SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  11
Télécharger pour lire hors ligne
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067
DOI 10.1007/s10802-010-9425-y




Behavioral and Socio-emotional Functioning in Children
with Selective Mutism: A Comparison with Anxious
and Typically Developing Children Across Multiple
Informants
Diana Carbone & Louis A. Schmidt &
Charles C. Cunningham & Angela E. McHolm &
Shannon Edison & Jeff St. Pierre & Michael H. Boyle



Published online: 23 May 2010
# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010


Abstract We examined differences among 158 children, 44          implications for clinical practice, whereby social skills
with selective mutism (SM; M=8.2 years, SD=3.4 years), 65        training merits inclusion in intervention for children with
with mixed anxiety (MA; M=8.9 years, SD=3.2 years), and          anxiety disorders as well as children with SM.
49 community controls (M=7.7 years, SD=2.6 years) on
primary caregiver, teacher, and child reports of behavioral      Keywords Selective mutism . Mixed anxiety . Children .
and socio-emotional functioning. Children with SM were           Social skills . Social anxiety . Parent and teacher reports .
rated lower than controls on a range of social skills, but the   Internalizing problems
SM and MA groups did not significantly differ on many of
the social skills and anxiety measures. However, children
with SM were rated higher than children with MA and              Selective mutism (SM) is a disorder marked by a consistent
controls on social anxiety. Findings suggest that SM may be      failure to speak in certain social situations (e.g., at school)
conceptualized as an anxiety disorder, with primary deficits     despite the presence of speech in other social situations
in social functioning and social anxiety. This interpretation    (e.g., at home; American Psychiatric Association 2000).
supports a more specific classification of SM as an anxiety      SM is largely considered rare with prevalence rates
disorder for future diagnostic manuals than is currently         estimated to be between 0.03% and 0.2% (Bergman et al.
described in the literature. The present findings also have      2002; Brown and Lloyd 1975; Elizur and Perednik 2003;
                                                                 Kolvin and Fundudis 1981; Kopp and Gillberg 1997;
                                                                 Kumpulainen et al. 1998), with higher rates identified in
D. Carbone : L. A. Schmidt
                                                                 immigrant populations (Elizur and Perednik 2003). Preva-
McMaster Integrative Neuroscience, Discovery, & Study
(MiNDS), McMaster University,                                    lence rates for SM are variable in the literature and appear
Hamilton, ON, Canada                                             to be influenced by the origin of research, diagnostic
                                                                 criteria, age of children, immigrant status, and setting in
L. A. Schmidt (*)
                                                                 which SM is sampled (e.g., clinic versus school setting)
Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour,
McMaster University,                                             (Bergman et al. 2002; Kumpulainen 2002; Sharp et al.
Hamilton, ON L8S 4K1, Canada                                     2007). There is little consensus as to the sex ratio of the
e-mail: schmidtl@mcmaster.ca                                     disorder, with clinically-referred samples reporting a slightly
C. C. Cunningham : A. E. McHolm : S. Edison : M. H. Boyle
                                                                 higher prevalence of SM in females than males (e.g.,
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioural Neurosciences,            Cunningham et al. 2004; Dummit et al. 1997; Kristensen
McMaster University,                                             2000). Other studies investigating community- and school-
Hamilton, ON, Canada                                             based samples suggest comparable occurrence between the
                                                                 sexes (e.g., Bergman et al. 2002; Elizur and Perednik 2003).
J. St. Pierre
Child and Parent Resource Institute (CPRI),                         Although SM has a typical onset before the age of five,
London, ON, Canada                                               the disorder often does not become evident until school
1058                                                                                J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067


entry, when expectations (and related pressures) to speak        cooperation, than controls. More recently, Cunningham et
increase (Cunningham et al. 2004; Garcia et al. 2004;            al. (2006) used the SSRS again and found that both
Giddan et al. 1997). Despite this early onset, children are      teachers and parents rated children with SM to be lower on
not commonly referred for clinical assessment until they are     verbal and nonverbal social skills than control children, but
between approximately 6.5 to 9 years of age (Ford et al.         the two groups were not different on measures of nonverbal
1998; Kumpulainen et al. 1998; Standart and Le Couteur           social cooperation.
2003). SM may persist for a few months to several years,            In addition to the work by Cunningham and colleagues
and adults diagnosed with SM as children often continue to       (2004, 2006), a study by Vecchio and Kearney (2005)
suffer with social anxiety and deficits in social communi-       found that children with SM and children with anxiety
cation, in addition to displaying other problems with socio-     disorders were rated by teachers and parents to have greater
emotional and daily adjustment (Remschmidt et al. 2001).         internalizing behaviors than controls. Vecchio and Kearney
Despite being diagnostically well-documented (Dummit et          also found that children with SM and children with anxiety
al. 1997; Kopp and Gillberg 1997; Sharp et al. 2007), the        disorders did not differ from children in a control group on
etiology of SM remains equivocal.                                externalizing behaviors. Furthermore, children with SM had
    Currently, SM is ambiguously classified under ‘Other         significantly greater total comorbid diagnoses and anxiety
Disorders of Infancy, Childhood and Adolescence’ in the          disorder diagnoses than children with anxiety disorders,
current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV-TR,            based on child, but not parent, report.
American Psychiatric Association 2000). This classification         There are, in addition, other studies, which have used
is largely disputed as the greater consensus in the literature   clinician, parent, and children’s self-report measures to
classifies SM as an anxiety disorder (Anstendig 1999;            compare children with SM to children with SP (Yeganeh
Sharp et al. 2007; Standart and Le Couteur 2003). The basis      et al. 2003, 2006). For example, Yeganeh et al. (2003,
for the designation of SM as an anxiety disorder comes           2006) found that children with SM did not report greater
from four main findings. First, strong comorbidity with and      levels of social anxiety than children with SP, despite
characteristic similarities between SM and anxiety disor-        clinician and observer ratings of greater or equal levels of
ders, specifically social phobia (SP), have been identified in   social anxiety in the SM and SP groups. Yeganeh and
the literature (e.g., Black and Uhde 1995; Dummit et al.         colleagues (2003, 2006) suggested that children with SM
1997; Sharp et al. 2007; Vecchio and Kearney 2005). There        do not report as large a degree of anxiety, since their
is even the suggestion that SM may be a subtype (Black &         inability to talk may serve as a compensatory strategy to
Uhde, 1992) or developmental precursor (Bergman et al.           reduce their anxiety.
2002) to SP in some cases. Second, there is evidence                Studies that have examined the socio-emotional charac-
showing a greater prevalence of anxiety disorders amongst        teristics of SM have been limited by several factors,
relatives of children with SM than among typically               including small sample sizes (e.g., Vecchio and Kearney
developing children, which further implicates genetic            2005; Yeganeh et al. 2003, 2006) and a lack of other
factors in the etiology of the disorder (Black and Uhde          clinical group comparisons (e.g., anxious children;
1995; Cohan et al. 2006b; Kristensen and Torgerson 2002).        Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006). Furthermore, few studies
Third, both SM (Ford et al. 1998) and anxiety disorders          have examined social skills in children with SM. Studies
(e.g. Hirshfeld-Becker et al. 2007) have been associated         that address these issues are needed, given the argument
with similar temperaments, namely, behavioral inhibition.        that 1) SM would be better classified as an anxiety disorder
Lastly, SM and anxiety disorders both share common               (Anstendig 1999; Sharp et al. 2007; Standart and Le
psychotherapeutic and pharmacological treatments (Cohan          Couteur 2003), 2) there are conceptual and behavioral
et al. 2006a; Standart and Le Couteur 2003).                     similarities between SM and SP, and 3) the reported
    Recent research has examined the socio-emotional char-       findings of children (e.g., Bernstein et al. 2008) and adults
acteristics of children with SM. For example, Cunningham         (e.g., Voncken et al. 2008) with SP suffer from social skills
and colleagues (2004; Cunningham et al. 2006) have               deficits.
suggested that children with SM exhibit lower social                A further limitation concerns the issue that most studies
competence compared to their typically developed peers.          examining SM have largely been informed by parent report
Using the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham and         despite the recommendation that a multi-method, multi-
Elliot 1990), Cunningham et al. (2004) found that children       informant approach be utilized in the assessment of anxiety
with SM were rated significantly lower than controls on          disorders (Schniering et al. 2000) and SM (Mclnnes and
the parent report of social assertion, social responsibility,    Manassis 2005). Given the personal nature of fears, self-
social cooperation, and social control subscales. Compar-        report is a recommended component of the multi-modal
atively, teachers considered children with SM to exhibit         battery (Schniering et al. 2000). Teacher ratings of a child’s
less social assertion, but not social control or social          socio-emotional behavior, in addition to parent and self-
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067                                                                              1059


report, are also imperative, since children spend substantial   mental health agencies in Southern Ontario and
amounts of time in the classroom during their formative         children from the McMaster Child Database. Children
years (Clarizio 1994). Moreover, there exist anxiety-           classified as controls were also obtained from the
provoking situations unique to the school setting about         McMaster Child Database only, and all were healthy
which only a teacher may be apt to report. Accordingly,         without mental health problems. The McMaster Child
teacher assessments are essential for children who typically    Database contains the names of children from the
do not speak in the school setting. There is, however, scant    community who were recruited at birth from the McMas-
research on SM that has included teacher reports.               ter University Medical Centre and St. Joseph’s Healthcare,
                                                                Hamilton, Ontario. Parents consented for their infant’s
                                                                inclusion in the McMaster Child Database if they were
The Present Study                                               interested in participating in future research studies.
                                                                Participant demographics can be found in Table 1.
In order to extend the recent work of Cunningham and
colleagues (2004, 2006) and clarify other previous findings,    Selective Mutism (SM) Group
we compared a relatively large sample of children with SM
to children with mixed anxiety (MA) disorders and               The SM group comprised 44 children (female n=23, male
typically developing children on teacher, primary caregiver,    n=21), with a mean age of 8.2 years (SD=3.4 years). The
and child self-report measures of social and emotional          children were included in the SM group if their primary
functioning. Based on the prior findings by Cunningham          caregivers or teachers indicated that the child did not speak
and his colleagues (e.g., Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006), we     in two or more situations on the Speech Situations
predicted that children with SM would exhibit significantly     Questionnaire—Parent Version (SSQ—Parent; or the
lower verbal and nonverbal social competence than               Speech Situations Questionnaire—Teacher version (SSQ-
typically developing children as rated by both primary          Teacher; Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006). Classification in
caregivers and teachers. Given that SM appears to be            the SM group also required that the absence of speaking
associated with internalizing rather than externalizing         was not due to a communication disorder, and that the
factors (e.g. Black and Uhde 1995; Dummit et al. 1997;          absence of speaking persisted for a minimum of one month.
Ford et al. 1998; Kristensen 2000; Steinhausen and Juzi         The same criteria used to classify children with SM in the
1996; Vecchio and Kearney 2005), we also anticipated that       present study have also been used elsewhere (Nowakowski
children with SM would be significantly higher on               et al. 2009).
internalizing behaviors and lower on externalizing, hyper-         Primary caregiver report on the internalizing section of
active and problem behaviors than typically developing          the Computerized Diagnostic Individual Schedule for Child
children. Since it may be the case that children with SM do     (C-DISC IV; Shaffer et al. 2000) was used to examine the
not report as great a degree of anxiety as indicated by other   number of internalizing disorders for which the participants
informants (Yeganeh et al. 2003, 2006), we expected that        met criteria. The C-DISC IV was not used to classify
self-report of social anxiety by children with SM would not     children with SM, since its reliability has been established
differ from children with MA and typically developing           for anxiety disorders but not for SM. There were seven
children, but primary caregivers of children with SM would      (15.9%) children classified as SM for which the C-DISC IV
indicate higher levels of social anxiety than children with     could not be obtained because primary caregivers could not
MA and typically developing children. We further hypoth-        be reached via telephone to conduct the questionnaire.
esized that social skills and anxiety would not be different    Twenty-eight of the 37 (63.6%) children classified as SM
between the SM and MA groups, if SM is conceptualized           were diagnosed with one or more anxiety disorders as per
as an anxiety disorder.                                         the C-DISC IV. The most common anxiety disorder
                                                                diagnosis was specific phobia (29.5%), followed by social
                                                                phobia (18.2%), agoraphobia (15.9%), separation anxiety
Method                                                          (13.6%), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD; 2.3%),
                                                                obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; 2.3%), and post-
Participants                                                    traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; 2.3%).

