SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 681
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY
For the Case Study assignment the current pathogen selections
may be requested by sending
an email to your instructor!
Assigned Case Study Problem:
You will create a case study for a microbial infection selected
from the current pathogen list. Your case
study will be assembled using a detailed rubric (see below).
Upon completion, you will submit your
case study to the Blackboard gradebook in Unit 5 and to
SafeAssign.
How to create a case study
The case studies are meant to be an enjoyable, interesting, and
informative assignment. This is your
chance to show that you understand the key teaching points
about a microbe and to communicate
these points in a written format.
What information belongs in my case study?
Have at least 3-4 key referenced points in each of the five areas
shown in the Case Study Information
Chart (see below). The left-hand heading in the chart suggests
the type of information requested for the
pathogen. Outlines can be in whatever form you prefer
(bullets/charts/outlines/diagrams or a mix). Be
sure to include two discussion questions (and provide complete
answers) that you can incorporate
into your case study (place them at the end of your write-up).
These questions should help connect your
case to other material in the course. For example, what other
microbes have an A-B toxin? What other
viruses are transmitted by fecal-oral spread?
How much information should I provide for my case study?
For the Case Study, you are asked to provide at least the
information requested in the chart below. The
boxed questions are suggestions for the minimum amount of
information within each category. The
more detailed the information, the better the study. You may
consult your textbook, CDC, WHO, Access
Medicine, Google Scholar, NCBI, WebMD, etc. to find the
information. For example, if you perform a
Google search using the name of the pathogen and the word
‘vaccine’, you will find information on
current vaccines (if any), those in clinical trials, vaccines used
only in animals, etc.
Case Study Information Chart
Typical Case What does a typical case look like? Use the
standard format for a
patient presentation with chief complaint (CC), history of
present illness
(HPI), key physical exam details (PE), lab findings, signature
signs, and
any other important findings.
Description of the infectious
agent
If it is a bacterium, how is it classified? If it is a virus, what
kind of
nucleic acid does it have? Does it target specific cellular types
(tropism)? Does it form a spore? Is it aerobic? Is it
intracellular? Can it
only be grown in a specific type of media? How is it
distinguished from
other members of the species? Does the pathogen have a
significant
history with humans or animals?
Epidemiology What do you feel are the most important points
about the
epidemiology of the disease? Incidence? Portal of entry?
Source? Is it a
normal microbiota component in the human body? Does it only
occur
in certain populations or certain geographical areas? Is there a
vector
involved? What and who is the vector? Is it zoonotic? Does it
appear
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY
seasonally? Are there currently any outbreaks of this pathogen?
What
is its ecological niche? Is there a reservoir? Are there currently
any
outbreaks or epidemics of disease from this pathogen?
Pathogenesis What is the range of diseases caused by the agent?
What organs are
affected? What symptoms might the patient have? What is the
disease
course? Will the patient recover? Are there any long-term
sequelae of
infection? Latency?
Prophylaxis/Treatment Is there an antitoxin? Specific
antibiotics or a class of antibiotics that
are used? Is there a vaccine available? Is treatment curative?
Does
infection make you immune? Is this immunity life-long? Is there
drug
resistance? Are there novel treatments?
Discussion Questions With
Responses
Two (2) discussion questions with responses and scientific
literature
references are required to complete the Case Study; These
questions
should help connect your case to other material in the course.
For
example, what other microbes have an A-B toxin? What other
viruses
are transmitted by fecal-oral spread? Is this pathogen a
candidate for
use as a biological weapon?
How should I format my references for my case study?
You will need references for all factual information in your case
study. Pertinent references can be listed
at the bottom of your outline in a small font. Your references
must include at least two current peer-
reviewed publications from the scientific literature post-2010.
Use APA Sixth Edition Style format for all
references (a PDF document is provided with the case study
directions).
For example, your text book in-text citation would look like this
(Anderson, Salm, & Allen, 2016) and
your textbook would be shown on your reference page as:
Anderson, D., Salm, S., & Allen, D. (2016). Microbiology: A
Human Perspective. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Addenda
1. The written case presentations are submitted through
Blackboard in Unit 5 of the course. When
you reach Unit 5, be sure to e-mail your instructor to receive a
list of the Case Study selections.
2. Please name the files in the following manner
“LastnameFirstinitial CS Y”. For example, if I am
submitting a Case Study on tularemia, I would name the case
study file “Frisardi M CS tularemia”. This
will ensure that you receive appropriate credit and that I will be
able to find your Case Study
document easily when grading.
Be sure to provide the two (2) required discussion questions (as
well as provide answers) with
your case study.
3. Your library privileges from UNE give you access to all of
the scientific literature so you are able to
find references, abstracts, and complete articles in most cases.
Your library privileges from UNE also give you access to the
medical textbooks. Do this:
http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources
Resources (the page will be here:
http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources/databases-title )
http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources
http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources/databases-title
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY
it.
-in page and you may use your UNE e-mail
log-in information.
bar in the
middle of the page. Type in your
Case Study pathogen and get started.
4. Finally, use Google Scholar. Make a noun string of your
pathogen’s Latin name and a qualifying term
like “vaccines” or “virulence factors”. The site is:
http://scholar.google.com/
Be sure to consider the directions carefully and include all the
requested information (especially the two
discussion questions with responses and your scientific
literature references).
By the time you provide all information for your pathogen
(including any figures, illustrations, maps) the
case study will be several pages in length. Use APA format (or
any format you are used to), in-text
citations, and a reference list at the end.
So:
• Read the rubric carefully, provide all information, discussion
questions/responses, and
references in APA format.
• Submit to SafeAssign to check that you have referenced
everything and avoid any
inadvertent plagiarism, then
Remember that the Case Study is a teaching tool and you are,
essentially, teaching me about
the pathogen you selected.
http://scholar.google.com/
Running Head: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators
Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators
Title: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators
Student’s Name:
Institution:
Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions
of Teachers and Administrators
According to the chapters on PAS, the conceptual framework
has been effectively presented with the main points analyzed in
an effective manner. The conceptual framework of the chapters
on PAS has analyzed the challenges experienced in the learning
institutions and the manner in which the challenges should be
addressed to bring about the required success in the schools.
However, the conceptual framework has given me a chance to
identify evaluation of the challenges as a model to address the
problems experienced. This model will make it easy for me to
understand the problems experienced hence coming up with a
strategy in which problems are addressed in an effective
manner.
The working title of my PAS is the challenges of charter
schools and the public schools: perceptions of Teachers and the
administrators. This is because different challenges are
encountered when running the charter schools as well as the
public schools in a country. For the purposes of ensuring that
both the charter schools and the public schools do not
experience the problems, different strategies should be put in
place by the government o protect the learning institutions from
the challenges experienced in their efforts to teach the young
people in the society.
The teachers and the administrators in the learning institutions
should change their perceptions and hence develop mechanisms
in which the challenges are overcome. However, the challenges
should be addressed in a way that does not affect the learning of
the students in both the charter schools and the public schools.
The challenges experienced by the teacher, administrators and
the learners in the learning institutions should be analyzed and
hence the right solutions identified for the purposes of making
the learning and the teaching process effective.
The administrators in both the charter schools and the public
schools should use their ability in addressing the challenges
experienced by the teachers and the learners. This is because
they have the authority to carry out different activities for the
sake of bringing about the success of the charter schools and the
public schools. Moreover, consultations should be carried so as
to ensure that the charter schools and the public schools do not
experience the challenges as they might bring about the
unnecessary failures n place especially when not effectively
controlled. (Dunn, 2018)
The stakeholders in charter schools and the public schools
should be consulted before different solutions are achieved as
they might give out ideas which might help in the solving and
addressing the challenges in an effective manner. Moreover, it
is the responsibility of the stakeholders to support the charter
schools and the public schools so as to ensure that the learners
receive a quality education. (Cookson, 2018) As a result, both
material support and psychological support is required to enable
the administrators and teachers to carry out different activities
for the success of the learning institution. The support provided
by the stakeholders will help in addressing some of the
challenges hence enabling the administrators to come up with
different projects which will make the learning process easy.
Finally, the challenges experienced should be addressed in time
before they affect the learning process of the students.
(Maranto, 2018)
As far as the research questions are concerned, some of the
research questions identified are:
· What should be done to reduce the challenges experienced in
both charter schools and the public schools?
· What are the roles of the teachers and the administrators in
addressing of the challenges experienced in the learning
institutions?
· How have the perceptions of the teachers and the
administrators changed as a result of the challenges experienced
in both the charter schools and the public schools?
· What efforts have been carried so far to have the problems
addressed?
References
Cookson, P. (2018). Expect miracles: Charter schools and the
politics of hope and despair. Routledge.
Dunn, M. E., Katsiyannis, A., & Ryan, J. B. (2018). Charter
schools and students with disabilities: legal and practical
considerations. Intervention in School and Clinic, 53(4), 252-
255.
Maranto, R. (2018). The death of one best way: Charter schools
as reinventing government. In School choice in the real world
(pp. 39-57). Routledge.
Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions
of Teachers and Administrators
Name of student
Institution
Date
Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions
of Teachers and Administrators
The topic that I will be handling in this study is “Challenges of
Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers
and Administrators.” This therefore, is the working title of my
Professional Administrative study (PAS). To pursue this topic
in detail, I believe that the application of the qualitative
approach will be key in ensuring that the study obtains more
information from the teachers and administrators who will be
the respondents. I believe that application of a theory will also
be key in establishing relationships between various variables in
the study design. The reason why a study has to rely on a theory
is because “theories and conceptual frameworks are developed
to account for or describe abstract phenomena that occur under
similar conditions” (Rudestam & Newton, 2015). We expect that
the responses that are given by the participants and which make
a key part of the findings of the study should be reproducible
under similar conditions.
The problem statement in this study is that there are many
challenges that charter schools and public schools face and the
perceptions of teachers and administrators are key in reaching a
solution. According to Rudestam & Newton, 2015, a
professional scientific study should be based on a method of
inquiry that a community of scholars have tested and conformed
to be reliable and valid. It is in this connection that I chose to
use the qualitative approach because its scientifically proven.
Another advantage of using this approach lies in the fact that I
will be able to get detailed responses which I believe will be in
a descriptive manner.
I would also rely on an exploratory model to ensure that I have
the best methods for data collection with an aim of minimising
