasks, people can point out what information they’re using to make classifications.
Applications[edit source | editbeta]
Improving perceptual skills[edit source | editbeta]
An important potential application of perceptual learning is the acquisition of skill for practical purposes. Thus it
is important to understand whether training for increased resolution in lab conditions induces a general upgrade
which transfers to other environmental contexts, or results from mechanisms which are context specific.
Improving complex skills is typically gained by training under complex simulation conditions rather than one
component at a time. Recent lab-based training protocols with complex action computer games have shown
that such practice indeed modifies visual skills in a general way, which transfers to new visual contexts. In
2010, Achtman, Green, and Bavelier reviewed the research on video games to train visual skills.[53]
They cite a
previous review by Green & Bavelier (2006) [63]
on using video games to enhance perceptual and cognitive
abilities. A variety of skills were upgraded in video game players, including "improved hand-eye
coordination,[64]
increased processing in the periphery,[65]
enhanced mental rotation skills,[66]
greater divided
attention abilities,[67]
and faster reaction times,[68]
to name a few". An important characteristic is the functional
increase in the size of the effective visual field (within which viewers can identify objects), which is trained in
action games and transfers to new settings. Whether learning of simple discriminations, which are trained in
separation, transfers to new stimulus contexts (e.g. complex stimulus conditions) is still an open question.
Like experimental procedures, other attempts to apply perceptual learning methods to basic and complex skills
use training situations in which the learner receives many short classification trials. Tallal, Merzenich and their
colleagues have successfully adapted auditory discrimination paradigms to address speech and language
difficulties.[69][70]
They reported improvements in language learning-impaired children using specially enhanced
and extended speech signals. The results applied not only to auditory discrimination performance but speech
and language comprehension as well.
Technologies for perceptual learning[edit source | editbeta]
In educational domains, recent efforts by Philip Kellman and colleagues showed that perceptual learning can
be systematically produced and accelerated using specific, computer-based technology. Their approach to
perceptual learning methods take the form of perceptual learning modules (PLMs): sets of short, interactive
trials that develop, in a particular domain, learners’ pattern recognition, classification abilities, and their abilities
to map across multiple representations. As a result of practice with mapping across transformations (e.g.,
algebra, fractions) and across multiple representations (e.g., graphs, equations, and word problems), students
show dramatic gains in their structure recognition in fraction learning and algebra. They also demonstrated that
when students practice classifying algebraic transformations using PLMs, the results show remarkable
improvements in fluency at algebra problem solving.[71][72][73]
These results suggests that perceptual learning
can offer a needed complement to conceptual and procedural instructions in the classroom.
Similar results have also been replicated in other domains with PLMs, including anatomic recognition in
medical and surgical training,[74]
reading instrumental flight displays,[75]
and apprehending molecular structures
in chemistry.[76]
Perceptual learning in humans occurs when a person is repeatedly exposed to specific stimuli (information).
Perceptual learning involves long lasting and amazing changes to the human perceptual system that incredibly
improve one’s ability to respond to the environment. The mechanisms of perceptual learning include attention
weighting, imprinting, differentiation, and unitization. With attention weighting, perception becomes adapted to tasks
by increasing the attention paid to important dimensions and features. With imprinting, special receptors are
developed that are specialized for specific stimuli. With differentiation, stimuli that were once indistinguishable
become psychologically separated. With unitization, tasks that originally required detection of several components
are accomplished by detecting a single construct.
Because dramatic changes to human perceptual systems occur within the first six years of life (and the younger the
more dramatic), your child’s training in perceptual learning in reading and maths must occur during this period in
order to achieve effortless learning for success and happiness in school and life.
Attention Weighting
Perception becomes adapted to tasks by increasing the attention paid to important features and by decreasing
attention to irrelevant features. Both Perceptual Maths™ and Perceptual Reading™ expose materials that are
pertinent to learning in each discipline. Your child will perceptually learn to pay attention to those features that are
important and ignore those that are not. Often children without perceptual learning in maths and reading will waste
processing time on irrelevant features. And not devoting enough processing time to relevant features means
problems become unnecessarily more difficult to solve.
Stimulus Imprinting
Through imprinting, receptors that are specialized for stimuli or parts of stimuli are developed. Imprinting implies that
the receptor is shaped by the pertinent stimulus. Receptors develop for repeated stimuli, and these receptors
increase the speed, accuracy, and fluency with which the stimuli are processed. In Perceptual Maths™ the stimuli are
quantities (dots), numerals, operations and clocks. In Perceptual Reading™ the stimuli are phonemes, graphemes,
words, phrases and sentences.
Whole Stimulus Imprinting
As more instances of maths and reading materials are stored, performance improves because more relevant
instances can be retrieved, and the time required for retrieving them decreases. Your child’s performance in
perceptual tasks is dependent on the amount of their experience with a particular stimulus. Performance is better on
frequently presented items than rare items. Perceptual Maths™ and Perceptual Reading™ expose relevant materials
that improve their performance in maths and reading. As your child gets older they will be better able to perceptually
identify unclear or quickly presented maths and reading materials because they will have already been exposed to
them during a period of perceptual plasticity.
Feature Imprinting
As well as imprinting on entire stimuli, the human perceptual system also imprints on parts of a stimulus. If a stimulus
part is important, varies independently of other parts, or occurs frequently, people develop a specialized receptor for
that part. This leads to the development of new building blocks for describing stimuli. Parts that are developed in one
context can be used to efficiently describe subsequent objects. Efficient representations are promoted because the
parts have been extracted for their prevalence. When new maths or reading problems present themselves, your child
will be able to extract relevant features and relate them to their prior learning, which thus builds upon their learning.
Topological Imprinting
Topological imprinting occurs when the space and the positions of patterns within the space are learned as a result of
training. Rather than simply developing independent receptors, topological imprinting implies that a spatially
organized network of receptors is created. In Perceptual Maths™ the clock and single line equations expose the child
to these spatial patterns. The spatial abilities will benefit your child’s future problem solving. In Perceptual Reading™
the spatial abilities will help comprehension by rapid pickup and processing of text.
Differentiation
With differentiation, stimuli that were once psychologically fused become separated. Once separated, distinction can
be made between components that were originally indistinguishable. Differentiation is crucial in understanding maths
and reading insofar as understanding the subtle yet important differences in text, numbers and their operations.
Differentiation also occurs at the levels of whole stimuli and features within stimuli.
Whole Stimuli Differentiation
Perceptual adaptation involves developing increasingly differentiated object representations rather than its features.
In Perceptual Reading™, this applies to whole words, phrases, sentences and beyond, rather than letters. In
Perceptual Maths™, whole stimuli are whole equations and clocks rather than individual numbers and operators.
Category
Learning involves dividing a large, general category into subcategories. Experts in a field have several differentiated
categories where a novice has only a single category. For example a computer expert would know the computer as
the sum of CPU, memory, storage, BUS etc, while a novice would only see the computer as a box. By the end of the
course, at age 3, your child will have been an expert in reading and maths; having had many categories with which to
differentiate reading and maths problems.
Dimensions Differentiation
Just as experience can lead to the psychological separation of stimuli or categories, it can also lead to the separation
of perceptual dimensions that comprise a single stimulus. Dimensions that are originally treated as fused often
become separated with perceptual training. Your child will shift from perceiving stimuli in terms of the holistic to
analytically decomposing them into separate dimensions. For example, unlike many young children, your child will be
able to differentiate between the letters b, d, p and q at a subconscious level. The process of decomposition gives
your perceptually trained child the ability to solve problems well beyond what has been taught.
Unitization
A task that originally required detection of several parts can be accomplished by detecting a single complex unit.
Using the above example, a computer can be perceptually identified as a whole object by an expert while a novice
needs to systematically list the components in order to identify the same computer. At last, as a culmination of
Perceptual Maths™ and Perceptual Reading™, your child will have perceptually learnt whole words, phrases,
sentences, equations and clocks as single units. Thus, retrieval and processing are more efficient and at more rapid
speeds and fluency.
Conclusion
Perceptual learning in your child occurs when they are repeatedly exposed to specific reading and maths concepts.
These long lasting and amazing changes to their perceptual system incredibly improve their ability to effectively and
positively respond to life and school.
At any given time, what can be learned depends on what has already been learned, perceptually and cognitively.
Therefore your child, having learnt a great deal by the age of three, will have a powerful unfair advantage over the
average population. And they deserve it. Your child will possess incredible superior learning skills that permit them to
stay well ahead of their peers throughout life. They will achieve effortless lifelong learning for success and happiness
in school and life.