Participants were 158 (81 males, 77 females; M=8.4 years,       Mixed Anxiety (MA) Group
SD=3.1 years) children. Children with SM consisted of
referrals from children’s mental health agencies in             The MA group included 65 children (female n=29, male
Southern Ontario. Children classified as mixed anxiety          n=36), with a mean age of 8.9 years (SD=3.2 years). The
(MA) were a combination of referrals from children’s            children were classified in the MA group if they had one or
1060                                                                                             J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067

Table 1 Demographic Information

                                            Selective mutism          Mixed anxiety      Community control      Statistic           p-value
                                            (n=44)                    (n=65)             (n=49)

Percent female                              52.3                      44.6               51.0                   X2(2) = 0.7         0.682
Mean (SD) age of child in years             8.2 (3.4)                 8.9 (3.2)          7.7 (2.6)              F(2,155) = 2.2      0.166
Percent in regular classroom                97.7                      95.3a              100a                   X2(4) = 2.9         0.577
Age range of primary caregiver              <19 (n=1)                 19–39 (n=25)       19–39a (n=21)          X2(4) = 2.9         0.570
                                            19–39 (n=17)              40–64 (n=40)       40–64 (n=27)
                                            40–64 (n=26)
Approximate total income of                                                                                     X2(12) = 35.5       0.0005
 household, in prior year,
 before taxes
 Less than $15 000                          1a                        2a                 0a
 $ 15 000–$30 000                           3                         10                 1
 $ 30 000–$45 000                           6                         5                  0
 $ 45 000–$60 000                           0                         11                 2
 $ 60 000–$75 000                           12                        5                  8
 $ 75 000–$90 000                           11                        14                 21
 Greater than $100 000                      10                        17                 13
a
    indicates group with missing data for that demographic variable



more anxiety disorder diagnoses on the C-DISC IV (social                      comprehend the questions. A questionnaire package was
phobia, separation anxiety, specific phobia, panic disorder,                  also sent out to the child’s teacher in which he/she reported
GAD, PTSD, OCD, or agoraphobia), and no diagnosis of                          on the child’s social skills and problem behaviors.
SM as per teacher and primary caregiver report on the                            The measures collected in the present study were part of
Speech Situations Questionnaires. Of the 65 children in the                   a larger study examining the etiology, academic abilities,
MA group, 40 (61.5%) had one anxiety disorder, and 25                         familial characteristics, and behavioral and psychophysio-
(38.5%) had two or more anxiety disorders as per the C-                       logical correlates of children with SM. Data collection took
DISC IV. The most common anxiety disorder diagnosis                           place at the Child Emotion Laboratory at McMaster
among the MA group was specific phobia (67.7%),                               University. All procedures were approved by the McMaster
followed by social phobia (27.7%), separation anxiety                         University Health Sciences Research Ethics Board. The
(23.1%), GAD (16.9%), OCD (10.8%), panic disorder                             child received a toy or gift certificate as a token of our
(6.2%), and PTSD (1.5%).                                                      appreciation at the end of the visit.

Community Control Group                                                       Primary Caregiver Report Measures

The typically developing group comprised 49 children                          Diagnostic Questionnaires The Speech Situations Ques-
(female n=25, male n=24), with a mean age of 7.7 years                        tionnaire—Parent Version (SSQ-Parent; Cunningham et al.
(SD=2.6 years). Children were classified as typically                         2004, 2006) assesses a child’s speech patterns in a number
developing if they had no diagnoses as per the CDISC IV,                      of situations and to several different people as reported by
and if they did not meet criteria for SM based on primary                     the primary caregiver. High internal consistencies of 0.82
caregiver and teacher report on the Speech Situations                         (Cunningham et al. 2006) and 0.92 (Nowakowski et al.
Questionnaires.                                                               2009) have been found in prior studies. An internal
                                                                              consistency of 0.92 was found in the present study.
Procedure                                                                        Additionally, primary caregiver report on the internaliz-
                                                                              ing section of the computer assisted Diagnostic Interview
Following arrival at the Child Emotion Laboratory, the                        Schedule for Children (C-DISC IV; Shaffer et al. 2000) was
parent and child were briefed about the procedures and                        obtained by a trained research assistant via telephone
written consent was obtained. The primary caregiver then                      following the initial laboratory visit. The C-DISC IV is a
filled out a number of questionnaires. Child self-report of                   structured diagnostic interview that evaluates the presence
anxiety was also obtained if the child was old enough to                      of 34 psychiatric disorders in children based on the DSM-
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067                                                                                  1061


IV (Shaffer et al. 2000). Panic disorder, GAD, social            Teacher Report Measures
phobia, specific phobia, separation anxiety, OCD, PTSD,
agoraphobia, and major depression are assessed on the            Teacher Diagnostic Questionnaires The Speech Situations
internalizing section of the C-DISC IV. Only the internal-       Questionnaire—Teacher Version (SSQ-Teacher; Cunningham
izing section of the C-DISC IV was administered due to           et al. 2004, 2006) assesses a child’s speech patterns in a
time contraints.                                                 number of situations at school and to several different people
                                                                 as reported by the teacher. A high internal consistency of 0.95
Social Competence and Problem Behavior Measures                  was reported recently (Nowakowski et al. 2009) and was 0.95
Primary caregivers completed the Social Skills Rating            in the present study.
System (SSRS)—Parent Version (Gresham and Elliot
1990), which is comprised of nine different scales. There        Social Competence and Problem Behavior Measures
are four scales that examine social skills: social assertion,    Teachers also completed a version of the SSRS that yields
social cooperation, social responsibility, and self-control;     identical scales to that of the parent version with the
these four sum to a total social skills scale.                   exception of a social responsibility scale.
   The SSRS has three scales that pertain to externaliz-
ing, internalizing, and hyperactive (only grades K-6)            Verbal and Nonverbal Social Interactions We also com-
behaviors subscales, which sum to a total problem                puted subscales developed by Cunningham et al. (2006) to
behaviors scale. Internal consistency among the scales           assess verbal social interactions, nonverbal social interactions
of the SSRS—Parent Version has been reported between             and nonverbal classroom cooperation and competence.
0.87 and 0.90 (as cited in Cunningham et al. 2004). The          Internal consistency among the scales of SSRS—Teacher
internal consistencies for grades K-6 in the present study       Version has been reported between 0.93 and 0.94 (as cited in
were between 0.62 and 0.88, and for grades 7–12 were             Cunningham et al. 2004). The internal consistencies for
between 0.74 and 0.91.                                           grades K-6 in the present study were between 0.87 and 0.96,
                                                                 and for grades 7–12 were between 0.72 and 0.93. Cunning-
Verbal and Nonverbal Social Skills We also computed three        ham et al. (2006) reported internal consistencies of 0.90,
scales developed by Cunningham et al. (2006) to assess           0.94 and 0.92 for the verbal social interaction subscale,
verbal social skills, nonverbal social skills, and nonverbal     nonverbal social interaction subscale, and nonverbal class-
cooperation subscales. Cunningham et al. (2006) reported         room cooperation and competence subscale, respectively. In
internal consistencies of 0.78, 0.71, and 0.78 for the verbal    the present study, the internal consistencies were: 0.87 for
social skills, nonverbal social skills, and nonverbal cooper-    grades K-6 and 0.80 for grades 7–12 verbal social
ation subscales, respectively. For the present study, internal   interactions scales, 0.86 for grades K-6 and 0.90 for grades
consistencies were 0.70 for grades K-6 and 0.58 for grades       7–12 nonverbal social interaction scales, and 0.92 for grades
7–12 verbal social skills scales, and 0.83 for grades K-6,       K-6 and 0.88 for grades 7–12 nonverbal classroom compe-
and 0.76 for grades 7–12 nonverbal social skills scales, and     tence scales.
were 0.81 for grades K-6, and 0.85 for grades 7–12 for the
nonverbal social competence scales.                              Child Report Measures

Screen for Child Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED)            Although reliability for the SCARED has been tested for
The SCARED (Birmaher et al. 1997) yields five scales             children between the ages of 9 to 18 (Birmaher et al. 1997),
related to five different anxiety disorders: panic disorder/     we administered the SCARED—Child Version if the child
significant somatic symptoms, GAD, social anxiety disor-         was able to comprehend the questions. The mean age for
der, separation anxiety disorder, and significant school         children who completed the SCARED—Child Version was
avoidance. We also computed a measure of total anxiety,          9.4 (SD=2.9) years. The SCARED—Child Version has
which was a sum of all of the questions on the SCARED—           identical questions and yields identical scales as the
Parent Version. The parent version of the SCARED has an          SCARED—Parent Version. The child version of the
internal consistency of 0.74 (Birmaher et al. 1997). The         SCARED has an internal consistency of 0.93 (Birmaher et
internal consistencies for subscales of the SCARED—              al. 1997). We also computed a total score for the SCARED—
Parent Version in the present study were between 0.63            Child Version, which was the total sum of all of the questions
and 0.75, and for the total score was 0.92 for children          on the SCARED—Child Version. The internal consistencies
below age eight. Internal consistencies for subscales of the     for subscales of the SCARED—Child Version in the present
SCARED—Parent Version in the present study were                  study were between 0.56 and 0.82, and for the total score was
between 0.76 and 0.93 and for the total score was 0.95           0.86 for children below age eight. Internal consistencies of the
for children eight and above.                                    SCARED—Child Version in the present study were between
1062                                                                                   J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067