the possibility of bias. According to Rudestam & Newton 2015,
“The research question is the precisely stated form of the
researcher’s intent and may be accompanied by one or more
specific hypotheses”. Looking at the study topic in question, it
is important to consider the use of short interviews and factor
the necessity of an exploratory element with theoretical
implications. The research questions that will govern the study
include the following;
i. What are the three major challenges that charter and public
schools face?
ii. In the view of teachers which are the three greatest
challenges facing charter and public schools?
iii. In the view of administrators which are the three greatest
challenges facing charter and public schools?
iv. What should be done to rectify the situation?
References
Rudestam, K. E., & Newton, R. R. (2015). Surviving your
dissertation: A comprehensive
guide to content and process. Sage Publications.
This is a 4 page paper. The PAS Prospectus is a preliminary
document that serves as the foundation for the proposal
document for the PAS. It is a relatively short document and
should not exceed approximately 6 pages of text, excluding
appendices and references.
This paper is a qualitative study using a case study, using a
survey questionnaireNOT interviews or observations.
· Title Page(The qualitative study of Charter Schools vs.
Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators)
· Problem Statement(What is the problem that needs to be
solved from the title page)
· Purpose Statement(What am I going to solve the problem?
How am I going to identify it from the problem statement)?
· Nature of the Study
· Research Question(s)(Must align with the problem
statement and purpose statement)
· Conceptual Framework
· Significance of the Study
· References(Need to be included in the body of the paper
and in the reference section as well)
1
Surviving Your Dissertation
4
2
Surviving Your Dissertation
A Comprehensive Guide to Content and Process
4
Kjell Erik Rudestam
Fielding Graduate University
Rae R. Newton
Fielding Graduate University
3
FOR INFORMATION:
SAGE Publications, Inc.
2455 Teller Road
Thousand Oaks, California 91320
E-mail: [email protected]
SAGE Publications Ltd.
1 Oliver’s Yard
55 City Road
London EC1Y 1SP
United Kingdom
SAGE Publications India Pvt. Ltd.
B 1/I 1 Mohan Cooperative Industrial Area
Mathura Road, New Delhi 110 044
India
SAGE Publications Asia-Pacific Pte. Ltd.
3 Church Street
#10-04 Samsung Hub
Singapore 049483
4
Copyright © 2015 by SAGE Publications, Inc.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or
utilized in
any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including
photocopying, recording, or by any information storage and
retrieval
system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Rudestam, Kjell Erik.
Surviving your dissertation : a comprehensive guide to content
and process / Kjell Erik
Rudestam, Fielding Graduate University, Rae R. Newton,
Fielding Graduate University.
—Fourth edition.
pages cm.
Includes bibliographical references.
ISBN 978-1-4522-6097-6 (pbk. : alk. paper)
1. Dissertations, Academic—United States. 2. Report writing. 3.
Research—United
States. I. Newton, Rae R. II. Title.
LB2369.R83 2015
378.2—dc23 2014002905
This book is printed on acid-free paper.
14 15 16 17 18 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Acquisitions Editor: Vicki Knight
Editorial Assistant: Yvonne McDuffee
Production Editors: Laura Barrett, David C. Felts
Copy Editor: Paula L. Fleming
Typesetter: C&M Digitals (P) Ltd.
Proofreader: Sarah J. Duffy
Indexer: Joan Shapiro
5
Cover Designer: Rose Storey
Marketing Manager: Nicole Elliott
6
Contents
Preface
About the Authors
Part I: Getting Started
1. The Research Process
2. Selecting a Suitable Topic
3. Methods of Inquiry: Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches
Part II: Working With Content: The Dissertation Chapters
4. Literature Review and Statement of the Problem
5. The Method Chapter: Describing Your Research Plan
6. Presenting the Results of Quantitative Research
7. Presenting the Results of Qualitative Research
8. Discussion
Part III: Working With Process: What You Need to Know to
Make the Dissertation Easier
9. Overcoming Barriers: Becoming an Expert While Controlling
Your Own Destiny
10. Writing
11. How to Complete Your Dissertation Using Online Data
Access and Collection
12. Guidelines for the Presentation of Numbers in the
Dissertation
13. Informed Consent and Other Ethical Concerns
References
Name Index
Subject Index
7
Preface
We are pleased to present the fourth edition of Surviving Your
Dissertation. As with previous editions, we have sought to
answer
questions that students and faculty have at every stage of the
dissertation
process. In past editions, we have illustrated the challenge of
engaging in
such a notable project with book covers that depict a bridge
leading into an
impenetrable jungle and life buoys close at hand to negotiate
the stormy
seas. The cover of this edition offers a different, but equally
relevant
image: the pride of victory achieved by scaling a lofty peak.
In many ways, these images also reflect our own experience in
writing the
book. The field of research in the social and behavioral sciences
has
expanded rapidly over the past several years, and we have
frequently felt
as if we are scrambling to keep up. This edition reflects our
experience.
We have maintained the overall structure of the book, which has
been well
received so far, while updating content on those topics that are
indispensable to the dissertation process: the selection of an
appropriate
research topic; the review of the literature; the description of
the
methodology and research design; the collection and analysis of
data; and
the interpretation, presentation, and discussion of the results
and
implications of the study. Within this updated material, we have
once
again attempted to provide sufficient detail to enable the reader
to know
exactly what goes into each section and chapter of the
dissertation and how
to format that information. In addition, we continue to include
topics that
are not always present in sources of this kind: the many types of
quantitative and qualitative research models and approaches that
are
available to the student, the principles of good scholarly and
academic
writing, suggestions for how to select and work with
committees, and tips
for overcoming task and emotional blocks that may impede
progress.
Throughout, we have replaced older references with newer,
more
contemporary ones, including many new dissertation examples
taken from
our students and colleagues.
We have also added significant new content to the fourth
edition. We note
that the traditional null hypothesis significance-testing model is
being
challenged and augmented by an emphasis on clinical or
practical
significance and a corresponding use of measures of effect size
and
8
confidence intervals. We have explained and illustrated this new
approach
to the presentation of statistical results. Similarly, we have
acknowledged
an emerging emphasis on theoretical “models” and the influence
of model
building on the design and presentation of research. We provide
several
examples of studies that incorporate this approach. We are
increasingly
impressed by the implications of the Internet for the entire
research
enterprise. We have expanded our discussion of the Internet as a
source of
data, an opportunity for data collection, and a vehicle for data
analysis, as
well as providing recommendations of potentially helpful
websites and
software programs that may be unfamiliar to the average reader.
We also
acknowledge the parallel expansion of available data sources in
all their
varied forms, including data archives, social media, and what is
currently
known as “big data.” Finally, we have expanded the sections on
qualitative
and multimodal methods of research, which have an inductive,
theory-
building focus. With respect to all these topics, we have tried to
explain
the concepts, illustrate them with new tables and figures, and,
in many
cases, provide very specific details about how to incorporate
them into a
research study.
We believe that this book is suitable for a large academic and
professional
audience. Of course, it is primarily directed at the graduate
student who
envisions or is involved in writing a research dissertation. Thus,
there is
significant focus on material that is best suited for the
beginning doctoral
student—for example, how to develop a research question, how
to
construct a table or figure, how to report a statistical finding,
how to use
American Psychological Association formatting conventions,
and so on.
However, there is also content directed at the more advanced
student—for
example, how to conceptualize and illustrate a mediation model,
how to
report multiple regression findings, and how to code text for a
grounded
theory study. Moreover, we have become increasingly aware
that
Surviving Your Dissertation serves also as a resource for
researchers and
practitioners who have either forgotten important details or are
motivated
to keep abreast of evolving research practices in their fields.
Perhaps more
important, we view the book as a convenient source of
information for
faculty who are currently supervising graduate students’
dissertations or
research projects.
We remain deeply indebted to our own students, who continue
to thrill us
with their creativity and force us to keep learning in order to
stay a step
ahead. We hope that they, and you, find this volume a helpful
and steady
9
companion in your research and writing endeavors.
A large number of individuals have contributed to the
completion of this
project. We called upon many faculty colleagues to nominate
student
dissertations that exemplify high levels of scholarship and have
sprinkled
references to these dissertations throughout the book to
illustrate important
principles and recommendations. We are very appreciative of
these
relatively recent graduates for allowing us to share their first
major
research endeavors in this venue. We also benefitted from the
critical
reflections and insight of the following reviewers of the third
edition of
Surviving Your Dissertation: Anne J. Hacker, Bernie Kerr,
Karin Klenke,
Kaye Pepper, and Udaya R. Wagle. Their observations and
suggestions
were both reinforcing and helpful in crystallizing changes for
this edition.
We are also grateful to be part of the SAGE family, a
collaborative,
dedicated group of professionals who have facilitated our
writing careers
in so many ways. The following individuals were notable
contributors:
Vicki Knight, publisher and senior editor, has always provided
us with a
balance of inspiring leadership and nurturing support. Her
editorial
assistant, Jessica Miller, has been consistently responsive to our
frequent
requests for assistance. Laura Barrett and David Felts, project
editors, have
gracefully guided the editorial process from start to finish. And
Paula
Fleming, our copy editor, is truly a paragon in her craft. Her
grammatical
acuity, common sense, and work ethic cannot be overestimated.
Thank you
all.
Finally, we must thank our partners in life, Jan and Kathy, for
their
continuing patience and support as we have devoted our energy
and
attention to four editions of this volume.
10
About the Authors
Kjell Erik Rudestam
is Professor of Psychology at Fielding Graduate University,
Santa
Barbara, California, where he served as Associate Dean of
Academic
Affairs for many years. He was previously a psychology
professor at
York University, Toronto, and Miami University, Oxford, Ohio,
after
receiving his PhD in Psychology (Clinical) from the University
of
Oregon. He is the author of Your Statistical Consultant:
Answers to
Your Data Analysis Questions, 2nd edition (also with Rae R.
Newton), Handbook of Online Learning, 2nd edition (with
Judith
Schoenholtz-Read), and eight other books, as well as numerous
articles in professional journals on topics including suicide,
psychotherapy, and family and organizational systems. He is a
Fellow
of the American Psychological Association (Division 12), a
Diplomate of the American Board of Examiners in Professional
Psychology (Clinical), a Diplomate of the American Academy
of
Experts in Traumatic Stress, and holds an Honorary Doctorate
of
Science from The Professional School of Psychology.
Rae R. Newton
is Professor of Sociology Emeritus at California State
University,
Fullerton. He recently joined the faculty of the School of
Psychology
at Fielding Graduate University where he serves as a research
consultant and statistical advisor to doctoral students and
faculty. He
received his PhD in sociology from the University of California,
Santa Barbara, and completed postdoctoral training in mental
health
measurement at Indiana University. His primary interests
include
longitudinal modeling of outcomes for high risk youth and
foster care
populations, family violence and statistics education. He is
author,
with Kjell Erik Rudestam, of Your Statistical Consultant:
Answers to
Your Data Analysis Questions, now in its second edition and
numerous articles in professional journals on topics including
family
violence, child maltreatment, and measurement. In semi-
retirement he
enjoys traveling with his wife in their RV and surfing
throughout
Mexico and Central America.
11
Part I Getting Started
The Research Process
Selecting a Suitable Topic
Methods of Inquiry: Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches
12
1 The Research Process
There is a story about a Zen Buddhist who took a group of
monks into the
forest. The group soon lost its way. Presently one of the monks
asked the
leader where they were going. The wise man answered, “To the
deepest,
darkest part of the forest so that we can all find our way out
together.”
Doctoral research for the graduate student in the social sciences
is often
just such an experience—trekking into a forest of impenetrable
density and
making many wrong turns. Over the years, our students have
used various
metaphors to describe the dissertation process, metaphors that