Contenu connexe

Tendances

Tendances (20)

Psychodynamic Model
Psychodynamic ModelPsychodynamic Model
Psychodynamic Model
 
Social Identity Theory
Social Identity TheorySocial Identity Theory
Social Identity Theory
 
Indian psychology
Indian psychologyIndian psychology
Indian psychology
 
Ethical issues in psychological research
Ethical issues in psychological researchEthical issues in psychological research
Ethical issues in psychological research
 
Psychodynamic theory
Psychodynamic theoryPsychodynamic theory
Psychodynamic theory
 
Ethical Issues in Assessment
Ethical Issues in AssessmentEthical Issues in Assessment
Ethical Issues in Assessment
 
Free Association
Free AssociationFree Association
Free Association
 
Nature and use of Psychological Tests
Nature and use of Psychological TestsNature and use of Psychological Tests
Nature and use of Psychological Tests
 
Representation of knowledge
Representation of knowledgeRepresentation of knowledge
Representation of knowledge
 
Lesson 42
Lesson 42Lesson 42
Lesson 42
 
Ablation & lesion
Ablation & lesionAblation & lesion
Ablation & lesion
 
Carl Jung Theory of Personality
Carl Jung Theory of PersonalityCarl Jung Theory of Personality
Carl Jung Theory of Personality
 
SENTENCE COMPLETION TESTS AND DRAWING TESTS
SENTENCE COMPLETION TESTS AND DRAWING TESTSSENTENCE COMPLETION TESTS AND DRAWING TESTS
SENTENCE COMPLETION TESTS AND DRAWING TESTS
 
Neo personality inventory
Neo personality inventoryNeo personality inventory
Neo personality inventory
 
Schema theory
Schema theorySchema theory
Schema theory
 
ETHICAL STANDARDS IN TESTING.
ETHICAL STANDARDS IN TESTING.ETHICAL STANDARDS IN TESTING.
ETHICAL STANDARDS IN TESTING.
 
Humanistic Psychology
Humanistic PsychologyHumanistic Psychology
Humanistic Psychology
 
Ethical issues psychology
Ethical issues psychologyEthical issues psychology
Ethical issues psychology
 
Physiology of perception
Physiology of perceptionPhysiology of perception
Physiology of perception
 
Aggression
AggressionAggression
Aggression
 

En vedette

Psychology
PsychologyPsychology
Psychologyjoan_29
 
Perception and learning
Perception and learningPerception and learning
Perception and learningyousuf adil
 
Intuitive Appeal of Learning Styles
Intuitive Appeal of Learning StylesIntuitive Appeal of Learning Styles
Intuitive Appeal of Learning StylesLuis Pinto
 
The greek educational thinkers
The greek educational thinkersThe greek educational thinkers
The greek educational thinkersKristoffer Arroyo
 
Protestant Reformation
Protestant ReformationProtestant Reformation
Protestant ReformationMr.J
 
The Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest Philosophers
The Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest PhilosophersThe Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest Philosophers
The Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest PhilosophersCJ F.
 