0.63 and 0.83, and for the total score, it was 0.92 for children   using income as a covariate. All post hoc analyses were
eight and above.                                                   Bonferroni corrected.
                                                                       The total social skills scale for the SSRS-Parent and
Missing Data                                                       Teacher Versions and the total anxiety scales for the SCARED
                                                                   Parent and Child Versions were not included in the MAN-
Data were missing if specific questions were not completed,        COVA analyses as they represent the sum of the subscales in
questions were deemed non-applicable to the child by the           each of the questionnaires. The total scales from these scales
primary caregiver or teacher, certain questions were missed,       were analyzed using separate one-way ANCOVAs, with
entire questionnaires were not returned to the laboratory, or      group (SM, MA, Control) as the between subject-factor and
the child was too young to understand the question(s) in the       income as a covariate. All post hoc analyses were Bonferroni
case of the SCARED-Child Version. Missing questions for            corrected. The total problem behaviors scale was not
the SSRS-Parent and Teacher Version were filled out in             analyzed, as it is the sum of three different constructs
accordance with the protocol outlined in Gresham and               (internalizing, externalizing and hyperactive behaviors) that
Elliot (1990). Missing questions for the SCARED-Parent             should be analyzed and reported separately.
and Child Versions were filled in by computing the average             To assess whether primary caregiver and child report of
of the questions pertaining to a specific scale that the           anxiety differed within the SM and MA groups, paired-samples
missing question was relevant to and then recalculating the        t-tests were conducted using the SCARED—Parent and Child
scale. Importantly, the groups did not significantly differ on     Versions total anxiety score and social anxiety score.
the amount of missing data. There were a total of seven
participants missing the SSRS—Parent Version, 25 partic-
ipants missing the SSRS—Teacher Version, eight partic-             Results
ipants missing the SCARED—Child Version, and five
participants missing the SCARED—Parent Version. Of the             SSRS-Parent Version: Social Competence
remaining participants, there were 1.1% data missing from
the SSRS—Parent Version, 0.6% data missing from the                Consistent with our predictions, the MANCOVA for the
SSRS—Teacher Version, 0.09% data missing from the                  social skills subscales indicated a significant multivariate
SCARED—Parent Version, and 0.06% data missing from                 main effect of group (Wilks’Λ, F(8,256) = 6.886, p=0.0005)
the SCARED—Child Version.                                          that further indicated a significant main effect of group on
                                                                   the social assertion, social responsibility, and self-control,
Data Analyses                                                      but not the social cooperation subscales (see Table 2). A
                                                                   one-way ANCOVA also indicated that there was a
Income in the prior year before taxes differed significantly       significant main effect of group on the total social skills scale
among the three groups (X2(12) = 35.5, p<0.05) and was             (F(2,131) = 12.3, p=0.0005; see Table 2).
significantly correlated with all of the measures in the study        Follow-up tests confirmed that the SM and MA groups
except for the SSRS—Teacher Version social assertion and           had significantly lower social assertion (p<0.006) and total
verbal social skills scales, the SCARED—Child Version,             social skills (all p’s<0.02) than the control group. Primary
and the SCARED—Parent Version social anxiety and                   caregivers also rated the SM group lower in social
school avoidance scales. Accordingly, income was consid-           responsibility skills than the MA and control groups (all
ered as a covariate in all analyses.                               p’s<0.006). There were no significant group differences on
   To examine differences among the three groups on the            the self-control scale following Bonferroni correction.
various behavioral and socio-emotional functioning mea-
sures completed by the primary caregiver, teacher, and             SSRS-Parent Version: Verbal and Nonverbal Social Skills
children, separate one-way multivariate analyses of covari-
ance (MANCOVA) tests were conducted, with group (SM,               The MANCOVA for the verbal social skills scale, nonver-
MA, Control) as the between-subject factor and the                 bal social skills scale, and nonverbal cooperation scale
subscales (e.g., SSRS-Parent subscales: social cooperation,        revealed a main effect of group (Wilks’Λ, F(6, 260) =
social assertion, social responsibility, self-control) as the      11.679, p=0.0005) for all of the subscales (see Table 2).
dependent variables. A similar MANCOVA was performed               The SM group had significantly lower verbal social skills
with group (SM, MA, Control) as the between-subject                than the MA and control groups (p’s<0.006; see Table 2
factor and the subscales (internalizing, externalizing, and        and Fig. 2). Furthermore, the SM and MA groups had
hyperactive behaviors) as the dependent measures. Signif-          significantly lower nonverbal social skills than the control
icant main effects were followed up with analyses of               group (p’s<0.006). There were no other significant group
covariance (ANCOVA) tests on the dependent variables               differences following Bonferroni correction.
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067                                                                                                       1063

Table 2 Group Differences on Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) and Verbal and Nonverbal Social Behavior Measures as Completed by
Primary Caregivers

Measure                                 Selective mutism              Mixed anxiety                  Controls                  f-ratio       p

                                        Mean            SD            Mean           SD              Mean             SD

Social competence measures
 Social cooperation                     11.4            (3.6)         10.6            (3.8)          12.4              (3.0)     1.8         0.175
 Social assertion                       11.0b           (3.5)         12.5a           (3.9)          16.1a,b           (3.3)    17.8         0.0005
 Social responsibility                   9.9c,d         (3.7)         11.8c           (2.9)          13.6d             (2.9)    13.5         0.0005
 Self-control                           11.9            (2.8)         10.9            (3.9)          13.5              (3.8)     3.5         0.034
 Total social skills                    44.2e           (9.1)         45.9f          (10.0)          48.3e,f          (11.0)    12.3         0.0005
Problem behavior measures
 Externalizing behaviors                 3.7            (2.1)          4.8             (2.9)           3.4             (2.4)     2.0         0.139
 Internalizing behaviors                 5.6g           (2.2)          5.9h            (3.1)           2.9g,h          (2.4)    11.2         0.0005
 Hyperactive behaviors                   3.6            (2.5)          5.3             (2.8)           4.1             (2.7)     3.0         0.054
Verbal and nonverbal behavior measures
 Verbal social skills                10.8i,j            (3.2)         13.8i            (3.8)         16.1j             (4.0)    22.8         0.0005
 Nonverbal social skills             15.4k              (4.2)         15.7l            (4.1)         20.2k,l           (4.8)    12.5         0.0005
 Nonverbal social cooperation        15.8               (3.5)         13.6             (4.0)         16.3              (4.5)     3.7         0.028

Identical superscripts indicate statistical significance between those groups

SSRS-Parent Version: Problem Behaviors                                        SM group had lower verbal social skills than the MA and
                                                                              control groups (p<0.006; see Table 3 and Fig. 2). The SM
The MANCOVA indicated a significant main effect for                           group was also lower in nonverbal social skills than the
group on the problem behavior subscales (Wilks’Λ, F(6,                        control group (p<0.006), and the MA group had lower
202) = 5.088, p=0.002), but only for the internalizing
subscale (see Table 2 and Fig. 1). Follow up analyses
supported our hypothesis that the SM and MA groups
had significantly higher internalizing behaviors than the
control group (p<0.02), but they did not differ from each
other.

SSRS-Teacher Version: Social Competence

The MANCOVA indicated a significant multivariate main
effect for group on the social skills subscales (Wilks’Λ, F(6,
194) = 9.713, p=0.0005; see Table 3). A one-way ANCOVA
also revealed a significant main effect for group on the total
social skills scale (F(2, 99) = 9.126, p=0.001). Follow-up
tests indicated that the SM and MA groups had significantly
lower self-control (p’s<0.006) and total social skills than the
control group (p’s<0.02). Teachers also rated children with
SM lower in social assertion skills than the MA and control
group (p’s<0.006). Additionally, children with MA were
considered to be lower in social cooperation skills than the
control group (p<0.006).

SSRS-Teacher Version: Verbal and Nonverbal Social Skills                      Note: ** p < .02 for SSRS-Parent Version
                                                                                    ** p < .006 for SSRS-Teacher Version
The MANCOVA revealed a significant main effect of group
                                                                              Fig. 1 Primary caregiver and teacher report of internalizing behaviors
(Wilks’Λ, F(6,196) = 12.406, p=0.0005) on all of the                          on the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). Note: Internalizing
subscales (see Table 3). Follow-up tests showed that the                      behaviors were z-scored to standardize comparison across informants
1064                                                                                                   J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067

Table 3 Group Differences on Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) and Verbal and Nonverbal Social Behavior Measures as Completed by
Teachers

Measure                                                    Selective mutism           Mixed anxiety          Controls           f-ratio   p

                                                           Mean          SD           Mean        SD         Mean       SD

Social competence measures
 Social cooperation                                        14.5          (4.0)        12.9a        (5.5)     16.6a      (3.8)     4.8     0.010
 Social assertion                                           5.0b,c       (3.6)        10.3b        (4.7)     11.9c      (4.8)    22.5     0.0005
 Self-control                                              12.6d         (4.0)        12.7e        (5.0)     16.3d,e    (2.2)     5.8     0.004
 Total social skills                                       32.0f         (8.8)        35.8g       (13.1)     44.7f,g    (6.1)     9.1     0.0005
Problem behavior measures
 Externalizing behaviors                                     0.5h        (1.2)         2.9h,i      (3.6)      0.6i      (1.3)     8.0     0.001
 Internalizing behaviors                                     6.6k        (3.2)         4.9j        (3.1)      1.7j,k    (1.8)    17.5     0.0005
 Hyperactive behaviors                                       2.2         (2.3)         4.3         (4.0)      1.8       (2.4)     5.2     0.007
Verbal and nonverbal behavior measures
  Verbal social skills                                      2.0l,m       (2.4)         6.9l        (3.1)      7.8m      (2.2)    33.6     0.0005
  Nonverbal classroom cooperation and competence           18.1          (4.9)        16.n         (7.1)     21.2n      (4.2)     7.0     0.001
  Nonverbal social interactions                            10.9o         (4.8)        12.7         (4.6)     15.7o      (2.4)     5.5     0.005

Identical superscripts indicate statistical significance between those groups


classroom cooperation and competence skills compared to                       (Wilks’Λ, F(6,170) = 8.774, p=0.0005; see Table 3). In line
the control group (p<0.006).                                                  with our predictions, the SM and MA groups had
                                                                              significantly higher internalizing behavior scores than the
SSRS-Teacher Version: Problem Behaviors                                       control group (p <.006; see Table 3 and Fig. 1). The SM
                                                                              and control groups had significantly lower externalizing
The MANCOVA revealed a significant multivariate main                          behaviors than the MA group (p’s<0.006). There were no
effect for group on all of the problem behavior subscales                     other group differences following Bonferroni correction.




Note: ** p < .006
                                                                              Note: ** p < .003
Fig. 2 Primary caregiver and teacher report of verbal social skills on
the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). Note: Verbal social skills            Fig. 3 Primary caregiver report of social anxiety on the Screen for
were z-scored to standardize comparison across informants                     Child Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED)
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067                                                                                                1065


SCARED—Parent Version                                                          Our findings support and extend recent research (e.g.,
                                                                            Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006; Vecchio and Kearney
Anxiety Subscales The MANCOVA revealed a significant                        2005), suggesting that children with SM appear less
multivariate main effect for group (F(10, 284) = 12.5, p=                   socially competent and more prone to internalizing behav-
0.0005) on all of the subscales (see Table 4). A one-way                    iors compared to controls as reported by primary caregivers
ANCOVA also showed a significant main effect for group                      and teachers. More specifically, children with SM were
on the total anxiety score (F(2, 146) = 28.4, p=0.0005).                    rated significantly lower than controls on teacher-reported
While the SM and MA groups had significantly higher                         social assertion, self-control and total social skills and on
GAD, separation anxiety, and school avoidance scores (all                   primary caregiver reported social responsibility and total
p’s<0.003) as well as higher total anxiety (p<0.02) than the                social skills. Furthermore, consistent with findings by
control group, the two groups did differ from each other on                 Cunningham et al. (2006), children with SM were rated
levels of social anxiety and panic/somatic symptoms. More                   by both primary caregivers and teachers lower on verbal
specifically, the SM group had higher social anxiety scores                 social skills compared to both the MA and control groups.
than the MA group and the control group and the MA                          Consistent with our predictions, we also found that primary
group had higher social anxiety scores than the control                     caregivers indicated greater social anxiety levels in children
group (see Fig. 3). The MA group also had higher panic/                     with SM than the children reported for themselves, which is
somatic symptoms compared to the SM and control groups                      supportive of the idea that children with SM may use their
(all p’s<0.003).                                                            mutism as a compensatory strategy to reduce their anxiety
                                                                            (Yeganeh et al. 2003, 2006).
SCARED—Child Version                                                           As expected, given the general agreement in the literature
                                                                            that SM should be better classified as an anxiety disorder (e.g.
There were no significant group differences.                                Anstendig 1999; Sharp et al. 2007; Standart and Le Couteur
                                                                            2003), children with SM, and children with MA, did not
SCARED—Parent and Child Comparisons                                         significantly differ on many of the social skills and anxiety
                                                                            measures regardless of informant. For example, the SM and
A paired-samples t-test on the social anxiety subscale of the               MA groups did not significantly differ on internalizing
SCARED indicated significantly greater report of child                      behaviors, social cooperation, self-control, total social skills,
social anxiety by primary caregivers (M=11.7, SD=2.7)                       and on teacher reported nonverbal social skills, and class-
than self-report of social anxiety by the children (M=7.7,                  room cooperation and competence. The SM and MA groups
SD=3.5) (t(35) = 6.1, p<0.01).                                              additionally did not differ on primary caregiver reported
                                                                            internalizing behaviors, social assertion, self-control, nonver-
Discussion                                                                  bal social skills and total social skills. Also in line with our
                                                                            predictions, children with SM and anxiety disorders were not
The goal of the present study was to examine group                          significantly different on behaviors reflecting GAD, separa-
differences among children with SM, mixed anxiety, and                      tion anxiety, school avoidance, and overall anxiety, although
controls on measures of behavioral and socio-emotional                      primary caregivers rated the SM group significantly higher in
functioning. We used a relatively large sample size, multi-                 behaviors reflecting social anxiety than the MA and
informant report (i.e. primary caregiver, teacher, and child                control groups. Taken together, these results suggest that
self-report) and included an anxious comparison group.                      deficits in social abilities are evident in both children with