convey the
feeling of being lost in the wilderness. One student compared
the process
to the Sisyphean struggle of reaching the top of a hill, only to
discover the
presence of an even higher mountain behind it. Another student
experienced the task as learning a Martian language, known to
the natives
who composed her committee but entirely foreign to her. A
third student
offered perhaps the best description when she suggested that it
was like
waiting patiently in a seemingly interminable line to gain
admission to a
desirable event, then finally reaching the front only to be told to
return to
the rear of the line.
One reason that students become more exasperated than
necessary on the
dissertation journey is that they fail to understand the
procedures and
practices that form the foundation for contemporary social
science
research. Many students who are attracted to their field of
interest out of an
applied concern are apprehensive about making the leap from
application
to theory, an indispensable part of the research enterprise. What
may not
be so evident is that many of the skills that go into being a
consummate
practitioner are the same ones demanded of a capable
researcher. It is well
known that curiosity and hypothesis testing are the bedrock of
empirical
research. In a similar fashion, experienced psychotherapists, to
take an
example from clinical psychology, are sensitive and keen
observers of
client behavior. They are persistent hypothesis testers. They are
curious
about the relationship between family history variables and
current
functioning. They draw on theory and experience to help select
a particular
intervention for a particular client problem or moment in
therapy.
Dispassionate logic and clear and organized thinking are as
necessary for
effectiveness in the field as they are for success in research. In
fact, the
13
bridge between research and just plain living is much shorter
than most
people think. All of us gather data about the world around us,
wonder what
will happen if we or others behave in particular ways, and test
our pet
hunches through deliberate action. To a large extent, the formal
research
enterprise consists of thinking systematically about these same
issues.
The procedures outlined in this book are intended to assist the
doctoral
student in planning and writing a research dissertation, but the
suggestions
are equally applicable to writing a master’s thesis. In fact, there
is
considerable overlap between these two challenging activities.
For most
students, the master’s thesis is the first rigorous research
project they
attempt. This means that, in the absence of strong, supportive
faculty
consultation, the student often concludes the thesis with
considerable relief
and an awareness of how not to do the study the next time! With
a doctoral
dissertation, it is generally expected, sometimes as an act of
faith, that the
student is a more seasoned and sophisticated researcher. The
consensus
opinion is that dissertations are generally longer than theses,
that they are
more original, that they rely more heavily on theoretically based
arguments, and that they make a greater contribution to the
field.
In most graduate programs, the prelude to conducting a
dissertation study
is presenting a dissertation proposal. A research proposal is an
action plan
that justifies and describes the proposed study. The completion
of a
comprehensive proposal is a very important step in the
dissertation
process. The proposal serves as a contract between the student
and his or
her dissertation or thesis committee that, when approved by all
parties,
constitutes an agreement that data may be collected and the
study may be
completed. As long as the student follows the steps outlined in
the
proposal, committee members should be discouraged from
demanding
significant changes to the study after the proposal has been
approved.
Naturally, it is not uncommon to expect small changes,
additions, or
deletions as the study progresses because one can never totally
envision
the unpredictable turns that research can take.
There is no universally agreed-on format for the research
proposal. To our
way of thinking, a good proposal contains a review of the
relevant
literature, a statement of the problem and the associated
hypotheses, and a
clear delineation of the proposed method and plan for data
analysis. In our
experience, an approved proposal means that a significant
percentage of
the work on the dissertation has been completed. As such, this
book is
14
intended to help students construct research proposals as well as
complete
dissertations.
The Research Wheel
One way to think about the phases of the research process is
with reference
to the so-called research wheel (see Figure 1.1). The wheel
metaphor
suggests that research is not linear but is rather a recursive
cycle of steps.
The most common entry point is some form of empirical
observation. In
other words, the researcher selects a topic from the infinite
array of
possible topics. The next step is a process of inductive logic
that
culminates in a proposition. The inductive process serves to
relate the
specific topic to a broader context and begins with some
hunches in the
form “I wonder if . . . .” These hunches typically are guided by
the values,
assumptions, and goals of the researcher, which need to be
explicated.
Figure 1.1 The Research Wheel
Stage 2 of the research wheel is a developed proposition, which
is
expressed as a statement of an established relationship (e.g.,
“the early bird
is more likely than the late bird to catch the worm”). The
proposition exists
within a conceptual or theoretical framework. The role of the
researcher is
to clarify the relationship between a particular proposition and
the broader
context of theory and previous research. This is probably the
most
challenging and creative aspect of the dissertation process.
Theories and conceptual frameworks are developed to account
for or
describe abstract phenomena that occur under similar
conditions. A theory
15
is the language that allows researchers to move from
observation to
observation and make sense of similarities and differences. A
conceptual
framework, which is simply a less-developed form of a theory,
consists of
statements that link abstract concepts (e.g., motivation, role) to
empirical
data. If not placed within such a context, the proposed study has
a “So
what?” quality. This is one of the main objections to the
research proposals
of novice researchers: The research question may be inherently
interesting
but ultimately meaningless. For instance, the question “Are
there more
women than men in graduate school today?” is entirely banal as
a research
question unless the answer has conceptual or theoretical
implications that
are developed within the study.
Although a study may be worthwhile primarily for its practical
implications (e.g., “Should we start recruiting more men into
graduate
schools?”), a purely applied study may not be acceptable as a
dissertation.
Kerlinger and Lee (1999), authors of a highly respected text on
research
methodology, noted that “the basic purpose of scientific
research is
theory” (p. 5). Generally speaking, a research dissertation is
expected to
contribute to the scholarly literature in a field and not merely
solve an
applied problem. Thus, developing a proposition for one’s
dissertation
typically involves immersing oneself in the research and
theoretical
literature of the field to identify a conceptual framework for the
study.
Having stated our position on the role of theory in dissertation
research,
we now need to take a step back. As a psychologist and a
sociologist,
respectively, we are most familiar with research conventions
within these
two disciplines. Other branches of the social sciences have their
own
standards of what constitutes an acceptable dissertation topic.
We have
attempted to keep this book as generalizable as possible and to
infuse it
with examples from other fields. Ultimately, of course, you will
need to
follow the rules and conventions that pertain to your discipline
as well as
to your university and department.
For example, a few major universities allow a doctoral student
to submit
one or more published articles as the equivalent of a
dissertation. Many
others encourage studies that consist of secondary data analyses
derived
from national databases, such as U.S. Census data or the
General Social
Survey, or data obtained from a larger study. Some fields—
notably social
work, education, policy evaluation, and professional
psychology—may
encourage dissertations that solve applied problems rather than
make
16
distinct theoretical contributions. Studies that evaluate the
effectiveness of
programs or interventions are a case in point because they
sometimes
contribute little to validating a theory. Political science and
economics are
examples of fields that are diverse enough to accommodate both
theoretically based studies and purely applied studies. Within
the
subspecialty of international relations, for instance, one could
imagine a
survey and analysis of security agreements of European nations
after the
unraveling of the North Atlantic Treaty Organization (NATO)
that rely on
interviews with foreign policy makers and are largely
descriptive and
applied. In contrast, a study of the role of a commitment to
ideology in the
success of political parties in the United States, based on an
analysis of
historical documents and voting records, might be grounded in a
theory of
how ideology attracts or alienates the voting public.
Moving forward along the research wheel, the researcher uses
deductive
reasoning to move from the larger context of theory to generate
a specific
research question. The research question is the precisely stated
form of the
researcher’s intent and may be accompanied by one or more
specific
hypotheses. The first loop is completed as the researcher seeks
to discover
or collect the data that will serve to answer the research
question. The data
collection process is essentially another task of empirical
observation,
which then initiates another round of the research wheel.
Generalizations
are made on the basis of the particular data that have been
observed
(inductive process), and the generalizations are tied to a
conceptual
framework, which then leads to the elucidation of further
research
questions and implications for additional study.
The kinds of skills called for at the various points of the
research wheel are
reminiscent of the thoughts about learning presented by
Bertrand Russell
many years ago. Russell noted that there are two primary kinds
of
knowledge acquisition: knowledge by description and
knowledge by
acquaintance. Knowledge by description is learning in a passive
mode,
such as by reading a book on how to change the oil in one’s car
or hearing
a lecture on Adam Smith’s theory of economics. This type of
learning is
especially well suited to mastering abstract information; in
other words, it
is better for learning about economics than about changing the
oil.
Knowledge by acquaintance, on the other hand, is learning by
doing—the
kind of skill training that comes from practicing a tennis serve,
driving an
automobile, and playing with a computer. This is concrete
knowledge
acquisition, oriented to solving problems.
17
The research process demands both skills. First, the researcher
needs to
apply clear, logical thinking to working with concepts and ideas
and
building theories. It is our impression that many graduate
students,
particularly those who have experience as practitioners in their
fields, are
weaker in this abstract conceptualization, and honing this skill
may be the
major challenge of the dissertation. Second, the researcher must
engage in
the practical application of ideas, including by systematically
planning a
study and then collecting and analyzing data. The ability to
focus, problem
solve, and make decisions will help bring the study to
completion.
18
2 Selecting a Suitable Topic
The selection of an appropriate topic is the first major challenge
in
conducting research. In many academic settings, this task is
simplified by
working with a faculty mentor who is already familiar with an
interesting
area of study, may have an extensive program of research in
that area, and
may even have defined one or more researchable questions. It is
quite
common for students interested in a particular area of research
to not only
select their doctoral institution but also select their dissertation
chair, with
the goal of joining the research program of a noted scholar in
that field.
On the other hand, you may not be blessed with a faculty role
model who
is actively engaged in research in an area of interest to you.
There are no
simple rules for selecting a topic of interest, but there are some
considerations with respect to appropriateness. It is generally
unwise to
define something as important as a dissertation topic without
first
obtaining a broad familiarity with the field. This implies a large
amount of
exploring the literature and studying the experts. Without this
initial
exploration, you can neither know the range of possibilities of
interesting
topics nor have a clear idea of what is already known. Most
students obtain
their research topics from the loose ends they discover in
reading within an
area, from an interesting observation they have made (“I notice
that men
shut up when a beautiful woman enters the room; I wonder what
the effect
of physical attractiveness is on group process?”), or from an
applied focus
in their lives or professional work (“I have a difficult time
treating these
alcoholics and want to discover how best to work with them”).
In short,
there is no substitute for immersing oneself in a field of study
by having
conversations with leading scholars, advisers, and peers;
critically reading
the existing literature; and reflecting upon the implications of
professional
and personal experiences.
Some Guidelines for Topic Selection
Here are some guidelines for deciding whether a topic is
appropriate as a
dissertation subject.