Reformation
ReformationReformation
Reformationsuwalden
 
Introduction to sociological foundations
Introduction to sociological foundationsIntroduction to sociological foundations
Introduction to sociological foundationsAaron James Lico
 
Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)
Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)
Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)Ronald Espiritu
 

En vedette (20)

Psychology
PsychologyPsychology
Psychology
 
Perception and learning
Perception and learningPerception and learning
Perception and learning
 
Intuitive Appeal of Learning Styles
Intuitive Appeal of Learning StylesIntuitive Appeal of Learning Styles
Intuitive Appeal of Learning Styles
 
The greek educational thinkers
The greek educational thinkersThe greek educational thinkers
The greek educational thinkers
 
philosophy of educators
philosophy of educatorsphilosophy of educators
philosophy of educators
 
AP Counter Reformation
AP Counter ReformationAP Counter Reformation
AP Counter Reformation
 
Foundation of-education-history
Foundation of-education-historyFoundation of-education-history
Foundation of-education-history
 
The Guild system
The Guild systemThe Guild system
The Guild system
 
Guilds
GuildsGuilds
Guilds
 
Protestant Reformation
Protestant ReformationProtestant Reformation
Protestant Reformation
 
The Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest Philosophers
The Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest PhilosophersThe Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest Philosophers
The Educational Philosophies of the World’s Greatest Philosophers
 
Sociological foundation of education
Sociological foundation of educationSociological foundation of education
Sociological foundation of education
 
Reformation
ReformationReformation
Reformation
 
The Protestant Reformation
The Protestant ReformationThe Protestant Reformation
The Protestant Reformation
 
Perception Ppt New
Perception Ppt NewPerception Ppt New
Perception Ppt New
 
The reformation
The reformationThe reformation
The reformation
 
Introduction to sociological foundations
Introduction to sociological foundationsIntroduction to sociological foundations
Introduction to sociological foundations
 
Cognitive Learning Theory
Cognitive Learning TheoryCognitive Learning Theory
Cognitive Learning Theory
 
Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)
Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)
Philosophy of Education in the Philippine Setting (Hermeneutics)
 
Humanism
HumanismHumanism
Humanism
 

Similaire à Perceptual learning

Book Obama Promised Land
Book Obama Promised LandBook Obama Promised Land
Book Obama Promised Landelhosanyreda
 
POWERFUL Brain & Hearing Support
POWERFUL Brain & Hearing SupportPOWERFUL Brain & Hearing Support
POWERFUL Brain & Hearing Supportelhosanyreda
 
Essay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essay
Essay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essayEssay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essay
Essay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essaySamantha Brown
 
Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.
Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.
Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.iosrjce
 
From inclusion to creativity through haptic drawing
From inclusion to creativity through haptic drawingFrom inclusion to creativity through haptic drawing
From inclusion to creativity through haptic drawingIla Angah
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageMery Machie
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageDorothy Bishop
 
The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay. Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...
The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay.  Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay.  Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...
The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay. Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...Elizabeth Montes
 
The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...
The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...
The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...James Cook University
 
Methods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptx
Methods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptxMethods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptx
Methods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptxSubramanian Mani
 
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and HopeReport – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and HopeDyslexia International
 
2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx
2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx
2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docxdomenicacullison
 
ResearchEd Interweaving
ResearchEd Interweaving ResearchEd Interweaving
ResearchEd Interweaving Mark Enser
 
Teaching English Pronunciation to Spanish Speakers
Teaching English Pronunciation to Spanish SpeakersTeaching English Pronunciation to Spanish Speakers
Teaching English Pronunciation to Spanish Speakersinventionjournals
 
Theories and hypothesis in psycholinguistics
Theories and hypothesis in psycholinguisticsTheories and hypothesis in psycholinguistics
Theories and hypothesis in psycholinguisticsAbolfazl Ghanbary
 

Similaire à Perceptual learning (20)

Book Obama Promised Land
Book Obama Promised LandBook Obama Promised Land
Book Obama Promised Land
 
POWERFUL Brain & Hearing Support
POWERFUL Brain & Hearing SupportPOWERFUL Brain & Hearing Support
POWERFUL Brain & Hearing Support
 
Sla
SlaSla
Sla
 
Sla
SlaSla
Sla
 
Cogmaster 2011_Ep2
Cogmaster 2011_Ep2Cogmaster 2011_Ep2
Cogmaster 2011_Ep2
 
Cogmaster_Ep2
Cogmaster_Ep2Cogmaster_Ep2
Cogmaster_Ep2
 
Essay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essay
Essay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essayEssay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essay
Essay On Hardwork. Narrative Essay: Hard work leads to success essay
 
Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.
Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.
Effect of depth perception on motor-coordinative ability of tribal adolescents.
 
From inclusion to creativity through haptic drawing
From inclusion to creativity through haptic drawingFrom inclusion to creativity through haptic drawing
From inclusion to creativity through haptic drawing
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn language
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn language
 
The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay. Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...
The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay.  Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay.  Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...
The Pursuit Of Happiness Essay. Analysis of Film The Pursuit of Happiness Fr...
 
The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...
The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...
The relationship between multiple intelligences and reading proficiency of ir...
 
Methods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptx
Methods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptxMethods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptx
Methods and Theories of Languaage learning.pptx
 
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and HopeReport – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
 
2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx
2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx
2Resources for the Integrative Literature Review8Resou.docx
 
ResearchEd Interweaving
ResearchEd Interweaving ResearchEd Interweaving
ResearchEd Interweaving
 
Learning activity 1.4
Learning activity 1.4Learning activity 1.4
Learning activity 1.4
 
Teaching English Pronunciation to Spanish Speakers
Teaching English Pronunciation to Spanish SpeakersTeaching English Pronunciation to Spanish Speakers
Teaching English Pronunciation to Spanish Speakers
 
Theories and hypothesis in psycholinguistics
Theories and hypothesis in psycholinguisticsTheories and hypothesis in psycholinguistics
Theories and hypothesis in psycholinguistics
 

Dernier

Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL CertsScanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL CertsRizwan Syed
 
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfUnraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfAlex Barbosa Coqueiro
 
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine TuningDSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine TuningLars Bell
 
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024BookNet Canada
 
Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)
Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)
Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)Mark Simos
 
Hyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdf
Hyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdfHyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdf
Hyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdfPrecisely
 
SALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICES
SALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICESSALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICES
SALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICESmohitsingh558521
 
The Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxThe Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.
How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.
How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.Curtis Poe
 
How to write a Business Continuity Plan
How to write a Business Continuity PlanHow to write a Business Continuity Plan
How to write a Business Continuity PlanDatabarracks
 
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding ClubUnleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding ClubKalema Edgar
 
Advanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An IntroductionAdvanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An IntroductionDilum Bandara
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024BookNet Canada
 
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxUse of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
The Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxThe Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxLoriGlavin3
 
The Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and Cons
The Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and ConsThe Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and Cons
The Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and ConsPixlogix Infotech
 