Table 4 Group Differences on the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) as Completed by Primary Caregivers

Measure                                     Selective mutism             Mixed anxiety           Controls                 f-ratio     p

                                            Mean           SD            Mean        SD          Mean          SD

Panic/significant somatic symptoms           3.1a           (2.9)         7.7a,b      (4.6)       3.7b         (3.0)      11.3        0.0005
Generalized anxiety disorder                 6.6c           (4.9)         8.7d        (5.5)       2.6c,d       (3.2)      18.1        0.0005
Separation anxiety disorder                  5.7e           (3.6)         6.5f        (4.3)       2.6e,f       (3.1)      11.5        0.0005
Social anxiety disorder                     11.7g,h         (2.7)         7.7g,i      (4.6)       3.7h,i       (3.0)      47.6        0.0005
School avoidance                             1.9j           (2.0)         2.4k        (2.0)       0.5j,k       (1.0)      13.9        0.0005
Total anxiety                               28.9l          (11.4)        30.2m       (16.2)      10.6l,m       (9.3)      28.4        0.0005

Identical superscripts indicate statistical significance between those groups
1066                                                                                      J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067


SM and children with anxiety disorders. Furthermore, the           Chair in Patient-Centred Health Care. The authors would like to thank
                                                                   Lindsay Bennett, Sue McKee, Renee Nossal, Matilda Nowakowski,
findings suggest that SM may be considered an anxiety
                                                                   and Jamie Sawyer for their assistance with data collection. We would
disorder characterized by social anxiety. More detailed            also like to thank the many children, their primary caregivers and
observational analyses of the types of social inhibitions          teachers for their participation in the study.
experienced by children and youth with social phobia but
who speak at school versus those with SM and SP versus             References
those with SM but less social inhibition is warranted. This
approach would also aid in the understanding of the role           American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
of oral speech in social skill development in children and              manual of mental disorders DSM-IV-TR. Washington: Author.
may help to refine the classification of SM.                       Anstendig, K. D. (1999). Is selective mutism an anxiety disorder?
                                                                        Rethinking its DSM-IV classification. Journal of Anxiety
                                                                        Disorders, 13, 417–434.
Limitations                                                        Bergman, R. L., Piacentini, J., & McCracken, J. T. (2002). Prevalence
                                                                        and description of selective mutism in a school-based sample.
There were at least three limitations of the present study.             Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
                                                                        Psychiatry, 41, 938–946.
First, although there was no systematic loss of data among         Bernstein, G. A., Bernat, D. H., Davis, A. A., & Layne, A. E. (2008).
the three groups, several questionnaire packages from the               Symptom presentation and classroom functioning in a nonclinical
child’s teacher were not returned, which reduced the sample             sample of children with social phobia. Depression and Anxiety,
size for the teacher analyses and perhaps reliability. Second,          25, 752–760.
                                                                   Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D., Cully, M., Balach, L.,
the mean substitution method for filling in missing data for            Kaufman, J., et al. (1997). The screen for child anxiety related
some of our measures has been criticized by some (e.g.,                 emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psycho-
Enders 2006). However, given the small percentage of                    metric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child
missing data in this sample the mean substitution method is             and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 545–553.
                                                                   Black, B., & Uhde, T. W. (1992). Elective mutism as a variant of
not likely to have impacted results in a meaningful way.                social phobia. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Third, some research (e.g., Comer and Kendall 2004)                     Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 1090–1094.
suggests that parental psychopathology (e.g. anxiety) may          Black, B., & Uhde, T. W. (1995). Psychiatric characteristics of children
influence parental ratings of their child’s psychopathology.            with selective mutism: A pilot study. Journal of the American
                                                                        Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 847–856.
We were, however, not able to examine whether any of the           Brown, J., & Llyod, H. (1975). A controlled study of not speaking in
parents met clinical diagnosis for psychopathology.                     school. Journal of the Association of Workers for Maladjusted
                                                                        Children, 3, 49–63.
Conclusion and Implications                                        Clarizio, H. (1994). Assessment of depression in children and
                                                                        adolescents by parents, teachers and peers. In W. Reynolds &
                                                                        H. Johnston (Eds.), Handbook of depression in children and
We found that children with selective mutism were rated to              adolescents (pp. 235–248). New York: Plenum Press.
have less social competence than controls by both primary          Cohan, S. L., Chavira, D. A., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Practitioner
caregivers and teachers. SM children, however, were not                 review: Psychosocial interventions for children with selective
                                                                        mutism: A critical evaluation of the literature from 1990–2005.
rated significantly different on a number of social skills and          Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1085–1097.
anxiety measures than children with anxiety disorders.             Cohan, S. L., Price, J. M., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Suffering in silence:
Importantly, children with SM were rated higher in levels of            Why a developmental psychopathology perspective on selective
social anxiety compared to their anxious and typically                  mutism is needed. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
                                                                        Pediatrics, 27, 341–355.
developing counterparts. Based upon the present findings, it       Comer, J. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2004). A symptom-level examination
is plausible that SM may be conceptualized as an anxiety                of parent-child agreement in the diagnosis of anxious youths.
disorder with a primary deficit in social functioning. This             Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
suggestion further impacts the formation of future diagnos-             Psychiatry, 43, 878–886.
                                                                   Cunningham, C. E., McHolm, A. E., & Boyle, M. H. (2006). Social
tic manuals: It may be more appropriate to classify SM as               phobia, anxiety, oppositional behavior, social skills, and self-
an anxiety disorder, and more specifically a social anxiety             concept in children with specific selective mutism, generalized
disorder, than the currently ambiguous classification of SM             selective mutism, and community controls. European Child &
in ‘Other Disorders of Infancy, Childhood, and Adoles-                  Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 245–255.
                                                                   Cunningham, C. E., McHolm, A., Boyle, M. H., & Patel, S. (2004).
cence’. Furthermore, the present findings have implications             Behavioral and emotional adjustment, family functioning, aca-
for clinical practice, whereby social skills training merits            demic performance, and social relationships in children with
inclusion in intervention for children with anxiety disorders           selective mutism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
as well as children with SM.                                            45, 1363–1372.
                                                                   Dummit, E. S., Klein, R. G., Tancer, N. K., & Asche, B. (1997).
                                                                        Systematic assessment of 50 children with selective mutism.
Acknowledgements This research was supported by a grant from            Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
the Ontario Mental Health Foundation (OMHF) and the Jack Laidlaw        Psychiatry, 36, 653–660.
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067                                                                                                 1067

Elizur, Y., & Perednik, R. (2003). Prevalence and description of           Mclnnes, A., & Manassis, K. (2005). When silence is not golden: An
     selective mutism in immigrant and native families: A controlled            integrated approach to selective mutism. Seminars in Speech and
     study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent             Language, 26, 201–210.
     Psychiatry, 42, 1451–1459.                                            Nowakowski, M. E., Cunningham, C. C., McHolm, A. E., Evans, M.
Enders, C. K. (2006). A primer on the use of modern missing-data                A., Edison, S., St. Pierre, J., et al. (2009). Language and
     methods in Psychosomatic medicine research. Psychosomatic                  academic abilities in children with selective mutism. Infant and
     Medicine, 68, 427–736.                                                     Child Development, 18, 271–290.
Ford, M. A., Sladeczek, I. E., Carlson, J., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1998).   Remschmidt, H., Poller, M., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Hennighausen,
     Selective mutism: Phenomenological characteristics. School                 K., & Gutenbrunner, C. (2001). A follow-up study of 45 patients
     Psychology Quarterly, 12, 192–227.                                         with elective mutism. European Archives of Psychiatry and
Garcia, A. M., Freeman, J. B., Francis, G., Miller, L. M., & Leonard,           Clinical Neuroscience, 251, 284–296.
     H. L. (2004). Selective mutism. New York: Oxford University           Schniering, C. A., Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2000). Issues in the
     Press.                                                                     diagnosis and assessment of anxiety disorders in children and
Giddan, J. J., Ross, G. J., Sechler, L. L., & Becker, B. R. (1997).             adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 453–478.
     Selective mutism in elementary school: Multidisciplinary inter-       Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone,
     ventions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,               M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children
     28, 127–133.                                                               version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from
Gresham, F. M., & Elliot, S. N. (1990). Social skills rating system.            previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses.
     Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.                                   Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Biederman, J., Henin, A., Faraone, S. V.,              Psychiatry, 39, 28–38.
     Davis, S., Harrington, K., et al. (2007). Behavioral inhibition in    Sharp, W. G., Sherman, C., & Gross, A. M. (2007). Selective mutism
     preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle               and anxiety: A review of the current conceptualization of the
     childhood social anxiety: A five-year follow-up. Journal of                disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 568–579.
     Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28, 225–233.                 Standart, S., & Le Couteur, A. (2003). The quiet child: A literature
Kolvin, I., & Fundudis, T. (1981). Elective mute children: Psycho-              review of selective mutism. Child and Adolescent Mental Health,
     logical development and background factors. Journal of Child               8, 154–160.
     Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 219–232.                               Steinhausen, H., & Juzi, C. (1996). Elective mutism: An analysis of
Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (1997). Selective mutism: A population-                100 cases. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
     based study: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and              Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 606–614.
     Psychiatry, 38, 257–262.                                              Vecchio, J. L., & Kearney, C. A. (2005). Selective mutism in children:
Kristensen, H. (2000). Selective mutism and comorbidity with                    Comparison to youths with and without anxiety disorders. Journal
     developmental disorder/delay, anxiety disorder, and elimination            of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 31–37.
     disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and                Voncken, M. J., Alden, L. E., Bögels, S. M., & Roelofs, J. (2008).
     Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 249–256.                                        Social rejection in social anxiety disorder: The role of perfor-
Kristensen, H., & Torgersen, S. (2002). A case-control study of EAS             mance deficits, evoked negative emotions and dissimilarity.
     child and parental temperaments in selectively mute children with          British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 439–450.
     and without a co-morbid communication disorder. Nordic                Yeganeh, R., Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2006). Selective mutism:
     Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 347–353.                                        More than social anxiety? Depression and Anxiety, 23, 117–123.
Kumpulainen, K. (2002). Phenomenology and treatment of selective           Yeganeh, R., Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Pina, A. A., & Silverman,
     mutism. CNS Drugs, 16, 175–180.                                            W. K. (2003). Clinical distinctions between selective mutism and
Kumpulainen, K., Räsänen, E., Raaska, H., & Somppi, V. (1998).                  social phobia: An investigation of childhood psychopathology.
     Selective mutism among second-graders in elementary school.                Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
     European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 24–29.                          Psychiatry, 42, 1069–1075.