1. A topic needs to sustain your interest over a long period of
time. A
19
study on learning nonsense syllables under two sets of
environmental
conditions may sound appealing in its simplicity, but remember
Finagle’s
first law of research: If something can go wrong, it will go
wrong!
Dissertations usually take at least twice as much time as
anticipated, and
there are few worse fates than slaving for hour after hour on a
project that
you abhor. Remember, too, that all dissertations are recorded
and
published by the Library of Congress, and you will always be
associated
with your particular study.
2. At the other extreme, it is wise to avoid a topic that is overly
ambitious
and overly challenging. Most students want to graduate,
preferably within
a reasonable period of time. Grandiose dissertations have a way
of never
being completed, and even the best dissertations end up being
compromises among your own ambition, the wishes of your
committee,
and practical circumstances. It is not realistic for a dissertation
to say
everything there is to say about a particular topic (e.g., the
European
Union), and you need to temper your enthusiasm with
pragmatism. As one
student put it, “There are two types of dissertations: the great
ones and
those that are completed!” Sometimes it makes sense to select a
research
topic on the basis of convenience or workability and use the
luxury of the
postgraduate years to pursue more esoteric topics of personal
interest.
3. We suggest that you avoid topics that may be linked too
closely with
emotional issues in your own life. It always makes sense to
choose a topic
that is interesting and personally meaningful. Some students,
however, try
to use a dissertation to resolve an emotional issue or solve a
personal
problem. For example, even if you think you have successfully
overcome
the personal impact of the death of your child, this is a topic to
be avoided.
It will necessarily stir up emotional issues that may get in the
way of
completing the dissertation.
4. A related issue is selecting a topic in which you have a
personal ax to
grind. Remember that conducting research demands ruthless
honesty and
objectivity. If you initiate a study to demonstrate that men are
no damned
good, you will be able neither to allow yourself the sober
reflections of
good research nor to acknowledge the possibility that your
conclusions
may contradict your expectations. It is much better to begin
with a hunch
(“I’ve noticed that men don’t do very well with housekeeping; I
wonder if
that has something to do with being pampered as children”) and
to regard
the research as an adventurous exploration to shed light on this
topic rather
20
than as a polemical exercise to substantiate your point of view.
5. Finally, you need to select a topic that has the potential to
make an
original contribution to the field and allow you to demonstrate
your
independent mastery of subject and method. In other words, the
topic must
be worth pursuing. At the very least, the study must generate or
help
validate theoretical understanding in an area or, in those fields
where
applied dissertations are permissible, contribute to the
development of
professional practice. Some students are put off when they
discover that a
literature review contains contradictory or puzzling results or
explanations
for a phenomenon. However, such contradictions should be
taken not as
reasons to steer away from a topic but rather as opportunities to
resolve a
mystery. When people disagree or when existing explanations
seem
inadequate, there is often room for a critical study to be
conducted. An
opportunity to design a study that resolves theoretical
contradictions
within a discipline should not be overlooked.
From Interesting Idea to Research Question
Let us assume that you have identified a general area of
research and that
your choice is based on curiosity and may involve resolving a
problem,
explaining a phenomenon, uncovering a process by which
something
occurs, demonstrating the truth of a hidden fact, building on or
reevaluating other studies, or testing some theory in your field.
To know
whether or not the topic is important (significant), you must be
familiar
with the literature in the area. In Chapter 4, we present a
number of
suggestions for conducting a good review and assessment of the
literature.
In the meantime, we have noticed that many students have
difficulty
transforming an interesting idea into a researchable question,
and we have
designed a simple exercise to help in that endeavor.
Researchable questions almost invariably involve a relationship
between
two or more variables, phenomena, concepts, or ideas. The
nature of that
relationship may vary. Research studies generally consist of
methods to
explicate the nature of the relationship. Research in the social
sciences
rarely consists of explicating a single construct (e.g., “I will
look at
everything there is to know about the ‘imposter phenomenon’”)
or a single
variable (e.g., voting rates in presidential elections).1 Even the
presence of
two variables is apt to be limiting, and oftentimes it is only
when a third
21
“connecting” variable is invoked that an idea becomes
researchable. As a
caveat, however, we acknowledge that research questions that
are
qualitative rather than quantitative in nature might not be as
focused on the
relationship between variables as on “how” processes develop
or are
experienced. We will have more to say about this distinction in
forthcoming chapters.
An example might help to demonstrate how the introduction of
an
additional variable can lead to the birth of a promising study.
Let us
assume that I am interested in how members of a younger
generation
perceive the elderly. At this level, a study would be rather
mundane and
likely to lead to a “So what?” response. So far, it implies asking
people
what they think of the elderly, perhaps using interviews or tests
or even
behavioral observations. But we really won’t learn much of
value about
the nature of perceptions of the elderly in contemporary society
and what
influences those perceptions. Introducing a second variable,
however, can
lead to a set of questions that have promising theoretical (as
well as
practical) implications: I wonder what the role of the media is
in shaping
social perceptions of the elderly? I wonder if living with a
grandparent
makes any difference in how the elderly are viewed? I wonder
how
specific legislation designed to benefit the elderly has changed
our
perception of them? I wonder if there is a relationship between
how
middle-aged adults deal with their aging parents and how they
view the
elderly? The new variables introduced in these potential
research questions
are, respectively, the slant of the media, presence or absence of
a
grandparent, type of legislation, and treatment of one’s own
parents. These
variables impart meaning to the research because they offer
suggestions as
to what accounts for variability in perceptions of the elderly.
As an example of generating a research question using three
primary
variables, let’s say that you have inferred that many women lose
interest in
sexual relations with their husbands after the birth of a child. At
this level,
the proposed study would consist of checking out this hunch by
assessing
the sexual interest of women (Variable 1) before and after
childbirth
(Variable 2). But what would this finding mean? The
introduction of a
third variable or construct could lead to a much more
sophisticated and
conceptually meaningful study. An investigator might ask, “I
wonder if the
partner’s involvement in parenting makes a difference? What’s
the role of
his sexual initiative? How about childbirth complications?
Father’s
involvement in the birthing? The length of time they have been
married?
22
Presence of other children in the home? Mother’s level of
fatigue? Her
body image?” There is no end to the number of interesting
questions that
can be raised simply by introducing another variable into the
proposed
study. This variable would then help to explain the nature of the
relationship between the primary variables. In fact, one could
brainstorm a
whole list of third variables that could contribute to a better
understanding
of the relationship between childbirth and sexuality.
Note that the precise function of the third, or connecting,
variable depends
on the logic of the conceptual model or theory underlying the
study. In this
regard, a distinction can be made between two terms, mediator
and
moderator, which play important roles in research questions. A
moderator
variable pinpoints the conditions under which an independent
variable
exerts its effects on a dependent variable. Strictly speaking, a
moderator
effect is an interaction effect in which the influence of one
variable
depends upon the level of another variable (Frazier, Tix, &
Barron, 2004).
One commonly employed moderator variable is gender, which
has two
levels, male and female. The relationship between provocation
and
aggression, for example, may be very different for men and
women. The
role of context can also be conceptualized as a moderator
variable. The
famous Kinsey report on sexual behavior would certainly have
generated
very different results if the interviews with participants about
their sex
lives had taken place in the presence of family members.
Identification of
relevant contextual variables has important implications for the
design of a
study because such variables will affect the generalizability of
research
findings.
A mediating variable, on the other hand, tries to describe how
or why
rather than when or for whom effects will occur by accounting
for the
relationship between the independent variable (the predictor)
and the
dependent variable (the criterion). The mediator is the
mechanism through
which the predictor affects the outcome.2 As such, one can
think of
mediators as process variables. For example, in the counseling
psychology
field, maladaptive perfectionism can be regarded as either a
moderating
variable or a mediating variable (Wei, Mallinckrodt, Russell, &
Abraham,
2004). Conceptualized as a moderator, attachment anxiety could
be seen to
exert negative effects on depressive mood only under conditions
of high
maladaptive perfectionism (i.e., there is a statistical interaction
between
maladaptive perfectionism and attachment anxiety).
Conceptualized as a
mediator, maladaptive perfectionism acts as an intervening
variable
23
between attachment anxiety and depressive mood (i.e., there is
an indirect
relationship between anxiety and depression). As Wei et al.
stated,
It is possible for maladaptive perfectionism to serve as both an
intermediate link in the causal chain leading from attachment
insecurity to depressive mood (i.e., as a mediator) and as a
variable that alters the strength of association between
attachment insecurity and depressive mood (i.e., as a
moderator).
(p. 203)
The diagram in Figure 2.1 captures the distinction between
moderating and
mediating variables in a theoretical model. In the case of
mediation, the
mediating variable (maladaptive perfectionism) is placed
between
attachment anxiety and depressed mood. In the case of
moderation, the
arrow from maladaptive perfectionism points to another arrow,
that from
attachment anxiety to depressed mood, indicating that the
relationship
between attachment anxiety and depressed mood depends on the
level of
maladaptive perfectionism.
Figure 2.1 The Distinction Between Moderator and Mediator
Variables, Represented in a Causal Diagram
24
Figure 2.2 Classification Plot Representing Moderation of
Relationship Between Attachment Anxiety and Depressed
Mood,
Moderated by Maladaptive Perfectionism
25
Source: Author created using data from Wei, W., Mallinckrodt,
B.,
Russell, D., & Abraham, W. T. (2004). Maladaptive
perfectionism as
a mediator and moderator between adult attachment and
depressive
mood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(2), 201–212.
We have illustrated one potential moderated outcome in the
classification
plot shown in Figure 2.2. Note that under conditions of low
maladaptive
perfectionism, there is only a small difference in depression
between those
with low and high attachment anxiety; however, under
conditions of high
maladaptive perfectionism, those with high levels of attachment
anxiety
are more likely to be depressed than those with low levels. In
other words,
maladaptive perfectionism moderates the relationship between
attachment
anxiety and depression, or, put another way, maladaptive
perfectionism
and attachment anxiety interact.
One research study is not likely to establish and verify all of the
important
elements of a complex conceptual model. As one of our
colleagues puts it,
you would need a video camera to capture the entire Grand
Canyon on
film, whereas the dissertation is more like a snapshot, perhaps
of a mule
26
and rider descending one small section of one canyon trail. Yet
the
proposed model can provide a useful context for current and
future
research studies. Most ambitious research studies rely heavily
on just such
theoretical models.
As you might imagine, a researcher is in no position to test a
model of this
scope in a single study. For example, Gerald Patterson and his
colleagues
(Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989) spent many years
developing
and testing a model to explain aggressive and deviant behavior
among
young males. The model hypothesizes that such antisocial
behavior can be
causally linked to disrupted parental discipline and poor family
management skills. Moreover, the relationship between these
two sets of
variables is not direct but is mediated by a network of other
variables. The
process is thought to begin with parents “training” a child to
behave
aggressively by relying on aversive behaviors in both
punishment and
negative reinforcement contingencies. The inability of the
parents to
control coercive exchanges among family members constitutes
“training
for fighting,” which leads, in turn, to aggressive behavior and
poor peer
relationships. This lack of social skills generalizes to antisocial
behavior in