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .Alan Dix
 
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQLDeveloper Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQLScyllaDB
 
Commit 2024 - Secret Management made easy
Commit 2024 - Secret Management made easyCommit 2024 - Secret Management made easy
Commit 2024 - Secret Management made easyAlfredo García Lavilla
 

Dernier (20)

DMCC Future of Trade Web3 - Special Edition
DMCC Future of Trade Web3 - Special EditionDMCC Future of Trade Web3 - Special Edition
DMCC Future of Trade Web3 - Special Edition
 
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL CertsScanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
 
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfUnraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
 
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine TuningDSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
DSPy a system for AI to Write Prompts and Do Fine Tuning
 
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
Transcript: New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
 
Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)
Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)
Tampa BSides - Chef's Tour of Microsoft Security Adoption Framework (SAF)
 
Hyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdf
Hyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdfHyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdf
Hyperautomation and AI/ML: A Strategy for Digital Transformation Success.pdf
 
SALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICES
SALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICESSALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICES
SALESFORCE EDUCATION CLOUD | FEXLE SERVICES
 
The Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxThe Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Fit for Passkeys for Employee and Consumer Sign-ins: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.
How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.
How AI, OpenAI, and ChatGPT impact business and software.
 
How to write a Business Continuity Plan
How to write a Business Continuity PlanHow to write a Business Continuity Plan
How to write a Business Continuity Plan
 
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding ClubUnleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
 
Advanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An IntroductionAdvanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
Advanced Computer Architecture – An Introduction
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC CataList - Tech Forum 2024
 
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxUse of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
Use of FIDO in the Payments and Identity Landscape: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
The Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptxThe Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
The Role of FIDO in a Cyber Secure Netherlands: FIDO Paris Seminar.pptx
 
The Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and Cons
The Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and ConsThe Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and Cons
The Ultimate Guide to Choosing WordPress Pros and Cons
 
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
From Family Reminiscence to Scholarly Archive .
 
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQLDeveloper Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
Developer Data Modeling Mistakes: From Postgres to NoSQL
 
Commit 2024 - Secret Management made easy
Commit 2024 - Secret Management made easyCommit 2024 - Secret Management made easy
Commit 2024 - Secret Management made easy
 