Contenu connexe

Tendances

Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the chViolence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the chojas18
 
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and FriendshipWeinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and FriendshipTamara Weinstein
 
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation andThe relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation andojas18
 
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceCallaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceOmar Gonzalez-Valentino
 
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewLit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewroxcine
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Inman R
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Inman RAu Psy492 M7 A2 Inman R
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Inman Rrobininman
 
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitNarcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitamit657720
 
Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...
Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...
Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...Clare Mehta
 
Week 15 Twin Studies
Week 15 Twin StudiesWeek 15 Twin Studies
Week 15 Twin StudiesJamie Davies
 
Research project power point
Research project power pointResearch project power point
Research project power pointafonderwhite
 

Tendances (19)

Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the chViolence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
Violence prevention programs an exploratory study of the ch
 
Literature review
Literature reviewLiterature review
Literature review
 
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and FriendshipWeinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
 
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation andThe relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
The relationship between types ofchildhood victimisation and
 
Psihologija
PsihologijaPsihologija
Psihologija
 
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image DisturbanceCallaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
Callaghan, et. al. 2012-An Empirical Model of Body Image Disturbance
 
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewLit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Inman R
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Inman RAu Psy492 M7 A2 Inman R
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Inman R
 
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitNarcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
 
Final Paper Olszower
Final Paper OlszowerFinal Paper Olszower
Final Paper Olszower
 
Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...
Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...
Mehta, Alfonso, Delaney, & Ayotte_Associations between mixed gender friendshi...
 
senior sem paper
senior sem papersenior sem paper
senior sem paper
 
Casual sex and Well-Being
Casual sex and Well-BeingCasual sex and Well-Being
Casual sex and Well-Being
 
Week 15 Twin Studies
Week 15 Twin StudiesWeek 15 Twin Studies
Week 15 Twin Studies
 
Hnc psychology report
Hnc psychology reportHnc psychology report
Hnc psychology report
 
Research project power point
Research project power pointResearch project power point
Research project power point
 
FINAL thesis 4.28
FINAL thesis 4.28FINAL thesis 4.28
FINAL thesis 4.28
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Jones K
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Jones KAu Psy492 M7 A2 Jones K
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Jones K
 
Halford Capstone Paper
Halford Capstone PaperHalford Capstone Paper
Halford Capstone Paper
 

Similaire à Fulltext

). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism
). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism
). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutismbyepluto
 
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disordersHandbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disordersSpringer
 
Bullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docx
Bullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docxBullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docx
Bullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docxcurwenmichaela
 
O R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docx
O R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docxO R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docx
O R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docxhopeaustin33688
 
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]Italo Santana
 
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]Italo Santana
 
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...Justin Knight
 
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)MarioBuzz1
 
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docxThe Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docxcherry686017
 
J Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docx
J Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docxJ Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docx
J Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docxpriestmanmable
 
LauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final PaperLauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final PaperSasha Albrecht
 
Chronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects Upon
Chronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects UponChronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects Upon
Chronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects Uponsportydebb
 
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And DistressAdolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And DistressDarian Pruitt
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective studylucacerniglia
 
Writing Sample MA Thesis
Writing Sample MA ThesisWriting Sample MA Thesis
Writing Sample MA ThesisJoseph M. Finck
 
Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...
Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...
Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...inventionjournals
 
Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...
Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...
Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...James Cook University
 

Similaire à Fulltext (20)

). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism
). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism
). Language and academic abilities in children with selective mutism
 
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disordersHandbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
 
Bullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docx
Bullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docxBullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docx
Bullying and PTSD SymptomsThormod Idsoe & Atle Dyregrov & .docx
 
O R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docx
O R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docxO R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docx
O R I G I N A L P A P E RSelf-Reported Depressive Symptoms.docx
 
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
 
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
Letter autismo e down jaacap s-06-00468[1]
 
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
 
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
 
IPA_JHP
IPA_JHPIPA_JHP
IPA_JHP
 
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docxThe Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
The Experiences of Adults Exposed toIntimate Partner Violenc.docx
 
J Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docx
J Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docxJ Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docx
J Youth Adolescence (2007) 361048–1057DOI 10.1007s10964-00.docx
 
LauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final PaperLauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final Paper
 
Chronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects Upon
Chronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects UponChronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects Upon
Chronically Ill Children And The Psychosocial Effects Upon
 
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And DistressAdolescent Adjustment And Well Being  Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
Adolescent Adjustment And Well Being Effects Of Parental Divorce And Distress
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective study
 
Writing Sample MA Thesis
Writing Sample MA ThesisWriting Sample MA Thesis
Writing Sample MA Thesis
 
CCHD_2007
CCHD_2007CCHD_2007
CCHD_2007
 
Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...
Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...
Gender and Age Differences in Social Anxiety among Iranian Immigrants Childre...
 
Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...
Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...
Parental stress, affective symptoms and marital satisfaction in parents of ch...
 