the classroom, which makes it next to impossible for the youth
to obtain
basic academic skills, thus preparing him poorly to cope with
life outside
school. Ultimately, this set of factors leads to high rates of
delinquent
behavior. An abbreviated summary of one version of the model
is shown
in Figure 2.3.
Figure 2.3 A Model of Antisocial Behavior
27
Source: From Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E.
A
developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American
Psychologist, 44. Copyright © 1989, American Psychological
Association. Reprinted with permission.
Over the years that Patterson and his colleagues (Patterson et
al., 1989)
spent elaborating the nature of these relationships, they
conducted
numerous studies that each constituted a “snapshot” of one
aspect of this
complex model, perhaps focusing on a particular set of
relationships. For
instance, the investigator might ask whether a relationship
exists between
physical fighting and poor peer relationships. Each variable
would have to
be operationalized, probably by obtaining more than one
measure of both
fighting and peer relationships. In Patterson’s work, he asked
mothers,
peers, and teachers to rate levels of physical fighting because
their
perspectives might differ. Likewise, peers, teachers, and self-
reports are
used to obtain measures of peer relations. The objective of the
study—that
is, to determine the nature and form of the relationship between
the
primary variables—determines the research method that is
employed. In
the early years of his career, Patterson focused on the
relationships among
contextual variables, parental beliefs, parenting practices, and
child
outcomes. He concluded that parenting practices—such as
discipline,
monitoring, problem solving, involvement, and positive
reinforcement—
serve as mediating variables between parenting beliefs and
attitudes and
the child’s behavior. Once this model was supported by
sufficient data,
Patterson proceeded to establish links between children’s
behavior
problems and subsequent chronic juvenile and adult offending
(Reid,
Patterson, & Snyder, 2002).
Whether or not a particular dissertation is designed to test a
theory or
model derived from the research literature, we believe that the
creation of
a visual model, which shows how the network of relevant
variables and
constructs may be related to one another, can serve as a
powerful tool for
guiding the study. Arranging your ideas spatially helps to
organize your
thinking, which in turn helps position your proposed study
within a larger
framework.
Research models are developed to account for the relationships
among
variables at a conceptual level and then used to guide the
construction of
research designs by which the relationships will be tested,
usually, but not
always, using contemporary statistics. The process is iterative
so that the
28
models are modified on the basis of data and then reevaluated in
further
studies. Two primary types of relationships can be identified
and explored
within a causal model (Jaccard & Jacoby, 2010): predictive
relationships
and causal relationships. A predictive relationship implies that
an
association or correlation exists between two (or more)
variables without
assuming that one causes the other. For example, we may
determine that
traveling frequently as a child is related to (predicts) being
more proficient
in languages as an adult, without knowing whether travel
actually causes
improvement in language skills. Above, we used the terms
independent
variables and predictor variables more or less interchangeably.
Strictly
speaking, however, when the issue is prediction, the
relationship is
between one or more predictor variables and a criterion variable
(Jaccard
& Jacoby, 2010).
Causal relationships imply that one variable “causes” another;
that is,
changes in the primary variable, usually referred to as the
independent
variable, elicit changes in the second variable, the dependent
variable or
outcome variable. Although the principle of causation is the
foundational
bedrock for the experimental method in social science research,
philosophers of science have argued for centuries whether
causality can
ever be truly demonstrated. Jaccard and Jacoby (2010) made a
persuasive
argument that the concept of causality is ultimately a heuristic
that enables
us to maintain an organized view of our world and of human
behavior. By
inferring causality, we can identify systematic relationships
between
variables and produce socially significant changes by
manipulating some
variables to influence others. Whether or not causality can be
definitively
demonstrated, much of contemporary research is conducted to
give us
confidence in theoretical models that purport causal
relationships.
There are several types of causal relationships, and each type
can play a
role in developing a causal model. How causal models can be
employed
and evaluated in dissertations is a subject for a later chapter
(Chapter 6).
How to think about and construct causal models as a way of
describing
research ideas constitutes much of Jaccard and Jacoby’s (2010)
very useful
book, and the following discussion is stimulated by their work.
Jaccard
and Jacoby observed that most researchers begin by identifying
an
outcome variable that they want to understand better. An
example might
be the level of concern people express for the environment,
including by
engaging in behaviors that are environmentally sensitive, such
as recycling
waste or reducing pollution. The next challenge is to identify
some
29
variables that could potentially influence or relate to
environmentally
sensitive behavior. One could imagine a study, for example, that
seeks to
determine which interventions would increase the motivation to
recycle
trash (or, more modestly, just to understand differences between
those who
readily recycle and those who do not). Perhaps we predict that
having a
neighborhood trash collection system that mandates sorting
trash into
recyclable and nonrecyclable categories will directly affect
environmentally pro-social behavior. Of course, not all studies
begin by
identifying a dependent variable; it is also possible to choose an
independent variable and speculate about its effects. For
instance, a study
might address the implications of working in a highly polluting
industry
for health, socioeconomic status, and social relationships.
Indirect causal relationships have an effect through the
influence of an
intermediary variable, which we have referred to above as a
mediating
variable. Moderated causal relationships are a third type of
causal
relationship, again defined earlier in the chapter. Most causal
models
contain a combination of the various types of relationships.
Models can get
very complex because of the number of variables and their
subtle
relationships with one another. Thus, simple path diagrams
evolve into
sophisticated theoretical networks. Many of these models have
been
developed over the course of a career by dedicated researchers,
such as
Gerald Patterson, who started with studies that explored subsets
of
variables within a model that then evolved in complexity.
Because
computer software allows for the manipulation of multiple
variables
simultaneously and relatively effortlessly, researchers usually
begin by
proposing one or more theoretical models and evaluating them
empirically.
One example of a research model comes from the dissertation of
Bill
MacNulty (2004), one of our doctoral students. MacNulty
generated this
model from existing research literature and then tested it
empirically using
a number of well-validated self-report scales. The study
employed the
schema-polarity model of psychological functioning to assess
how self-
schemas (cognitive representations of self and others) influence
the
experience of gratitude and forgiveness and whether these
variables
mediate relationships between self-schemas and physical health
and well-
being. The model is summarized in Figure 2.4. The plus and
minus signs
refer to the direction of the hypothesized relationships among
the
variables. Although the results supported most of the initial
hypotheses,
the proposed model needed to be amended to accommodate the
data. This
30
is typical of the research enterprise, in which theories and
conceptual
models are continually tested and refined to serve as
increasingly
sophisticated representations of real-life phenomena.
Another dissertation example comes from Jenny Knetig’s (2012)
study of
active duty military personnel who are at risk of experiencing
symptoms of
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Knetig speculated that
being
psychologically minded (a component of what is referred to as
having a
mentalizing capacity) allows some soldiers to perceive and
interpret
cognitive and affective states of themselves and others in a way
that might
facilitate resilience and help-seeking behaviors, which, in turn,
mitigate
how they are affected by severe stress. At the outset of her
study, on the
basis of the available literature and her own experience, Knetig
postulated
that the relationships among these variables might look
something like the
diagram in Figure 2.5.
Figure 2.4 A Theoretical Framework Presented as a Causal
Diagram
Source: From Self-Schemas, Forgiveness, Gratitude, Physical
Health,
and Subjective Well-Being, by W. MacNulty, 2004, unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Fielding Graduate University, Santa
Barbara,
CA. Copyright 2004 by W. MacNulty. Reprinted with
permission of
the author.
At the conclusion of the study, after collecting her data and
performing a
number of statistical analyses (canonical correlation analysis),
Knetig
amended her proposed model, as shown in Figure 2.6, to reflect
the
experiences of her participants more accurately.
31
Figure 2.5 Proposed Mediational Model Relating Psychological
Mindedness to PTSD
Source: Knetig, 2012, p. 52. Reprinted with permission of the
author.
In short, the data suggested that soldiers who are more
psychologically
minded are less apt to conceal their thoughts and feelings. It
also suggested
that the relationship between psychological mindedness and
symptoms is
mediated by self-concealment.
Figure 2.6 Final Mediation Model Relating Psychological
Mindedness to PTSD
Source: Knetig, 2012, p. 54. Reprinted with permission of the
author.
Generating Researchable Questions
To help students generate researchable questions from their
interesting
ideas, we use a brainstorming exercise that begins with labeling
one or two
variables and generating a second or third. Brainstorming
consists of
32
openly and noncritically listing all possible ideas in a given
period of time.
Later you can return to a more critical analysis of each idea and
delete
those that are uninteresting, not meaningful, or impractical.
Ultimately, of
course, it is contact with the literature that determines whether
or not a
research question is viable, because the literature houses the
scholarly
inquiry that goes beyond the limits of your own knowledge.
We suggest that you do this brainstorming exercise in a small
group so that
the person receiving the consultation merely serves as a scribe
to record
the ideas thrown out by the other group members (see Box 2.1).
After 5 or
10 minutes, move on to the next person’s partially formed
research topic.
We generally use this exercise in groups of three or four so that
group
members can frequently shift groups and draw on the
spontaneous
reactions of a larger number of peers, uncontaminated by prior
ideas or a
particular mind-set.
The exercise involves suspending critical thinking and allowing
new ideas
to percolate. It should especially suit divergent thinkers, who
will find the
demand to be expansive in their thinking exciting and creative.
Convergent
thinkers may experience the exercise as a bit overwhelming, but
they will
find fulfillment in other stages of the research process that
demand
compulsivity, care, and precision. Every chapter of a
dissertation contains
both divergent and convergent elements.
Note that not all worthwhile research studies focus on three (or
more)
primary variables. Many studies look at the relationship
between two
variables or concepts, and a few descriptive studies make do
with one
variable or construct. The latter generally occurs in the early
stages of
research in an area, when little is known about a topic. Some
investigators
are pathfinders in terms of opening up new topics of research by
trying to
understand as much as possible about a phenomenon and
generating more
informed hypotheses for others to test in the future.
Nevertheless, we
believe that most students underestimate what is currently
known about
most topics and that the most interesting, practical, and
theoretically
meaningful studies are likely to consider relationships among
several
variables.
Our exercise for generating research questions is only one
option. Jaccard
and Jacoby (2010) have listed 26 different heuristics for how to
think
creatively about questions or phenomena that may be of interest
to you.
Items on their list include analyzing your own experiences,
using case
33
studies, interviewing experts in the field, role-playing, and
conducting
thought experiments. The adoption of thought experiments
might be
particularly helpful. Jaccard and Jacoby viewed these as
experiments that
you conduct in your mind as if you are really obtaining the data
and
analyzing the results. You can then play with the possibilities of
adding
new variables or introducing different contingencies into the
situation. One
contingency is the introduction of so-called counterfactuals into
the
thought experiment (Tetlock & Belkin, 1996). Counterfactuals
refer to
“what-might-have-been” scenarios, such as what might happen
if parents
rather than administrators ran the public schools. Researchers
can use this
strategy to address theoretical alternatives that might otherwise
not be
raised or appreciated.
We conclude this chapter with the outline in Box 2.2, which
asks you to
look at the kinds of issues that need to be considered and
responded to