Perceptual learning

  • 1. What is PERCEPTUAL LEARNING? learning to understand the unions between stimulants and items in the surroundings or the variations among stimulants. PERCEPTUAL LEARNING: "Perceptual learning is acquired by all individuals over time." Psychology Dictionary: http://psychologydictionary.org/perceptual-learning/#ixzz2d4652bwh Perceptual learning From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Psychology Outline History Subfields Basic types Abnormal Biological Cognitive Comparative Cultural Differential
  • 2. Developmental Evolutionary Experimental Mathematical Personality Positive Quantitative Social Applied psychology Applied behavior analysis Clinical Community Consumer Educational Environmental Forensic Health Industrial and organizational Legal
  • 4. T E Perceptual learning is the process of learning improved skills of perception. These improvements range from simple sensory discriminations (e.g., distinguishing two musical tones from one another) to complex categorizations of spatial and temporal patterns relevant to real-world expertise (e.g., reading, seeing relations among chess pieces, knowing whether or not an X-ray image shows a tumor). Sensory modalities may include visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory, and taste. Perceptual learning forms important foundations of complex cognitive processes (i.e., language) and interacts with other kinds of learning to produce perceptual expertise.[1][2] Underlying perceptual learning are changes in the neural circuitry. The ability for perceptual learning is retained throughout life.[3] Contents [hide] 1 Examples o 1.1 Basic sensory discriminations o 1.2 In the natural world 2 Brief history 3 Characteristics o 3.1 Discovery and fluency effects o 3.2 The role of attention o 3.3 Time course of perceptual learning 4 Explanations and Models o 4.1 Receptive field modification o 4.2 Reverse hierarchy theory o 4.3 Enrichment versus Differentiation o 4.4 Selective reweighting 5 The impact of training protocol and the dynamics of learning o 5.1 Consolidation and sleep o 5.2 Comparison and contrast o 5.3 Task difficulty
  • 5. o 5.4 Active classification and attention o 5.5 Feedback o 5.6 Limits 6 Relations to other forms of learning o 6.1 Declarative & procedural learning o 6.2 Implicit learning 7 Applications o 7.1 Improving perceptual skills o 7.2 Technologies for perceptual learning 8 See also 9 References Examples[edit source | editbeta] Basic sensory discriminations[edit source | editbeta] Laboratory studies reported many examples of dramatic improvements in sensitivities from appropriately structured perceptual learning tasks. In visual Vernier acuity tasks, observers judge whether one line is displaced above or below a second line. Untrained observers are often already very good with this task, but after training, observers' threshold has been shown to improve as much as 6 fold.[4][5][6] Similar improvements have been found for visual motion discrimination [7] and orientation sensitivity.[8][9] In visual search tasks, observers are asked to find a target object hidden among distractors or in noise. Studies of perceptual learning with visual search show that experience lead to great gains in sensitivity and speed. In one study by Karni and Sagi,[10] the amount of time it took for subjects to search for an oblique line among a field of horizontal lines was found to also improve dramatically, from about 200ms in one session to about 50ms in a later session. With appropriate practice, visual search can become automatic and very efficient, such that observers do not need more time to search when there are more items present on the search field.[11] Tactile perceptual learning has been demonstrated on spatial acuity tasks such as tactile grating orientation discrimination, and on vibrotactile perceptual tasks such as frequency discrimination; tactile learning on these tasks has been found to transfer from trained to untrained fingers.[12][13][14][15] In the natural world[edit source | editbeta] Perceptual learning is prevalent and occurs continuously in everyday life. As our perceptual system adapts to the natural world, we become better at discriminating between different stimuli when they belong to different categories than when they belong to the same category. We also tend to become less sensitive to the differences between two instances of the same category.[16] These effects are described as the result of categorical perception. Categorical perception effects do not transfer across domains.
  • 6. Young infants, when different sounds belong to the same phonetic category in their native language, tend to lose sensitivity to differences between speech sounds by 10 months of age.[17] They learn to pay attention to salient differences between native phonetic categories, and ignore the less language-relevant ones. In chess, expert chess players encode larger chunks of positions and relations on the board and require fewer exposures to fully recreate a chess board. This is not due to their possessing superior visual skill, but rather to their advanced extraction of structural patterns specific to chess.[18][19] Extensive practice reading in English leads to extraction and rapid processing of the structural regularities of English spelling patterns. The word superiority effect demonstrates this—people are often much faster at recognizing words than individual letters.[20][21] In speech phonemes, observers who listen to a continuum of equally spaced consonant-vowel syllables going from /be/ to /de/ are much quicker to indicate that two syllables are different when they belonged to different phonemic categories than when they were two variants of the same phoneme, even when physical differences were equated between each pair of syllables.[22] Other examples of perceptual learning in the natural world include the ability to distinguish between relative pitches in music,[23] identify tumors in x-rays,[24] sort day-old chicks by gender,[25] taste the subtle differences between beers or wines,[26] identify faces as belonging to different races,[27] detect the features that distinguish familiar faces,[28] discriminate between two bird species ("great blue crown heron" and "chipping sparrow"),[29] and attend selectively to the hue, saturation and brightness values that comprise a color definition.[30] Brief history[edit source | editbeta] The fact that with huge amounts of practice, individuals can reach impressive perceptual expertise, whether in wine tasting, fabric evaluation or musical preference, has been well acknowledged for centuries, along with the prevalent idiom that "practice makes perfect". The first documented report, dating to the mid-19th century, is the earliest example of tactile training aimed at decreasing the minimal distance at which individuals can discriminate whether one or two points on their skin have been touched. It was found that this distance (JND, Just Noticeable Difference) decreases dramatically with practice, and that this improvement is at least partially retained on subsequent days. Moreover, this improvement is at least partially specific to the trained skin area. A particularly dramatic improvement was found for skin positions at which initial discrimination was very crude (e.g. on the back), though training could not bring the JND of initially crude areas down to that of initially accurate ones (e.g. finger tips).[31] William James devoted a section in his Principles of Psychology (1890/1950) to "the improvement in discrimination by practice.[32] He noted examples and emphasized the importance of perceptual learning for expertise. In 1918, Clark L. Hull, a noted learning theorist, trained human participants to learn to categorize deformed Chinese characters into categories. For each category, he used 6 instances that shared some invariant structural property. People learned to associate a sound as the name of each category,
  • 7. and more importantly, they were able to classify novel characters accurately.[33] This ability to extract invariances from instances and apply them to classify new instances marked this study as a perceptual learning experiment. It was not until 1969, however, that Eleanor Gibson published her seminal book The Principles of Perceptual learning and Development and defined the modern field of perceptual learning. She established the study of perceptual learning as an inquiry into the behavior and mechanism of perceptual change. By the mid-1970s, however, this area was in a state of dormancy due to a shift in focus to perceptual and cognitive development in infancy. Much of the scientific community tended to underestimate the impact of learning compared with innate mechanisms. Thus, most of this research focused on characterizing basic perceptual capacities of young infants rather than on perceptual learning processes. Since the mid-1980s, there has been a new wave of interest in perceptual learning due to findings of cortical plasticity at the lowest sensory levels of sensory systems. Our increased understanding of the physiology and anatomy of our cortical systems has been used to connect the behavioral improvement to the underlying cortical areas. This trend began with earlier findings of Hubel and Wiesel that perceptual representations at sensory areas of the cortex are substantially modified during a short ("critical") period immediately following birth. Merzenich, Kaas and colleagues showed that though neuroplasticity is diminished, it is not eliminated when the critical period ends.