Who Is Responsible
Who Is ResponsibleWho Is Responsible
Who Is Responsible
 

Fulltext

  • 1. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 DOI 10.1007/s10802-010-9425-y Behavioral and Socio-emotional Functioning in Children with Selective Mutism: A Comparison with Anxious and Typically Developing Children Across Multiple Informants Diana Carbone & Louis A. Schmidt & Charles C. Cunningham & Angela E. McHolm & Shannon Edison & Jeff St. Pierre & Michael H. Boyle Published online: 23 May 2010 # Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010 Abstract We examined differences among 158 children, 44 implications for clinical practice, whereby social skills with selective mutism (SM; M=8.2 years, SD=3.4 years), 65 training merits inclusion in intervention for children with with mixed anxiety (MA; M=8.9 years, SD=3.2 years), and anxiety disorders as well as children with SM. 49 community controls (M=7.7 years, SD=2.6 years) on primary caregiver, teacher, and child reports of behavioral Keywords Selective mutism . Mixed anxiety . Children . and socio-emotional functioning. Children with SM were Social skills . Social anxiety . Parent and teacher reports . rated lower than controls on a range of social skills, but the Internalizing problems SM and MA groups did not significantly differ on many of the social skills and anxiety measures. However, children with SM were rated higher than children with MA and Selective mutism (SM) is a disorder marked by a consistent controls on social anxiety. Findings suggest that SM may be failure to speak in certain social situations (e.g., at school) conceptualized as an anxiety disorder, with primary deficits despite the presence of speech in other social situations in social functioning and social anxiety. This interpretation (e.g., at home; American Psychiatric Association 2000). supports a more specific classification of SM as an anxiety SM is largely considered rare with prevalence rates disorder for future diagnostic manuals than is currently estimated to be between 0.03% and 0.2% (Bergman et al. described in the literature. The present findings also have 2002; Brown and Lloyd 1975; Elizur and Perednik 2003; Kolvin and Fundudis 1981; Kopp and Gillberg 1997; Kumpulainen et al. 1998), with higher rates identified in D. Carbone : L. A. Schmidt immigrant populations (Elizur and Perednik 2003). Preva- McMaster Integrative Neuroscience, Discovery, & Study (MiNDS), McMaster University, lence rates for SM are variable in the literature and appear Hamilton, ON, Canada to be influenced by the origin of research, diagnostic criteria, age of children, immigrant status, and setting in L. A. Schmidt (*) which SM is sampled (e.g., clinic versus school setting) Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour, McMaster University, (Bergman et al. 2002; Kumpulainen 2002; Sharp et al. Hamilton, ON L8S 4K1, Canada 2007). There is little consensus as to the sex ratio of the e-mail: schmidtl@mcmaster.ca disorder, with clinically-referred samples reporting a slightly C. C. Cunningham : A. E. McHolm : S. Edison : M. H. Boyle higher prevalence of SM in females than males (e.g., Department of Psychiatry & Behavioural Neurosciences, Cunningham et al. 2004; Dummit et al. 1997; Kristensen McMaster University, 2000). Other studies investigating community- and school- Hamilton, ON, Canada based samples suggest comparable occurrence between the sexes (e.g., Bergman et al. 2002; Elizur and Perednik 2003). J. St. Pierre Child and Parent Resource Institute (CPRI), Although SM has a typical onset before the age of five, London, ON, Canada the disorder often does not become evident until school
  • 2. 1058 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 entry, when expectations (and related pressures) to speak cooperation, than controls. More recently, Cunningham et increase (Cunningham et al. 2004; Garcia et al. 2004; al. (2006) used the SSRS again and found that both Giddan et al. 1997). Despite this early onset, children are teachers and parents rated children with SM to be lower on not commonly referred for clinical assessment until they are verbal and nonverbal social skills than control children, but between approximately 6.5 to 9 years of age (Ford et al. the two groups were not different on measures of nonverbal 1998; Kumpulainen et al. 1998; Standart and Le Couteur social cooperation. 2003). SM may persist for a few months to several years, In addition to the work by Cunningham and colleagues and adults diagnosed with SM as children often continue to (2004, 2006), a study by Vecchio and Kearney (2005) suffer with social anxiety and deficits in social communi- found that children with SM and children with anxiety cation, in addition to displaying other problems with socio- disorders were rated by teachers and parents to have greater emotional and daily adjustment (Remschmidt et al. 2001). internalizing behaviors than controls. Vecchio and Kearney Despite being diagnostically well-documented (Dummit et also found that children with SM and children with anxiety al. 1997; Kopp and Gillberg 1997; Sharp et al. 2007), the disorders did not differ from children in a control group on etiology of SM remains equivocal. externalizing behaviors. Furthermore, children with SM had Currently, SM is ambiguously classified under ‘Other significantly greater total comorbid diagnoses and anxiety Disorders of Infancy, Childhood and Adolescence’ in the disorder diagnoses than children with anxiety disorders, current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV-TR, based on child, but not parent, report. American Psychiatric Association 2000). This classification There are, in addition, other studies, which have used is largely disputed as the greater consensus in the literature clinician, parent, and children’s self-report measures to classifies SM as an anxiety disorder (Anstendig 1999; compare children with SM to children with SP (Yeganeh Sharp et al. 2007; Standart and Le Couteur 2003). The basis et al. 2003, 2006). For example, Yeganeh et al. (2003, for the designation of SM as an anxiety disorder comes 2006) found that children with SM did not report greater from four main findings. First, strong comorbidity with and levels of social anxiety than children with SP, despite characteristic similarities between SM and anxiety disor- clinician and observer ratings of greater or equal levels of ders, specifically social phobia (SP), have been identified in social anxiety in the SM and SP groups. Yeganeh and the literature (e.g., Black and Uhde 1995; Dummit et al. colleagues (2003, 2006) suggested that children with SM 1997; Sharp et al. 2007; Vecchio and Kearney 2005). There do not report as large a degree of anxiety, since their is even the suggestion that SM may be a subtype (Black & inability to talk may serve as a compensatory strategy to Uhde, 1992) or developmental precursor (Bergman et al. reduce their anxiety. 2002) to SP in some cases. Second, there is evidence Studies that have examined the socio-emotional charac- showing a greater prevalence of anxiety disorders amongst teristics of SM have been limited by several factors, relatives of children with SM than among typically including small sample sizes (e.g., Vecchio and Kearney developing children, which further implicates genetic 2005; Yeganeh et al. 2003, 2006) and a lack of other factors in the etiology of the disorder (Black and Uhde clinical group comparisons (e.g., anxious children; 1995; Cohan et al. 2006b; Kristensen and Torgerson 2002). Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006). Furthermore, few studies Third, both SM (Ford et al. 1998) and anxiety disorders have examined social skills in children with SM. Studies (e.g. Hirshfeld-Becker et al. 2007) have been associated that address these issues are needed, given the argument with similar temperaments, namely, behavioral inhibition. that 1) SM would be better classified as an anxiety disorder Lastly, SM and anxiety disorders both share common (Anstendig 1999; Sharp et al. 2007; Standart and Le psychotherapeutic and pharmacological treatments (Cohan Couteur 2003), 2) there are conceptual and behavioral et al. 2006a; Standart and Le Couteur 2003). similarities between SM and SP, and 3) the reported Recent research has examined the socio-emotional char- findings of children (e.g., Bernstein et al. 2008) and adults acteristics of children with SM. For example, Cunningham (e.g., Voncken et al. 2008) with SP suffer from social skills and colleagues (2004; Cunningham et al. 2006) have deficits. suggested that children with SM exhibit lower social A further limitation concerns the issue that most studies competence compared to their typically developed peers. examining SM have largely been informed by parent report Using the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham and despite the recommendation that a multi-method, multi- Elliot 1990), Cunningham et al. (2004) found that children informant approach be utilized in the assessment of anxiety with SM were rated significantly lower than controls on disorders (Schniering et al. 2000) and SM (Mclnnes and the parent report of social assertion, social responsibility, Manassis 2005). Given the personal nature of fears, self- social cooperation, and social control subscales. Compar- report is a recommended component of the multi-modal atively, teachers considered children with SM to exhibit battery (Schniering et al. 2000). Teacher ratings of a child’s less social assertion, but not social control or social socio-emotional behavior, in addition to parent and self-
  • 3. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 1059 report, are also imperative, since children spend substantial mental health agencies in Southern Ontario and amounts of time in the classroom during their formative children from the McMaster Child Database. Children years (Clarizio 1994). Moreover, there exist anxiety- classified as controls were also obtained from the provoking situations unique to the school setting about McMaster Child Database only, and all were healthy which only a teacher may be apt to report. Accordingly, without mental health problems. The McMaster Child teacher assessments are essential for children who typically Database contains the names of children from the do not speak in the school setting. There is, however, scant community who were recruited at birth from the McMas- research on SM that has included teacher reports. ter University Medical Centre and St. Joseph’s Healthcare, Hamilton, Ontario. Parents consented for their infant’s inclusion in the McMaster Child Database if they were The Present Study interested in participating in future research studies. Participant demographics can be found in Table 1. In order to extend the recent work of Cunningham and colleagues (2004, 2006) and clarify other previous findings, Selective Mutism (SM) Group we compared a relatively large sample of children with SM to children with mixed anxiety (MA) disorders and The SM group comprised 44 children (female n=23, male typically developing children on teacher, primary caregiver, n=21), with a mean age of 8.2 years (SD=3.4 years). The and child self-report measures of social and emotional children were included in the SM group if their primary functioning. Based on the prior findings by Cunningham caregivers or teachers indicated that the child did not speak and his colleagues (e.g., Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006), we in two or more situations on the Speech Situations predicted that children with SM would exhibit significantly Questionnaire—Parent Version (SSQ—Parent; or the lower verbal and nonverbal social competence than Speech Situations Questionnaire—Teacher version (SSQ- typically developing children as rated by both primary Teacher; Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006). Classification in caregivers and teachers. Given that SM appears to be the SM group also required that the absence of speaking associated with internalizing rather than externalizing was not due to a communication disorder, and that the factors (e.g. Black and Uhde 1995; Dummit et al. 1997; absence of speaking persisted for a minimum of one month. Ford et al. 1998; Kristensen 2000; Steinhausen and Juzi The same criteria used to classify children with SM in the 1996; Vecchio and Kearney 2005), we also anticipated that present study have also been used elsewhere (Nowakowski children with SM would be significantly higher on et al. 2009). internalizing behaviors and lower on externalizing, hyper- Primary caregiver report on the internalizing section of active and problem behaviors than typically developing the Computerized Diagnostic Individual Schedule for Child children. Since it may be the case that children with SM do (C-DISC IV; Shaffer et al. 2000) was used to examine the not report as great a degree of anxiety as indicated by other number of internalizing disorders for which the participants informants (Yeganeh et al. 2003, 2006), we expected that met criteria. The C-DISC IV was not used to classify self-report of social anxiety by children with SM would not children with SM, since its reliability has been established differ from children with MA and typically developing for anxiety disorders but not for SM. There were seven children, but primary caregivers of children with SM would (15.9%) children classified as SM for which the C-DISC IV indicate higher levels of social anxiety than children with could not be obtained because primary caregivers could not MA and typically developing children. We further hypoth- be reached via telephone to conduct the questionnaire. esized that social skills and anxiety would not be different Twenty-eight of the 37 (63.6%) children classified as SM between the SM and MA groups, if SM is conceptualized were diagnosed with one or more anxiety disorders as per as an anxiety disorder. the C-DISC IV. The most common anxiety disorder diagnosis was specific phobia (29.5%), followed by social phobia (18.2%), agoraphobia (15.9%), separation anxiety Method (13.6%), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD; 2.3%), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; 2.3%), and post- Participants traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; 2.3%). Participants were 158 (81 males, 77 females; M=8.4 years, Mixed Anxiety (MA) Group SD=3.1 years) children. Children with SM consisted of referrals from children’s mental health agencies in The MA group included 65 children (female n=29, male Southern Ontario. Children classified as mixed anxiety n=36), with a mean age of 8.9 years (SD=3.2 years). The (MA) were a combination of referrals from children’s children were classified in the MA group if they had one or
  • 4. 1060 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 Table 1 Demographic Information Selective mutism Mixed anxiety Community control Statistic p-value (n=44) (n=65) (n=49) Percent female 52.3 44.6 51.0 X2(2) = 0.7 0.682 Mean (SD) age of child in years 8.2 (3.4) 8.9 (3.2) 7.7 (2.6) F(2,155) = 2.2 0.166 Percent in regular classroom 97.7 95.3a 100a X2(4) = 2.9 0.577 Age range of primary caregiver <19 (n=1) 19–39 (n=25) 19–39a (n=21) X2(4) = 2.9 0.570 19–39 (n=17) 40–64 (n=40) 40–64 (n=27) 40–64 (n=26) Approximate total income of X2(12) = 35.5 0.0005 household, in prior year, before taxes Less than $15 000 1a 2a 0a $ 15 000–$30 000 3 10 1 $ 30 000–$45 000 6 5 0 $ 45 000–$60 000 0 11 2 $ 60 000–$75 000 12 5 8 $ 75 000–$90 000 11 14 21 Greater than $100 000 10 17 13 a indicates group with missing data for that demographic variable more anxiety disorder diagnoses on the C-DISC IV (social comprehend the questions. A questionnaire package was phobia, separation anxiety, specific phobia, panic disorder, also sent out to the child’s teacher in which he/she reported GAD, PTSD, OCD, or agoraphobia), and no diagnosis of on the child’s social skills and problem behaviors. SM as per teacher and primary caregiver report on the The measures collected in the present study were part of Speech Situations Questionnaires. Of the 65 children in the a larger study examining the etiology, academic abilities, MA group, 40 (61.5%) had one anxiety disorder, and 25 familial characteristics, and behavioral and psychophysio- (38.5%) had two or more anxiety disorders as per the C- logical correlates of children with SM. Data collection took DISC IV. The most common anxiety disorder diagnosis place at the Child Emotion Laboratory at McMaster among the MA group was specific phobia (67.7%), University. All procedures were approved by the McMaster followed by social phobia (27.7%), separation anxiety University Health Sciences Research Ethics Board. The (23.1%), GAD (16.9%), OCD (10.8%), panic disorder child received a toy or gift certificate as a token of our (6.2%), and PTSD (1.5%). appreciation at the end of the visit. Community Control Group Primary Caregiver Report Measures The typically developing group comprised 49 children Diagnostic Questionnaires The Speech Situations Ques- (female n=25, male n=24), with a mean age of 7.7 years tionnaire—Parent Version (SSQ-Parent; Cunningham et al. (SD=2.6 years). Children were classified as typically 2004, 2006) assesses a child’s speech patterns in a number developing if they had no diagnoses as per the CDISC IV, of situations and to several different people as reported by and if they did not meet criteria for SM based on primary the primary caregiver. High internal consistencies of 0.82 caregiver and teacher report on the Speech Situations (Cunningham et al. 2006) and 0.92 (Nowakowski et al. Questionnaires. 2009) have been found in prior studies. An internal consistency of 0.92 was found in the present study. Procedure Additionally, primary caregiver report on the internaliz- ing section of the computer assisted Diagnostic Interview Following arrival at the Child Emotion Laboratory, the Schedule for Children (C-DISC IV; Shaffer et al. 2000) was parent and child were briefed about the procedures and obtained by a trained research assistant via telephone written consent was obtained. The primary caregiver then following the initial laboratory visit. The C-DISC IV is a filled out a number of questionnaires. Child self-report of structured diagnostic interview that evaluates the presence anxiety was also obtained if the child was old enough to of 34 psychiatric disorders in children based on the DSM-