during the course of developing the research proposal. By and
large, your
dissertation committee will need to be convinced of three things
to be
comfortable with your proposal:
1. Is the question clear and researchable, and will the answer to
the
question extend knowledge in your field of study?
2. Have you located your question within the context of
previous study
such that you have demonstrated a mastery and consideration of
the
relevant background literature?
3. Is the proposed method suitable for exploring your question?
Box 2.1 Brainstorming Exercise
Begin by defining one or two variables (or constructs) of
interest. Then generate a list of additional variables (or
constructs) that in some way amplify the original variables or
illuminate the relationship between them. The new
variables you list may be independent variables, dependent
variables, moderating variables, or mediating
variables in the research questions you eventually select. After
brainstorming this list, go back and eliminate
those variables that do not interest you or do not seem
promising to pursue. Finally, see if you can now define
one or more research questions that speak to the relationship
among the two or three variables (or constructs) you
have specified. Ultimately, each of these variables will need to
be operationally defined as you develop your
research study.
Here are some examples of the application of this brainstorming
exercise to topics from different disciplines.
Political Science
Begin with an interest in citizen participation in city council
meetings. List variables or phenomena that might
influence, be influenced by, or be related to this variable. A
sample research question is “What is the impact of
citizen participation in city council meetings on legislative
decision making?”
34
Education
Begin with an interest in single mothers who return to school
while receiving Aid to Families with Dependent
Children (AFDC). List variables or phenomena that might
influence, be influenced by, or be related to this
variable. A sample research question is “What is the effect of
the availability of child care on whether single
mothers receiving AFDC return to school?”
Criminal Justice
Begin with an interest in the relationship between neighborhood
crime watch programs and robbery rates. List
variables that might influence or amplify the relationship
between these two variables. A sample research
question is “What is the effect of neighborhood crime watch
programs, in both urban and rural environments, on
the rate of burglaries?”
Psychology
Begin with an interest in the relationship between physical
attractiveness and self-esteem. List variables that
might amplify or influence the relationship between these two
variables. A sample research question is “What is
the role of body image and physical attractiveness in self-
esteem?” Another sample research question is “What is
the role of body image in mediating the relationship between
physical attractiveness and self-esteem?”
Selecting a Suitable Topic: Student Suggestions
35
Over the years, our students have provided many useful
suggestions for completing a dissertation. Here are some
of the suggestions they have offered to one another that pertain
to the earliest stages of developing a dissertation.
Other suggestions are noted at the appropriate places in
subsequent chapters.
1. Start a computer file where you can store good ideas for
future reference. Use the file for noting books
and articles to get from the library, good quotations,
inspirations for future studies, half-baked notions
that might be useful in the future, and so on.
2. Think of your topic as a large jigsaw puzzle with a piece
missing. That piece is what you want to
research to fill in the gap in your field. To discover which piece
is missing, you must read as much of the
literature as possible in your field.
3. Before you begin, read several well-written dissertations
recommended by your chairperson.
4. As you progress through the dissertation process and your
question shrinks due to the necessity of
maintaining a manageable project for one person, don’t lose
heart. Even very small questions can serve
much larger purposes.
5. To keep the perspective of meaningfulness throughout, keep
imagining an audience of individuals who
would want to know the results of your work. Even if you can
imagine only 25 people in the world who
would care, keep that group alive in front of your eyes.
6. A few students and I have found that beginning dissertation
work early on and setting “mile marker
dates” is very helpful. This has helped us keep each other
accountable by regular “check-ins” with each
other. Sharing these dates with friends and family as well as
posting them in a conspicuous place so you
see them pretty much daily has helped as well.
7. Make contact with researchers who may be of interest to you.
It cannot hurt, and it might be quite
helpful.
8. Travel to at least one professional meeting where research in
your area of interest is being presented. I
went to a paper presentation in my area of family violence. I
realized that I knew more about my topic
than I thought and shared my research ideas with one of the
presenters. I came home reinvigorated and
anxious to complete my dissertation.
36
Box 2.2 Outline of Issues for a Student Researcher to Complete
in the
Development of the Research Proposal
Review of the Literature
The classic, definitive, or most influential pieces of research in
this area are . . .
The journals that specialize in the kind of research in which I
propose to engage are . . .
The body(ies) of research to which I wish to add is (are) . . .
The experts in the field of my research are . . .
Statement of the Problem
The intellectual problem(s) I may help solve through this
research is (are) . . .
The moral, political, social, or practical problem(s) I may help
alleviate through this research is (are) . . .
Method
The method I propose to use to answer my question, prove my
point, or gain more detailed and
substantive knowledge is . . .
An alternative way to do this study would be . . .
Three important research studies that have been carried out
using the method I propose are . . .
The reason(s) this method is a good one for my question,
proposition, or subject is (are) . . .
Possible weaknesses of this method are . . .
The skills I will need to use this method are . . .
Of these skills, I still need to acquire . . .
I propose to acquire these skills by . . .
Notes
1. A construct is a concept used for scientific purposes in
building
theories. Constructs (e.g., self-esteem), like concepts, are
abstractions
formed by generalizing from specific behaviors or
manipulations. When
constructs are operationalized in such a way that they can be
“scored” to
take on different numerical values, they are referred to as
variables.
2. There has been extensive discussion in recent literature
regarding the
definition of, appropriate statistical analyses for, and
interpretation of
moderating and mediating effects. We recommend the following
discussions: Frazier, Tix, and Barron, 2004; Hayes, 2009;
Jaccard and
Jacoby, 2010; Kazdin, 2007; Kim, Kaye, and Wright, 2001; and
Preacher
and Hayes, 2008.
37
3 Methods of Inquiry Quantitative and
Qualitative Approaches
The principal characteristic of scholarly and scientific inquiry—
as opposed
to informal, intuitive kinds of inquiry—is the use of rationally
grounded
procedures to extend knowledge that a community of scholars
regards as
reliable and valid. The dissertation process is a ritual of
socialization into
that community of scholars, so it is necessary for you, as a
student, to
master the scholarly procedures within your discipline. The
specific
methods chosen to attack a problem will depend on your
discipline and the
nature of the specific problem. There is no universally accepted
approach
within the social sciences, although there are rich research
traditions that
cannot be ignored, as well as a common understanding that
chosen
methods of inquiry must rest on rational justification. This
means that
scientific methods differ from more informal methods of inquiry
by their
reliance on validated public procedures that have been
determined to
produce reliable knowledge.
Currently, there are many disagreements in the social sciences
regarding
what constitutes knowledge and the procedures for gaining it.
One way to
think about how research generally contributes to the knowledge
base of a
discipline is in terms of the following three-level hierarchy of
knowledge,
suggested by our colleague Marilyn Freimuth.
Axiologic/Epistemic Level.
This is the underlying level of basic world hypotheses that form
the
foundation for content and method within a field of inquiry.
Epistemology
refers to the study of the nature of knowledge, whereas axiology
refers to
the study of ethics, values, and aesthetics. Examples of
constructs at this
level include the explanatory principle of cause and effect and
the notion
of open systems.
Theoretical Level.
This is the level of models and theories. Theories are premises
to account
for data or, more informally, explanations of how things work
based on
38
data. Examples are the theory of loss aversion in economics
(Tversky &
Kahneman, 1991) and the five-factor theory of personality in
psychology
(McCrae & Costa, 2003). The distinction between theories and
models is
murky because these terms are often used interchangeably
within the
social sciences. At the most basic level, both theories and
models refer to
relationships between concepts. For our purposes, the term
model refers to
a higher-order theory, that is, a representational system at a
higher level of
abstraction that can inform and be informed by alternative
theories. (This
concept is similar to the framework or worldview that guides
researchers,
identified as a “paradigm” by Thomas Kuhn [1996].) Thus,
psychoanalysis
could be seen as a model, a wide lens with which to view and
understand
the mysteries of human behavior. Each model carries with it
certain sets of
assumptions. In the case of psychoanalysis, these assumptions
include the
unifying importance of causal determinism and unconscious
motivation.
Note that this use of the term model differs somewhat from that
in the
discussion of working models in Chapter 2.
Empirical Level.
In the field of epistemology, empiricism refers to a commitment
to
obtaining knowledge through sense experience (literally, “based
on
experience” in Greek). Empiricism is frequently contrasted with
rationalism, which refers to knowledge derived purely through
thought
and reason, and to more natural philosophical and religious
traditions of
reaching conclusions. In the present context, the empirical level
includes
hypotheses and methods and data of scientific research.
Hypotheses are
tentative answers to questions, generally based on theory.
The primary role of research within this three-level schema is to
link the
theoretical and the empirical. Theories need the support of data
to remain
viable, whereas methods carry assumptions that are theoretical
in nature.
Note that research findings do not contribute directly to the
axiologic/epistemic level or even to basic models. Those levels
reflect
fundamental value commitments and personal preferences that
are rarely
modified on the basis of additional data, especially the kind of
data
generated by scholarly research. It is hard to imagine a
psychoanalyst
becoming a behaviorist or a Republican joining the Democrats
without a
significant shift in values unlikely to be compelled by the
accumulated
wisdom imparted by a series of research studies. Because most
researchers
strongly identify with particular values and carry many personal
39
preferences into their work, it becomes especially important to
learn to
discriminate between beliefs and opinions, on the one hand, and
verifiable,
data-inspired support for ideas, on the other hand.
A brief look at the history of science is a humbling experience
that should
put to rest the misguided notion that research discovers truth.
Drilling
holes in the skull (trephining) used to be an acceptable way to
dismiss the
demons responsible for mental illness, and it wasn’t that long
ago that the
sun was thought to circle the earth. One wonders what remnants
of
contemporary scientific truth will be regarded as equally
ludicrous
tomorrow. Instead, what research contributes is a series of
thoughtful
observations that support or question the validity of theories,
which are in
turn based on a set of largely untestable beliefs and
assumptions. Every
once in a while, at opportunistic moments of scholarly
upheaval, a new
paradigm appears that seems to do a better job of explaining the
available
data and guiding further inquiry.
Each social science discipline and set of investigators seems to
have its
own favored approach to generating knowledge. For instance,
public
opinion studies usually rely on survey research methods,
psychoanalytic
studies of infants make use of observational techniques, studies
of
organizational effectiveness may employ action research
methods and case
studies, historical investigations of political and social events
rely on
archival records and content analysis, and laboratory studies of
perceptual
processes stress experimental manipulation and hypothesis
testing. Within
your chosen field, it is important to ask how a piece of research
acquires
legitimacy as reliable knowledge. No doubt part of the answer
comes
down to underlying epistemological assumptions and values.
Certainly
research strategies will differ in terms of the problems they
address and the
outcomes they produce. As we later show, one important
distinction in the
choice of method seems to be the nature of the relationship
between the
researcher and the topic of study.
We would argue that researchers in the social sciences have
generally been
myopic in defining the kinds of studies that might legitimately
lend
themselves to research dissertations. Most students in the social
sciences
are taught early on about the difference between independent
and
dependent variables and how experimental research implies
active