[34] Thus, when the external pattern of stimulation is substantially modified, neuronal representations in lower-level (e.g. primary) sensory areas are also modified. Research in this period centered on basic sensory discriminations, where remarkable improvements were found on almost any sensory task through discrimination practice. Following training, subjects were tested with novel conditions and learning transfer was assessed. This work departed from earlier work on perceptual learning, which spanned different tasks and levels. A question still debated today is to what extent improvements from perceptual learning stems from peripheral modifications compared with improvement in higher-level readout stages. Early interpretations, such as that suggested by William James, attributed it to higher-level categorization mechanisms whereby initially blurred differences are gradually associated with distinctively different labels. The work focused on basic sensory discrimination, however, suggests that the effects of perceptual learning are specific to changes in low-levels of the sensory nervous system (i.e., primary sensory cortices).[35] More recently, research suggest that perceptual learning processes are multilevel and flexible.[36] This cycles back to the earlier Gibsonian view that low-level learning effects are modulated by high-level factors, and suggests that improvement in information extraction may not involve only low-level sensory coding but also apprehension of relatively abstract structure and relations in time and space. Within the past decade, researchers have sought a more unified understanding of perceptual learning and worked to apply these principles to improve perceptual learning in applied domains.
  • 8. Characteristics[edit source | editbeta] Discovery and fluency effects[edit source | editbeta] Perceptual learning effects can be organized into two broad categories: discovery effects and fluency effects.[1] Discovery effects involve some change in the bases of response such as in selecting new information relevant for the task, amplifying relevant information or suppressing irrelevant information. Experts extract larger "chunks" of information and discover high-order relations and structures in their domains of expertise that are invisible to novices. Fluency effects involve changes in the ease of extraction. Not only can experts process high-order information, they do so with great speed and low attentional load. Discovery and fluency effects work together so that as the discovery structures becomes more automatic, attentional resources are conserved for discovery of new relations and for high-level thinking and problem-solving. The role of attention[edit source | editbeta] William James (Principles of Psychology, 1890) asserted that "My experience is what I agree to attend to. Only those items which I notice shape my mind - without selective interest, experience is an utter chaos.".[32] His view was extreme, yet its gist was largely supported by subsequent behavioral and physiological studies. Mere exposure does not seem to suffice for acquiring expertise. Indeed, a relevant signal in a given behavioral condition may be considered noise in another. For example, when presented with two similar stimuli, one might endeavor to study the differences between their representations in order to improve one's ability to discriminate between them, or one may instead concentrate on the similarities to improve one's ability to identify both as belonging to the same category. A specific difference between them could be considered 'signal' in the first case and 'noise' in the second case. Thus, as we adapt to tasks and environments, we pay increasingly more attention to the perceptual features that are relevant and important for the task at hand, and at the same time, less attention to the irrelevant features. This mechanism is called attentional weighting.[36] However, recent studies suggest that perceptual learning occurs without selective attention.[37] Studies of such task-irrelevant perceptual learning (TIPL) show that the degree of TIPL is similar to that found through direct training procedures.[38] TIPL for a stimulus depends on the relationship between that stimulus and important task events[39] or upon stimulus reward contingencies.[40] It has thus been suggested that learning (of task irrelevant stimuli) is contingent upon spatially diffusive learning signals.[41] Similar effects, but upon a shorter time scale, have been found for memory processes and in some cases is called attentional boosting.[42] Thus, when an important (alerting) event occurs, learning may also affect concurrent, non-attended and non-salient stimuli.[43]
  • 9. Time course of perceptual learning[edit source | editbeta] The time course of perceptual learning varies from one participant to another.[12] Perceptual learning occurs not only within the first training session but also between sessions.[44] Fast learning (i.e., within-first-session learning) and slow learning (i.e., between-session learning) involves different changes in the human adult brain. While the fast learning effects can only be retained for a short term of several days, the slow learning effects can be preserved for a long term over several months.[45] Explanations and Models[edit source | editbeta] Receptive field modification[edit source | editbeta] Research on basic sensory discriminations often show that perceptual learning effects are specific to the trained task or stimulus [46] Many researchers take this to suggest that perceptual learning may work by modifying the receptive fields of the cells (e.g., V1 and V2 cells) that initially encode the stimulus. For example, individual cells could adapt to become more sensitive to important features, effectively recruiting more cells for a particular purpose, making some cells more specifically tuned for the task at hand.[47] Evidence for receptive field change has been found using single-cell recording techniques in primates in both tactile and auditory domains.[48] However, not all perceptual learning tasks are specific to the trained stimuli or tasks. Sireteanu and Rettenback [49] discussed discrimination learning effects that generalize across eyes, retinal locations and tasks. Ahissar and Hochstein [50] used visual search to show that learning to detect a single line element hidden in an array of differently-oriented line segments could generalize to positions at which the target was never presented. In human vision, not enough receptive field modification has been found in early visual areas to explain perceptual learning.[51] Training that produces large behavioral changes such as improvements in discrimination does not produce changes in receptive fields. In studies where changes have been found, the changes are too small to explain changes in behavior.[52] Reverse hierarchy theory[edit source | editbeta] The Reverse Hierarchy Theory (RHT), proposed by Ahissar & Hochstein, aims to link between learning dynamics and specificity and the underlying neuronal sites.[53] RHT proposes that naïve performance is based on responses at high-level cortical areas, where crude, categorical level representations of the environment are represented. Hence initial learning stages involve understanding global aspects of the task. Subsequent practice may yield better perceptual resolution as a consequence of accessing lower-level information via the feedback connections going from high to low levels. Accessing the relevant low-level representations requires a backward search during which informative input populations of neurons in the low level are allocated. Hence, subsequent learning and its specificity reflect the resolution of lower levels. RHT thus proposes that initial performance is limited by the high-level resolution whereas post-training performance is limited by the
  • 10. resolution at low levels. Since high-level representations of different individuals differ due to their prior experience, their initial learning patterns may differ. Several imaging studies are in line with this interpretation, finding that initial performance is correlated with average (BOLD) responses at higher-level areas whereas subsequent performance is more correlated with activity at lower-level areas[citation needed] . RHT proposes that modifications at low levels will occur only when the backward search (from high to low levels of processing) is successful. Such success requires that the backward search will "know" which neurons in the lower level are informative. This "knowledge" is gained by training repeatedly on a limited set of stimuli, such that the same lower-level neuronal populations are informative during several trials. Recent studies found that mixing a broad range of stimuli may also yield effective learning if these stimuli are clearly perceived as different, or are explicitly tagged as different. These findings further support the requirement for top-down guidance in order to obtain effective learning. Enrichment versus Differentiation[edit source | editbeta] In some complex perceptual tasks, all humans are experts. We are all experts at scene identification, face identification and speech perception. Traditional explanations attribute this expertise to some holistic, somewhat specialized, mechanisms. Perhaps such quick identifications are achieved by more specific and complex perceptual detectors which gradually "chunk" (i.e., unitize) features that tend to concur, making it easier to pull a whole set of information. Whether any concurrence of features can gradually be chunked with practice or chunking can only be obtained with some pre-disposition (e.g. faces, phonological categories) is an open question. Current findings suggest that such expertise is correlated with a significant increase in the cortical volume involved in these processes. Thus, we all have somewhat specialized face areas, which may reveal an innate property, but we also develop somewhat specialized areas for written words as opposed to single letters or strings of letter-like symbols. Moreover, special experts in a given domain have larger cortical areas involved in that domain. Thus, expert musicians have larger auditory areas.