  • 5. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 1061 IV (Shaffer et al. 2000). Panic disorder, GAD, social Teacher Report Measures phobia, specific phobia, separation anxiety, OCD, PTSD, agoraphobia, and major depression are assessed on the Teacher Diagnostic Questionnaires The Speech Situations internalizing section of the C-DISC IV. Only the internal- Questionnaire—Teacher Version (SSQ-Teacher; Cunningham izing section of the C-DISC IV was administered due to et al. 2004, 2006) assesses a child’s speech patterns in a time contraints. number of situations at school and to several different people as reported by the teacher. A high internal consistency of 0.95 Social Competence and Problem Behavior Measures was reported recently (Nowakowski et al. 2009) and was 0.95 Primary caregivers completed the Social Skills Rating in the present study. System (SSRS)—Parent Version (Gresham and Elliot 1990), which is comprised of nine different scales. There Social Competence and Problem Behavior Measures are four scales that examine social skills: social assertion, Teachers also completed a version of the SSRS that yields social cooperation, social responsibility, and self-control; identical scales to that of the parent version with the these four sum to a total social skills scale. exception of a social responsibility scale. The SSRS has three scales that pertain to externaliz- ing, internalizing, and hyperactive (only grades K-6) Verbal and Nonverbal Social Interactions We also com- behaviors subscales, which sum to a total problem puted subscales developed by Cunningham et al. (2006) to behaviors scale. Internal consistency among the scales assess verbal social interactions, nonverbal social interactions of the SSRS—Parent Version has been reported between and nonverbal classroom cooperation and competence. 0.87 and 0.90 (as cited in Cunningham et al. 2004). The Internal consistency among the scales of SSRS—Teacher internal consistencies for grades K-6 in the present study Version has been reported between 0.93 and 0.94 (as cited in were between 0.62 and 0.88, and for grades 7–12 were Cunningham et al. 2004). The internal consistencies for between 0.74 and 0.91. grades K-6 in the present study were between 0.87 and 0.96, and for grades 7–12 were between 0.72 and 0.93. Cunning- Verbal and Nonverbal Social Skills We also computed three ham et al. (2006) reported internal consistencies of 0.90, scales developed by Cunningham et al. (2006) to assess 0.94 and 0.92 for the verbal social interaction subscale, verbal social skills, nonverbal social skills, and nonverbal nonverbal social interaction subscale, and nonverbal class- cooperation subscales. Cunningham et al. (2006) reported room cooperation and competence subscale, respectively. In internal consistencies of 0.78, 0.71, and 0.78 for the verbal the present study, the internal consistencies were: 0.87 for social skills, nonverbal social skills, and nonverbal cooper- grades K-6 and 0.80 for grades 7–12 verbal social ation subscales, respectively. For the present study, internal interactions scales, 0.86 for grades K-6 and 0.90 for grades consistencies were 0.70 for grades K-6 and 0.58 for grades 7–12 nonverbal social interaction scales, and 0.92 for grades 7–12 verbal social skills scales, and 0.83 for grades K-6, K-6 and 0.88 for grades 7–12 nonverbal classroom compe- and 0.76 for grades 7–12 nonverbal social skills scales, and tence scales. were 0.81 for grades K-6, and 0.85 for grades 7–12 for the nonverbal social competence scales. Child Report Measures Screen for Child Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) Although reliability for the SCARED has been tested for The SCARED (Birmaher et al. 1997) yields five scales children between the ages of 9 to 18 (Birmaher et al. 1997), related to five different anxiety disorders: panic disorder/ we administered the SCARED—Child Version if the child significant somatic symptoms, GAD, social anxiety disor- was able to comprehend the questions. The mean age for der, separation anxiety disorder, and significant school children who completed the SCARED—Child Version was avoidance. We also computed a measure of total anxiety, 9.4 (SD=2.9) years. The SCARED—Child Version has which was a sum of all of the questions on the SCARED— identical questions and yields identical scales as the Parent Version. The parent version of the SCARED has an SCARED—Parent Version. The child version of the internal consistency of 0.74 (Birmaher et al. 1997). The SCARED has an internal consistency of 0.93 (Birmaher et internal consistencies for subscales of the SCARED— al. 1997). We also computed a total score for the SCARED— Parent Version in the present study were between 0.63 Child Version, which was the total sum of all of the questions and 0.75, and for the total score was 0.92 for children on the SCARED—Child Version. The internal consistencies below age eight. Internal consistencies for subscales of the for subscales of the SCARED—Child Version in the present SCARED—Parent Version in the present study were study were between 0.56 and 0.82, and for the total score was between 0.76 and 0.93 and for the total score was 0.95 0.86 for children below age eight. Internal consistencies of the for children eight and above. SCARED—Child Version in the present study were between
  • 6. 1062 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 0.63 and 0.83, and for the total score, it was 0.92 for children using income as a covariate. All post hoc analyses were eight and above. Bonferroni corrected. The total social skills scale for the SSRS-Parent and Missing Data Teacher Versions and the total anxiety scales for the SCARED Parent and Child Versions were not included in the MAN- Data were missing if specific questions were not completed, COVA analyses as they represent the sum of the subscales in questions were deemed non-applicable to the child by the each of the questionnaires. The total scales from these scales primary caregiver or teacher, certain questions were missed, were analyzed using separate one-way ANCOVAs, with entire questionnaires were not returned to the laboratory, or group (SM, MA, Control) as the between subject-factor and the child was too young to understand the question(s) in the income as a covariate. All post hoc analyses were Bonferroni case of the SCARED-Child Version. Missing questions for corrected. The total problem behaviors scale was not the SSRS-Parent and Teacher Version were filled out in analyzed, as it is the sum of three different constructs accordance with the protocol outlined in Gresham and (internalizing, externalizing and hyperactive behaviors) that Elliot (1990). Missing questions for the SCARED-Parent should be analyzed and reported separately. and Child Versions were filled in by computing the average To assess whether primary caregiver and child report of of the questions pertaining to a specific scale that the anxiety differed within the SM and MA groups, paired-samples missing question was relevant to and then recalculating the t-tests were conducted using the SCARED—Parent and Child scale. Importantly, the groups did not significantly differ on Versions total anxiety score and social anxiety score. the amount of missing data. There were a total of seven participants missing the SSRS—Parent Version, 25 partic- ipants missing the SSRS—Teacher Version, eight partic- Results ipants missing the SCARED—Child Version, and five participants missing the SCARED—Parent Version. Of the SSRS-Parent Version: Social Competence remaining participants, there were 1.1% data missing from the SSRS—Parent Version, 0.6% data missing from the Consistent with our predictions, the MANCOVA for the SSRS—Teacher Version, 0.09% data missing from the social skills subscales indicated a significant multivariate SCARED—Parent Version, and 0.06% data missing from main effect of group (Wilks’Λ, F(8,256) = 6.886, p=0.0005) the SCARED—Child Version. that further indicated a significant main effect of group on the social assertion, social responsibility, and self-control, Data Analyses but not the social cooperation subscales (see Table 2). A one-way ANCOVA also indicated that there was a Income in the prior year before taxes differed significantly significant main effect of group on the total social skills scale among the three groups (X2(12) = 35.5, p<0.05) and was (F(2,131) = 12.3, p=0.0005; see Table 2). significantly correlated with all of the measures in the study Follow-up tests confirmed that the SM and MA groups except for the SSRS—Teacher Version social assertion and had significantly lower social assertion (p<0.006) and total verbal social skills scales, the SCARED—Child Version, social skills (all p’s<0.02) than the control group. Primary and the SCARED—Parent Version social anxiety and caregivers also rated the SM group lower in social school avoidance scales. Accordingly, income was consid- responsibility skills than the MA and control groups (all ered as a covariate in all analyses. p’s<0.006). There were no significant group differences on To examine differences among the three groups on the the self-control scale following Bonferroni correction. various behavioral and socio-emotional functioning mea- sures completed by the primary caregiver, teacher, and SSRS-Parent Version: Verbal and Nonverbal Social Skills children, separate one-way multivariate analyses of covari- ance (MANCOVA) tests were conducted, with group (SM, The MANCOVA for the verbal social skills scale, nonver- MA, Control) as the between-subject factor and the bal social skills scale, and nonverbal cooperation scale subscales (e.g., SSRS-Parent subscales: social cooperation, revealed a main effect of group (Wilks’Λ, F(6, 260) = social assertion, social responsibility, self-control) as the 11.679, p=0.0005) for all of the subscales (see Table 2). dependent variables. A similar MANCOVA was performed The SM group had significantly lower verbal social skills with group (SM, MA, Control) as the between-subject than the MA and control groups (p’s<0.006; see Table 2 factor and the subscales (internalizing, externalizing, and and Fig. 2). Furthermore, the SM and MA groups had hyperactive behaviors) as the dependent measures. Signif- significantly lower nonverbal social skills than the control icant main effects were followed up with analyses of group (p’s<0.006). There were no other significant group covariance (ANCOVA) tests on the dependent variables differences following Bonferroni correction.
  • 7. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 1063 Table 2 Group Differences on Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) and Verbal and Nonverbal Social Behavior Measures as Completed by Primary Caregivers Measure Selective mutism Mixed anxiety Controls f-ratio p Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Social competence measures Social cooperation 11.4 (3.6) 10.6 (3.8) 12.4 (3.0) 1.8 0.175 Social assertion 11.0b (3.5) 12.5a (3.9) 16.1a,b (3.3) 17.8 0.0005 Social responsibility 9.9c,d (3.7) 11.8c (2.9) 13.6d (2.9) 13.5 0.0005 Self-control 11.9 (2.8) 10.9 (3.9) 13.5 (3.8) 3.5 0.034 Total social skills 44.2e (9.1) 45.9f (10.0) 48.3e,f (11.0) 12.3 0.0005 Problem behavior measures Externalizing behaviors 3.7 (2.1) 4.8 (2.9) 3.4 (2.4) 2.0 0.139 Internalizing behaviors 5.6g (2.2) 5.9h (3.1) 2.9g,h (2.4) 11.2 0.0005 Hyperactive behaviors 3.6 (2.5) 5.3 (2.8) 4.1 (2.7) 3.0 0.054 Verbal and nonverbal behavior measures Verbal social skills 10.8i,j (3.2) 13.8i (3.8) 16.1j (4.0) 22.8 0.0005 Nonverbal social skills 15.4k (4.2) 15.7l (4.1) 20.2k,l (4.8) 12.5 0.0005 Nonverbal social cooperation 15.8 (3.5) 13.6 (4.0) 16.3 (4.5) 3.7 0.028 Identical superscripts indicate statistical significance between those groups SSRS-Parent Version: Problem Behaviors SM group had lower verbal social skills than the MA and control groups (p<0.006; see Table 3 and Fig. 2). The SM The MANCOVA indicated a significant main effect for group was also lower in nonverbal social skills than the group on the problem behavior subscales (Wilks’Λ, F(6, control group (p<0.006), and the MA group had lower 202) = 5.088, p=0.002), but only for the internalizing subscale (see Table 2 and Fig. 1). Follow up analyses supported our hypothesis that the SM and MA groups had significantly higher internalizing behaviors than the control group (p<0.02), but they did not differ from each other. SSRS-Teacher Version: Social Competence The MANCOVA indicated a significant multivariate main effect for group on the social skills subscales (Wilks’Λ, F(6, 194) = 9.713, p=0.0005; see Table 3). A one-way ANCOVA also revealed a significant main effect for group on the total social skills scale (F(2, 99) = 9.126, p=0.001). Follow-up tests indicated that the SM and MA groups had significantly lower self-control (p’s<0.006) and total social skills than the control group (p’s<0.02). Teachers also rated children with SM lower in social assertion skills than the MA and control group (p’s<0.006). Additionally, children with MA were considered to be lower in social cooperation skills than the control group (p<0.006). SSRS-Teacher Version: Verbal and Nonverbal Social Skills Note: ** p < .02 for SSRS-Parent Version ** p < .006 for SSRS-Teacher Version The MANCOVA revealed a significant main effect of group Fig. 1 Primary caregiver and teacher report of internalizing behaviors (Wilks’Λ, F(6,196) = 12.406, p=0.0005) on all of the on the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). Note: Internalizing subscales (see Table 3). Follow-up tests showed that the behaviors were z-scored to standardize comparison across informants
  • 8. 1064 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 Table 3 Group Differences on Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) and Verbal and Nonverbal Social Behavior Measures as Completed by Teachers Measure Selective mutism Mixed anxiety Controls f-ratio p Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Social competence measures Social cooperation 14.5 (4.0) 12.9a (5.5) 16.6a (3.8) 4.8 0.010 Social assertion 5.0b,c (3.6) 10.3b (4.7) 11.9c (4.8) 22.5 0.0005 Self-control 12.6d (4.0) 12.7e (5.0) 16.3d,e (2.2) 5.8 0.004 Total social skills 32.0f (8.8) 35.8g (13.1) 44.7f,g (6.1) 9.1 0.0005 Problem behavior measures Externalizing behaviors 0.5h (1.2) 2.9h,i (3.6) 0.6i (1.3) 8.0 0.001 Internalizing behaviors 6.6k (3.2) 4.9j (3.1) 1.7j,k (1.8) 17.5 0.0005 Hyperactive behaviors 2.2 (2.3) 4.3 (4.0) 1.8 (2.4) 5.2 0.007 Verbal and nonverbal behavior measures Verbal social skills 2.0l,m (2.4) 6.9l (3.1) 7.8m (2.2) 33.6 0.0005 Nonverbal classroom cooperation and competence 18.1 (4.9) 16.n (7.1) 21.2n (4.2) 7.0 0.001 Nonverbal social interactions 10.9o (4.8) 12.7 (4.6) 15.7o (2.4) 5.5 0.005 Identical superscripts indicate statistical significance between those groups classroom cooperation and competence skills compared to (Wilks’Λ, F(6,170) = 8.774, p=0.0005; see Table 3). In line the control group (p<0.006). with our predictions, the SM and MA groups had significantly higher internalizing behavior scores than the SSRS-Teacher Version: Problem Behaviors control group (p <.006; see Table 3 and Fig. 1). The SM and control groups had significantly lower externalizing The MANCOVA revealed a significant multivariate main behaviors than the MA group (p’s<0.006). There were no effect for group on all of the problem behavior subscales other group differences following Bonferroni correction. Note: ** p < .006 Note: ** p < .003 Fig. 2 Primary caregiver and teacher report of verbal social skills on the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). Note: Verbal social skills Fig. 3 Primary caregiver report of social anxiety on the Screen for were z-scored to standardize comparison across informants Child Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED)