manipulation of independent variables to observe a subsequent
impact on
dependent variables. This basic and time-honored strategy has
an earthy
40
history in the systematic evaluation of fertilizers for
agricultural
productivity (Cowles, 2000). It remains a cornerstone in
conducting social
science research with human subjects. Yet it is certainly not the
only way
to conduct research.
The only universal in scientific knowledge is a general
commitment to
using logical argument and evidence to arrive at conclusions
that are
recognized as tentative and subject to further amendment. Good
scientists
in action often deviate from an “official” philosophy of science
and a
prescribed methodology. William Bevan (1991), former
president of the
American Psychological Association, noted,
If you want to understand what effective science making is
about, don’t listen to what creative scientists say about their
formal belief systems. Watch what they do. When they engage
in
good, effective science making they don’t, as a rule, reflect on
their presuppositions; they engage in a practical art form in
which their decisions are motivated by the requirements of
particular problem solving. (p. 478)
The key to evaluating a completed study is to assess whether the
selected
method is sufficiently rigorous and appropriate to the research
question
and whether the study is conceptually and theoretically
grounded. The
more familiar you are with the full range of alternative research
strategies,
the more enlightened and appropriate your choice of a particular
method is
apt to be. Too often, students become so enamored with an
approach to
research that they choose the method before determining the
question.
Unless the dissertation is designed to illustrate the use of a
promising and
innovative methodology, this is putting the cart before the
horse. In
general, the method needs to evolve out of the research question
and be
determined by it.
Quantitative Methods
The epistemological foundation of most social science inquiry
throughout
the 20th century was logical positivism, a school of thought that
maintains
that all knowledge is derived from direct observation and
logical
inferences based on direct observation. To a great extent, the
notion of
41
objectively studying human beings is derived from a love affair
that social
scientists have had with the natural sciences, which sought to
understand
nature by isolating phenomena, observing them, and formulating
mathematical laws to describe patterns in nature. Current
research in the
social sciences is deeply steeped in the empirical and
quantitative
traditions.
Statistical methods are especially useful for looking at
relationships and
patterns and expressing these patterns with numbers.
Descriptive statistics
describe these patterns of behavior, whereas inferential
statistics draw on
probabilistic arguments to generalize findings from samples to
populations
of interest. Kerlinger (1977) focused on the inferential process
when he
defined statistics as
the theory and method of analyzing quantitative data obtained
from samples of observations in order to study and compare
sources of variance of phenomena, to help make decisions to
accept or reject hypothesized relations between the phenomena,
and to aid in making reliable inferences from empirical
observations. (p. 185)
Note that the focus in the natural science model of research is
the study of
average or group effects, not of individual differences. The
kinds of
inferential statements that derive from this model of research
refer to
groups of people or groups of events; that is, they are
probabilistic (e.g.,
“Surveys find that most people believe that police officers use
excessive
force in dealing with criminals,” or “Emotional expressiveness
is related to
coping effectively with natural disasters”).
In experimental research, quantitative research designs are used
to
determine aggregate differences between groups or classes of
subjects.
Emphasis is placed on precise measurement and controlling for
extraneous
sources of error. The purpose, therefore, is to isolate a variable
of interest
(the independent variable) and manipulate it to observe the
impact of the
manipulation on a second, or dependent, variable. This
procedure is
facilitated by the “control” of extraneous variables, thus
allowing the
researcher to infer a causal relationship between the two (or
more)
variables of interest.
42
Methodological control is generally accomplished by two
procedures that
rely on the principle of randomness. One is random sampling,
which uses
subjects that have randomly been drawn from the potential pool
of subjects
so that each member of the population has an equal chance or
known
probability of being selected. Random selection of subjects
permits the
researcher to generalize the results of the study from the sample
to the
population in question. The second procedure is randomization,
which
assigns subjects to groups or experimental conditions in such a
way that
each subject has an equal chance of being selected for each
condition.
Subject characteristics are thus randomly distributed in every
respect other
than the experimental manipulation or treatment, allowing the
researcher
to infer that resultant differences between the groups must be
the result of
the isolated variable in question.
Unfortunately, these efforts at experimental control are often
impractical in
social science research with human subjects. Psychology, for
instance, has
an honorable tradition of laboratory research using tight
experimental
designs, but research in the clinical or social arena may not
permit the kind
of control stipulated by the experimental method. This dilemma
is equally
prominent in field studies in disciplines such as sociology,
education, and
political science. Jared Diamond (2005), Pulitzer Prize–winning
geographer and biologist, for example, conducted quantitative
“natural
experiments” to investigate the problem of deforestation on
Pacific islands.
He and his colleague Barry Rolett numerically graded the extent
of
deforestation on 81 Pacific islands and statistically predicted
this outcome
from a combination of nine input variables, such as the amount
of rainfall,
isolation from human populations, and restoration of soil
fertility. In a
different context, one cannot practically conspire to rear
children using
two distinct parenting styles, nor can one ethically inflict child
abuse to
study its immediate impact in a controlled fashion. Researchers
can,
however, study analogs of these variables using pure
experimental designs
(e.g., one can ask parents to use specific interventions at the
onset of
particular child behaviors). Change studies, in which a
treatment or
program is being evaluated for its effectiveness, may also lend
themselves
well to experimental designs. Even so, it may not be possible to
randomize
subjects into groups that receive a treatment or intervention and
those that
do not. A number of ingenious solutions have been proposed to
deal with
the ethics of denying treatment to those who need it, including
the use of
placebos and waiting-list controls (Kazdin, 2002).
43
More typically, the research method of choice in the social
sciences seems
to be a quasi-experimental design that compromises some of the
rigor of
the controlled experiment but maintains the argument and logic
of
experimental research (Kline, 2009; Shadish, Cook, &
Campbell, 2001).
This kind of research uses a systematic, empirical approach in
which the
investigator does not employ a control group or does not
randomly assign
subjects to conditions because events have already occurred or
cannot be
sufficiently manipulated. So-called causal statements become
correlational
statements in quasi-experimental research, although it is often
possible to
infer a sequence of events in causal form. That is one reason
why it is
crucial to have a theoretical model as a foundation for an
empirical study.
The model itself informs your attempt to meaningfully interpret
the results
of the study. However, because it is difficult to ascribe
causality with
confidence from quasi-experimental designs that lack true
experimental
manipulation, independent variables are often termed
“predictor” variables
in these studies (Kline, 2009).
Caution is also needed in interpreting the meaning of results
whenever
subjects assign themselves to groups. A colorful example is the
apparent
negative correlation that exists between the numbers of mules
found in the
various states and the number of PhDs living there. The fact
that states that
have a lot of mules don’t have so many PhDs, and vice versa, is
an
empirical observation that can be statistically expressed in the
form of a
correlation coefficient. A researcher would be hard-pressed to
argue a
causal relationship between these two variables unless he or she
drew on
an underlying theoretical model that links the two variables
through a third
(mediating) variable, such as the degree of urbanization. Note
that this
simple correlational study could, at least theoretically, be
transformed into
an experimental study by, for example, flooding some states
with mules to
see if the PhDs leave or wooing the PhDs across state lines to
see if the
number of mules in the new state of residence decreases.
This is not a book on research design, but the adoption of a
particular
research strategy will affect the final form of your dissertation.
Whether a
study employs a true experimental design, a quasi-experimental
design, or
a cross-sectional survey design, the most popular strategy in the
social
sciences is a comparison between groups. That is, independent
(randomly
assigned) groups of subjects are used for each experimental or
control
condition. The best-known variant of this strategy, the pretest-
posttest
control group design, uses two equivalent groups of subjects
that both
44
receive pretests and posttests and differ only in the
experimental treatment
that is given to one group (see Table 3.1).
In this design, it becomes possible to evaluate the impact of an
intervention because the control group offers a baseline for
comparison.
One could use this design to evaluate, for example, whether the
inclusion
of spouses in an aftercare program for heart bypass surgery
patients
encourages greater compliance with medical regimens. Or one
could
design a study to evaluate the effect of introducing air bags in
automobiles
on the rate of physical injury to passengers. Some automobiles
of a given
make would receive air bags, some would not, and the change in
types and
rates of injuries would be the dependent measure.
The straightforward pretest–posttest–control group design
makes it
possible to attribute the effect of experimental interventions to
those
interventions rather than to some extraneous variable. The
interpretation of
results of studies using this design may be compromised,
however, if the
subjects have not been assigned to conditions in a truly random
manner. In
the proposed air bag study, for example, if automobiles and
drivers are not
randomly assigned to conditions, inherently safer drivers may
well choose
automobiles with better safety features. Because randomization
is not
always possible, it becomes crucial to argue for the
“equivalence” of the
two groups, even if they do not derive from the identical
population of
subjects. One way in which researchers attempt to make this
argument is
by matching the groups on key variables that are critical to the
understanding of the study, such as age, sex, symptomatology,
or, in the
current example, the previous driving records of the
participants.
The basic pretest–posttest–control group design does not
adequately
control for any effect that the pretest evaluations might have on
the
subjects. Some assessments can sensitize subjects by making
them aware
that they are now participating in a study or by providing a
practice
45
experience that contaminates the validity of posttest results. A
simple
posttest-only design may get around this difficulty and is
probably
underused (Campbell & Stanley, 2005). In any case, choosing a
basic
research design does not eliminate the need for you to think
carefully and
creatively about potential sources of error and alternative
explanations to
account for findings.
Most experimental designs are variants of the treatment and
control group
format described earlier.1 Such designs permit the researcher to
make
causal inferences regarding relationships among the variables.
In contrast,
correlational (or observational) studies do not generally enable
the
researcher to demonstrate causal relationships among variables.
Any
conclusions regarding causality must be inferred from the
underlying
theory rather than from the results of the study.
Studies built around experimental or correlational designs
generate data
that are subsequently analyzed using appropriate inferential
statistics.
Statistical techniques that are used to evaluate the effectiveness
of an
intervention or a difference between groups, such as an analysis
of
variance (ANOVA) or t test, compare the size of between-group
differences (e.g., the treatment effect) with the size of within-
group
differences due to individual variability. These techniques
express the
experimental tradition. The logic of the correlational paradigm
is quite
different (Cronbach, 1975). Correlations depend on comparing
two
distributions of scores that are broadly dispersed along two
dimensions,
such as longevity and alcohol use. Statistical techniques that
emerged from
this tradition, such as multiple regression, are especially
popular in social
science research that relies on questionnaires, surveys, or scales
and the
relationship between continuous variables. Because
correlational studies
typically cannot randomly assign subjects to groups, we have a
second
major type of control in social science research—statistical
control.
Statistical control attempts to use complex statistical procedures
to remove
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx
CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY  For the Case Study assig.docx