[54] These observations are in line with traditional theories of enrichment proposing that improved performance involves an increase in cortical representation. For this expertise, basic categorical identification may be based on enriched and detailed representations, located to some extent in specialized brain areas. Physiological evidence suggests that training for refined discrimination along basic dimensions (e.g. frequency in the auditory modality) also increases the representation of the trained parameters, though in these cases the increase may mainly involve lower-level sensory areas.[55] Selective reweighting[edit source | editbeta] In 2005, Petrov, Dosher and Lu pointed out that perceptual learning may be explained in terms of the selection of which analyzers best perform the classification, even in simple discrimination tasks. They explain that the some part of the neural system responsible for particular decisions have specificity, while low-level perceptual units do not.[36] In their model, encodings at the lowest level do not change. Rather, changes that occur in
  • 11. perceptual learning arise from changes in higher-level, abstract representations of the relevant stimuli. Because specificity can come from differentially selecting information, this "selective reweighting theory" allows for learning of complex, abstract representation. This corresponds to Gibson’s earlier account of perceptual learning as selection and learning of distinguishing features. Selection may be the unifying principles of perceptual learning at all levels.[56] The impact of training protocol and the dynamics of learning[edit source | editbeta] Ivan Pavlov discovered conditioning. He found that when a stimulus (e.g. sound) is immediately followed by food several times, the mere presentation of this stimulus would subsequently elicit saliva in a dog's mouth. He further found that when he used a differential protocol, by consistently presenting food after one stimulus while not presenting food after another stimulus, dogs were quickly conditioned to selectively salivate in response to the rewarded one. He then asked whether this protocol could be used to increase perceptual discrimination, by differentially rewarding two very similar stimuli (e.g. tones with similar frequency). However, he found that differential conditioning was not effective. Pavlov's studies were followed by many training studies which found that an effective way to increase perceptual resolution is to begin with a large difference along the required dimension and gradually proceed to small differences along this dimension. This easy-to-difficult transfer was termed "transfer along a continuum". These studies showed that the dynamics of learning depend on the training protocol, rather than on the total amount of practice. Moreover, it seems that the strategy implicitly chosen for learning is highly sensitive to the choice of the first few trials during which the system tries to identify the relevant cues. Consolidation and sleep[edit source | editbeta] Several studies asked whether learning takes place during practice sessions or in between, for example, during subsequent sleep. The dynamics of learning are hard to evaluate since the directly measured parameter is performance, which is affected by both learning, inducing improvement, and fatigue, which hampers performance. Current studies suggest that sleep contributes to improved and durable learning effects, by further strengthening connections in the absence of continued practice.[44][57][58] Both slow-wave and REM (rapid eye movement) stages of sleep may contribute to this process, via not-yet-understood mechanisms. Comparison and contrast[edit source | editbeta] Practice with comparison and contrast of instances that belong to the same or different categories allow for the pick-up of the distinguishing features—features that are important for the classification task—and the filter of the irrelevant features [59]
  • 12. Task difficulty[edit source | editbeta] Learning easy examples first may lead to better transfer and better learning of more difficult cases.[60] By recording ERPs from human adults, Ding and Colleagues investigated the influence of task difficulty on the brain mechanisms of visual perceptual learning. Results showed that difficult task training affected earlier visual processing stage and broader visual cortical regions than easy task training.[61] Active classification and attention[edit source | editbeta] Active classification effort and attention are often necessary to produce perceptual learning effects.[58] However, in some cases, mere exposure to certain stimulus variations can produce improved discriminations. Feedback[edit source | editbeta] In many cases, perceptual learning does not require feedback (whether or not the classification is correct).[55] Other studies suggest that block feedback (feedback only after a block of trials) produces more learning effects than no feedback at all.[62] Limits[edit source | editbeta] Despite the marked perceptual learning demonstrated in different sensory systems and under varied training paradigms, it is clear that perceptual learning must face certain unsurpassable limits imposed by the physical characteristics of the sensory system. For instance, in tactile spatial acuity tasks, experiments suggest that the extent of learning is limited by fingertip surface area, which may constrain the underlying density of mechanoreceptors.[12] Relations to other forms of learning[edit source | editbeta] Declarative & procedural learning[edit source | editbeta] In many domains of expertise in the real world, perceptual learning interacts with other forms of learning. Declarative knowledge tends to occur with perceptual learning. As we learn to distinguish between an array of wine flavors, we also develop a wide range of vocabularies to describe the intricacy of each flavor. Similarly, perceptual learning also interacts flexibly with procedural knowledge. For example, the perceptual expertise of a baseball player at bat can detect early in the ball's flight whether the pitcher threw a curveball. However, the perceptual differentiation of the feel of swinging the bat in various ways may also have been involved in learning the motor commands that produce the required swing.[1] Implicit learning[edit source | editbeta] Perceptual learning is often said to be implicit, such that learning occurs without awareness. It is not at all clear whether perceptual learning is always implicit. Changes in sensitivity that arise are often not conscious and do not involve conscious procedures, but perceptual information can be mapped onto various responses.[1]
  • 13. In complex perceptual learning tasks (e.g., sorting of newborn chicks by gender, playing chess), experts are often unable to explain what stimulus relationships they are using in classification. However, in less complex perceptual learning tasks, people can point out what information they’re using to make classifications. Applications[edit source | editbeta] Improving perceptual skills[edit source | editbeta] An important potential application of perceptual learning is the acquisition of skill for practical purposes. Thus it is important to understand whether training for increased resolution in lab conditions induces a general upgrade which transfers to other environmental contexts, or results from mechanisms which are context specific. Improving complex skills is typically gained by training under complex simulation conditions rather than one component at a time. Recent lab-based training protocols with complex action computer games have shown that such practice indeed modifies visual skills in a general way, which transfers to new visual contexts. In 2010, Achtman, Green, and Bavelier reviewed the research on video games to train visual skills.[53] They cite a previous review by Green & Bavelier (2006) [63] on using video games to enhance perceptual and cognitive abilities. A variety of skills were upgraded in video game players, including "improved hand-eye coordination,[64] increased processing in the periphery,[65] enhanced mental rotation skills,[66] greater divided attention abilities,[67] and faster reaction times,[68] to name a few". An important characteristic is the functional increase in the size of the effective visual field (within which viewers can identify objects), which is trained in action games and transfers to new settings. Whether learning of simple discriminations, which are trained in separation, transfers to new stimulus contexts (e.g. complex stimulus conditions) is still an open question. Like experimental procedures, other attempts to apply perceptual learning methods to basic and complex skills use training situations in which the learner receives many short classification trials. Tallal, Merzenich and their colleagues have successfully adapted auditory discrimination paradigms to address speech and language difficulties.[69][70] They reported improvements in language learning-impaired children using specially enhanced and extended speech signals. The results applied not only to auditory discrimination performance but speech and language comprehension as well. Technologies for perceptual learning[edit source | editbeta] In educational domains, recent efforts by Philip Kellman and colleagues showed that perceptual learning can be systematically produced and accelerated using specific, computer-based technology. Their approach to perceptual learning methods take the form of perceptual learning modules (PLMs): sets of short, interactive trials that develop, in a particular domain, learners’ pattern recognition, classification abilities, and their abilities to map across multiple representations. As a result of practice with mapping across transformations (e.g., algebra, fractions) and across multiple representations (e.g., graphs, equations, and word problems), students show dramatic gains in their structure recognition in fraction learning and algebra. They also demonstrated that