  • 9. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 1065 SCARED—Parent Version Our findings support and extend recent research (e.g., Cunningham et al. 2004, 2006; Vecchio and Kearney Anxiety Subscales The MANCOVA revealed a significant 2005), suggesting that children with SM appear less multivariate main effect for group (F(10, 284) = 12.5, p= socially competent and more prone to internalizing behav- 0.0005) on all of the subscales (see Table 4). A one-way iors compared to controls as reported by primary caregivers ANCOVA also showed a significant main effect for group and teachers. More specifically, children with SM were on the total anxiety score (F(2, 146) = 28.4, p=0.0005). rated significantly lower than controls on teacher-reported While the SM and MA groups had significantly higher social assertion, self-control and total social skills and on GAD, separation anxiety, and school avoidance scores (all primary caregiver reported social responsibility and total p’s<0.003) as well as higher total anxiety (p<0.02) than the social skills. Furthermore, consistent with findings by control group, the two groups did differ from each other on Cunningham et al. (2006), children with SM were rated levels of social anxiety and panic/somatic symptoms. More by both primary caregivers and teachers lower on verbal specifically, the SM group had higher social anxiety scores social skills compared to both the MA and control groups. than the MA group and the control group and the MA Consistent with our predictions, we also found that primary group had higher social anxiety scores than the control caregivers indicated greater social anxiety levels in children group (see Fig. 3). The MA group also had higher panic/ with SM than the children reported for themselves, which is somatic symptoms compared to the SM and control groups supportive of the idea that children with SM may use their (all p’s<0.003). mutism as a compensatory strategy to reduce their anxiety (Yeganeh et al. 2003, 2006). SCARED—Child Version As expected, given the general agreement in the literature that SM should be better classified as an anxiety disorder (e.g. There were no significant group differences. Anstendig 1999; Sharp et al. 2007; Standart and Le Couteur 2003), children with SM, and children with MA, did not SCARED—Parent and Child Comparisons significantly differ on many of the social skills and anxiety measures regardless of informant. For example, the SM and A paired-samples t-test on the social anxiety subscale of the MA groups did not significantly differ on internalizing SCARED indicated significantly greater report of child behaviors, social cooperation, self-control, total social skills, social anxiety by primary caregivers (M=11.7, SD=2.7) and on teacher reported nonverbal social skills, and class- than self-report of social anxiety by the children (M=7.7, room cooperation and competence. The SM and MA groups SD=3.5) (t(35) = 6.1, p<0.01). additionally did not differ on primary caregiver reported internalizing behaviors, social assertion, self-control, nonver- Discussion bal social skills and total social skills. Also in line with our predictions, children with SM and anxiety disorders were not The goal of the present study was to examine group significantly different on behaviors reflecting GAD, separa- differences among children with SM, mixed anxiety, and tion anxiety, school avoidance, and overall anxiety, although controls on measures of behavioral and socio-emotional primary caregivers rated the SM group significantly higher in functioning. We used a relatively large sample size, multi- behaviors reflecting social anxiety than the MA and informant report (i.e. primary caregiver, teacher, and child control groups. Taken together, these results suggest that self-report) and included an anxious comparison group. deficits in social abilities are evident in both children with Table 4 Group Differences on the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) as Completed by Primary Caregivers Measure Selective mutism Mixed anxiety Controls f-ratio p Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Panic/significant somatic symptoms 3.1a (2.9) 7.7a,b (4.6) 3.7b (3.0) 11.3 0.0005 Generalized anxiety disorder 6.6c (4.9) 8.7d (5.5) 2.6c,d (3.2) 18.1 0.0005 Separation anxiety disorder 5.7e (3.6) 6.5f (4.3) 2.6e,f (3.1) 11.5 0.0005 Social anxiety disorder 11.7g,h (2.7) 7.7g,i (4.6) 3.7h,i (3.0) 47.6 0.0005 School avoidance 1.9j (2.0) 2.4k (2.0) 0.5j,k (1.0) 13.9 0.0005 Total anxiety 28.9l (11.4) 30.2m (16.2) 10.6l,m (9.3) 28.4 0.0005 Identical superscripts indicate statistical significance between those groups
  • 10. 1066 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 SM and children with anxiety disorders. Furthermore, the Chair in Patient-Centred Health Care. The authors would like to thank Lindsay Bennett, Sue McKee, Renee Nossal, Matilda Nowakowski, findings suggest that SM may be considered an anxiety and Jamie Sawyer for their assistance with data collection. We would disorder characterized by social anxiety. More detailed also like to thank the many children, their primary caregivers and observational analyses of the types of social inhibitions teachers for their participation in the study. experienced by children and youth with social phobia but who speak at school versus those with SM and SP versus References those with SM but less social inhibition is warranted. This approach would also aid in the understanding of the role American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical of oral speech in social skill development in children and manual of mental disorders DSM-IV-TR. Washington: Author. may help to refine the classification of SM. Anstendig, K. D. (1999). Is selective mutism an anxiety disorder? Rethinking its DSM-IV classification. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13, 417–434. Limitations Bergman, R. L., Piacentini, J., & McCracken, J. T. (2002). Prevalence and description of selective mutism in a school-based sample. There were at least three limitations of the present study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 938–946. First, although there was no systematic loss of data among Bernstein, G. A., Bernat, D. H., Davis, A. A., & Layne, A. E. (2008). the three groups, several questionnaire packages from the Symptom presentation and classroom functioning in a nonclinical child’s teacher were not returned, which reduced the sample sample of children with social phobia. Depression and Anxiety, size for the teacher analyses and perhaps reliability. Second, 25, 752–760. Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D., Cully, M., Balach, L., the mean substitution method for filling in missing data for Kaufman, J., et al. (1997). The screen for child anxiety related some of our measures has been criticized by some (e.g., emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psycho- Enders 2006). However, given the small percentage of metric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child missing data in this sample the mean substitution method is and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 545–553. Black, B., & Uhde, T. W. (1992). Elective mutism as a variant of not likely to have impacted results in a meaningful way. social phobia. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Third, some research (e.g., Comer and Kendall 2004) Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 1090–1094. suggests that parental psychopathology (e.g. anxiety) may Black, B., & Uhde, T. W. (1995). Psychiatric characteristics of children influence parental ratings of their child’s psychopathology. with selective mutism: A pilot study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 847–856. We were, however, not able to examine whether any of the Brown, J., & Llyod, H. (1975). A controlled study of not speaking in parents met clinical diagnosis for psychopathology. school. Journal of the Association of Workers for Maladjusted Children, 3, 49–63. Conclusion and Implications Clarizio, H. (1994). Assessment of depression in children and adolescents by parents, teachers and peers. In W. Reynolds & H. Johnston (Eds.), Handbook of depression in children and We found that children with selective mutism were rated to adolescents (pp. 235–248). New York: Plenum Press. have less social competence than controls by both primary Cohan, S. L., Chavira, D. A., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Practitioner caregivers and teachers. SM children, however, were not review: Psychosocial interventions for children with selective mutism: A critical evaluation of the literature from 1990–2005. rated significantly different on a number of social skills and Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1085–1097. anxiety measures than children with anxiety disorders. Cohan, S. L., Price, J. M., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Suffering in silence: Importantly, children with SM were rated higher in levels of Why a developmental psychopathology perspective on selective social anxiety compared to their anxious and typically mutism is needed. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 341–355. developing counterparts. Based upon the present findings, it Comer, J. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2004). A symptom-level examination is plausible that SM may be conceptualized as an anxiety of parent-child agreement in the diagnosis of anxious youths. disorder with a primary deficit in social functioning. This Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent suggestion further impacts the formation of future diagnos- Psychiatry, 43, 878–886. Cunningham, C. E., McHolm, A. E., & Boyle, M. H. (2006). Social tic manuals: It may be more appropriate to classify SM as phobia, anxiety, oppositional behavior, social skills, and self- an anxiety disorder, and more specifically a social anxiety concept in children with specific selective mutism, generalized disorder, than the currently ambiguous classification of SM selective mutism, and community controls. European Child & in ‘Other Disorders of Infancy, Childhood, and Adoles- Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 245–255. Cunningham, C. E., McHolm, A., Boyle, M. H., & Patel, S. (2004). cence’. Furthermore, the present findings have implications Behavioral and emotional adjustment, family functioning, aca- for clinical practice, whereby social skills training merits demic performance, and social relationships in children with inclusion in intervention for children with anxiety disorders selective mutism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, as well as children with SM. 45, 1363–1372. Dummit, E. S., Klein, R. G., Tancer, N. K., & Asche, B. (1997). Systematic assessment of 50 children with selective mutism. Acknowledgements This research was supported by a grant from Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent the Ontario Mental Health Foundation (OMHF) and the Jack Laidlaw Psychiatry, 36, 653–660.
  • 11. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2010) 38:1057–1067 1067 Elizur, Y., & Perednik, R. (2003). Prevalence and description of Mclnnes, A., & Manassis, K. (2005). When silence is not golden: An selective mutism in immigrant and native families: A controlled integrated approach to selective mutism. Seminars in Speech and study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Language, 26, 201–210. Psychiatry, 42, 1451–1459. Nowakowski, M. E., Cunningham, C. C., McHolm, A. E., Evans, M. Enders, C. K. (2006). A primer on the use of modern missing-data A., Edison, S., St. Pierre, J., et al. (2009). Language and methods in Psychosomatic medicine research. Psychosomatic academic abilities in children with selective mutism. Infant and Medicine, 68, 427–736. Child Development, 18, 271–290. Ford, M. A., Sladeczek, I. E., Carlson, J., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1998). Remschmidt, H., Poller, M., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Hennighausen, Selective mutism: Phenomenological characteristics. School K., & Gutenbrunner, C. (2001). A follow-up study of 45 patients Psychology Quarterly, 12, 192–227. with elective mutism. European Archives of Psychiatry and Garcia, A. M., Freeman, J. B., Francis, G., Miller, L. M., & Leonard, Clinical Neuroscience, 251, 284–296. H. L. (2004). Selective mutism. New York: Oxford University Schniering, C. A., Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2000). Issues in the Press. diagnosis and assessment of anxiety disorders in children and Giddan, J. J., Ross, G. J., Sechler, L. L., & Becker, B. R. (1997). adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 453–478. Selective mutism in elementary school: Multidisciplinary inter- Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, ventions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children 28, 127–133. version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from Gresham, F. M., & Elliot, S. N. (1990). Social skills rating system. previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Biederman, J., Henin, A., Faraone, S. V., Psychiatry, 39, 28–38. Davis, S., Harrington, K., et al. (2007). Behavioral inhibition in Sharp, W. G., Sherman, C., & Gross, A. M. (2007). Selective mutism preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle and anxiety: A review of the current conceptualization of the childhood social anxiety: A five-year follow-up. Journal of disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 568–579. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28, 225–233. Standart, S., & Le Couteur, A. (2003). The quiet child: A literature Kolvin, I., & Fundudis, T. (1981). Elective mute children: Psycho- review of selective mutism. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, logical development and background factors. Journal of Child 8, 154–160. Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 219–232. Steinhausen, H., & Juzi, C. (1996). Elective mutism: An analysis of Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (1997). Selective mutism: A population- 100 cases. Journal of the American Academy of Child and based study: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 606–614. Psychiatry, 38, 257–262. Vecchio, J. L., & Kearney, C. A. (2005). Selective mutism in children: Kristensen, H. (2000). Selective mutism and comorbidity with Comparison to youths with and without anxiety disorders. Journal developmental disorder/delay, anxiety disorder, and elimination of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 31–37. disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Voncken, M. J., Alden, L. E., Bögels, S. M., & Roelofs, J. (2008). Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 249–256. Social rejection in social anxiety disorder: The role of perfor- Kristensen, H., & Torgersen, S. (2002). A case-control study of EAS mance deficits, evoked negative emotions and dissimilarity. child and parental temperaments in selectively mute children with British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 439–450. and without a co-morbid communication disorder. Nordic Yeganeh, R., Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2006). Selective mutism: Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 347–353. More than social anxiety? Depression and Anxiety, 23, 117–123. Kumpulainen, K. (2002). Phenomenology and treatment of selective Yeganeh, R., Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Pina, A. A., & Silverman, mutism. CNS Drugs, 16, 175–180. W. K. (2003). Clinical distinctions between selective mutism and Kumpulainen, K., Räsänen, E., Raaska, H., & Somppi, V. (1998). social phobia: An investigation of childhood psychopathology. Selective mutism among second-graders in elementary school. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 24–29. Psychiatry, 42, 1069–1075.