  • 14. when students practice classifying algebraic transformations using PLMs, the results show remarkable improvements in fluency at algebra problem solving.[71][72][73] These results suggests that perceptual learning can offer a needed complement to conceptual and procedural instructions in the classroom. Similar results have also been replicated in other domains with PLMs, including anatomic recognition in medical and surgical training,[74] reading instrumental flight displays,[75] and apprehending molecular structures in chemistry.[76] Perceptual learning in humans occurs when a person is repeatedly exposed to specific stimuli (information). Perceptual learning involves long lasting and amazing changes to the human perceptual system that incredibly improve one’s ability to respond to the environment. The mechanisms of perceptual learning include attention weighting, imprinting, differentiation, and unitization. With attention weighting, perception becomes adapted to tasks by increasing the attention paid to important dimensions and features. With imprinting, special receptors are developed that are specialized for specific stimuli. With differentiation, stimuli that were once indistinguishable become psychologically separated. With unitization, tasks that originally required detection of several components are accomplished by detecting a single construct. Because dramatic changes to human perceptual systems occur within the first six years of life (and the younger the more dramatic), your child’s training in perceptual learning in reading and maths must occur during this period in order to achieve effortless learning for success and happiness in school and life. Attention Weighting Perception becomes adapted to tasks by increasing the attention paid to important features and by decreasing attention to irrelevant features. Both Perceptual Maths™ and Perceptual Reading™ expose materials that are pertinent to learning in each discipline. Your child will perceptually learn to pay attention to those features that are important and ignore those that are not. Often children without perceptual learning in maths and reading will waste processing time on irrelevant features. And not devoting enough processing time to relevant features means problems become unnecessarily more difficult to solve. Stimulus Imprinting Through imprinting, receptors that are specialized for stimuli or parts of stimuli are developed. Imprinting implies that the receptor is shaped by the pertinent stimulus. Receptors develop for repeated stimuli, and these receptors increase the speed, accuracy, and fluency with which the stimuli are processed. In Perceptual Maths™ the stimuli are quantities (dots), numerals, operations and clocks. In Perceptual Reading™ the stimuli are phonemes, graphemes, words, phrases and sentences. Whole Stimulus Imprinting As more instances of maths and reading materials are stored, performance improves because more relevant instances can be retrieved, and the time required for retrieving them decreases. Your child’s performance in perceptual tasks is dependent on the amount of their experience with a particular stimulus. Performance is better on frequently presented items than rare items. Perceptual Maths™ and Perceptual Reading™ expose relevant materials that improve their performance in maths and reading. As your child gets older they will be better able to perceptually identify unclear or quickly presented maths and reading materials because they will have already been exposed to them during a period of perceptual plasticity. Feature Imprinting
  • 15. As well as imprinting on entire stimuli, the human perceptual system also imprints on parts of a stimulus. If a stimulus part is important, varies independently of other parts, or occurs frequently, people develop a specialized receptor for that part. This leads to the development of new building blocks for describing stimuli. Parts that are developed in one context can be used to efficiently describe subsequent objects. Efficient representations are promoted because the parts have been extracted for their prevalence. When new maths or reading problems present themselves, your child will be able to extract relevant features and relate them to their prior learning, which thus builds upon their learning. Topological Imprinting Topological imprinting occurs when the space and the positions of patterns within the space are learned as a result of training. Rather than simply developing independent receptors, topological imprinting implies that a spatially organized network of receptors is created. In Perceptual Maths™ the clock and single line equations expose the child to these spatial patterns. The spatial abilities will benefit your child’s future problem solving. In Perceptual Reading™ the spatial abilities will help comprehension by rapid pickup and processing of text. Differentiation With differentiation, stimuli that were once psychologically fused become separated. Once separated, distinction can be made between components that were originally indistinguishable. Differentiation is crucial in understanding maths and reading insofar as understanding the subtle yet important differences in text, numbers and their operations. Differentiation also occurs at the levels of whole stimuli and features within stimuli. Whole Stimuli Differentiation Perceptual adaptation involves developing increasingly differentiated object representations rather than its features. In Perceptual Reading™, this applies to whole words, phrases, sentences and beyond, rather than letters. In Perceptual Maths™, whole stimuli are whole equations and clocks rather than individual numbers and operators. Category Learning involves dividing a large, general category into subcategories. Experts in a field have several differentiated categories where a novice has only a single category. For example a computer expert would know the computer as the sum of CPU, memory, storage, BUS etc, while a novice would only see the computer as a box. By the end of the course, at age 3, your child will have been an expert in reading and maths; having had many categories with which to differentiate reading and maths problems. Dimensions Differentiation Just as experience can lead to the psychological separation of stimuli or categories, it can also lead to the separation of perceptual dimensions that comprise a single stimulus. Dimensions that are originally treated as fused often become separated with perceptual training. Your child will shift from perceiving stimuli in terms of the holistic to analytically decomposing them into separate dimensions. For example, unlike many young children, your child will be able to differentiate between the letters b, d, p and q at a subconscious level. The process of decomposition gives your perceptually trained child the ability to solve problems well beyond what has been taught. Unitization A task that originally required detection of several parts can be accomplished by detecting a single complex unit. Using the above example, a computer can be perceptually identified as a whole object by an expert while a novice needs to systematically list the components in order to identify the same computer. At last, as a culmination of Perceptual Maths™ and Perceptual Reading™, your child will have perceptually learnt whole words, phrases,
  • 16. sentences, equations and clocks as single units. Thus, retrieval and processing are more efficient and at more rapid speeds and fluency. Conclusion Perceptual learning in your child occurs when they are repeatedly exposed to specific reading and maths concepts. These long lasting and amazing changes to their perceptual system incredibly improve their ability to effectively and positively respond to life and school. At any given time, what can be learned depends on what has already been learned, perceptually and cognitively. Therefore your child, having learnt a great deal by the age of three, will have a powerful unfair advantage over the average population. And they deserve it. Your child will possess incredible superior learning skills that permit them to stay well ahead of their peers throughout life. They will achieve effortless lifelong learning for success and happiness in school and life.