More Related Content

Similar to CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY For the Case Study assig.docx

Chapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docx
Chapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docxChapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docx
Chapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docxketurahhazelhurst
 
Develop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docx
Develop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docxDevelop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docx
Develop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docxbradburgess22840
 
InstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docx
InstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docxInstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docx
InstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docxJeniceStuckeyoo
 
BIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project which
BIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project whichBIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project which
BIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project whichsaundersabelard
 
Research Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docx
Research Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docxResearch Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docx
Research Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docxdebishakespeare
 
Hcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.com
Hcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.comHcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.com
Hcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.comamaranthbeg19
 
The discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docx
The discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docxThe discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docx
The discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docxhe45mcurnow
 
Find an or news clip online or.docx
Find an or news clip online or.docxFind an or news clip online or.docx
Find an or news clip online or.docxwrite22
 
Starting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docx
Starting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docxStarting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docx
Starting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docxbryanwest16882
 
1 8Annotated Bib
1                                         8Annotated Bib1                                         8Annotated Bib
1 8Annotated BibVannaJoy20
 
Formulating a literature review
Formulating a literature reviewFormulating a literature review
Formulating a literature reviewLynn Hendricks
 
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)RandalHoffman
 
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)RichardAment
 

Similar to CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY For the Case Study assig.docx (14)

Chapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docx
Chapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docxChapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docx
Chapter 1 Invitation to Biology Chapter Review Book Title Biol.docx
 
Develop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docx
Develop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docxDevelop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docx
Develop a 3–4 page research paper based on a selected case study rel.docx
 
InstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docx
InstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docxInstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docx
InstructionsFinal ProjectThe Final Project should demonstrat.docx
 
BIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project which
BIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project whichBIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project which
BIOTECHNOLOGY PAPER. This quarter, you will complete a project which
 
Research Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docx
Research Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docxResearch Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docx
Research Presentation instructions Research Question andCitation.docx
 
Educator Toolbox
Educator ToolboxEducator Toolbox
Educator Toolbox
 
Hcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.com
Hcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.comHcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.com
Hcs 465 Future Our Mission/newtonhelp.com
 
The discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docx
The discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docxThe discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docx
The discussion assignment provides a forum for discussing releva.docx
 
Find an or news clip online or.docx
Find an or news clip online or.docxFind an or news clip online or.docx
Find an or news clip online or.docx
 
Starting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docx
Starting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docxStarting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docx
Starting the Research ProcessFormulating a specific, applica.docx
 
1 8Annotated Bib
1                                         8Annotated Bib1                                         8Annotated Bib
1 8Annotated Bib
 
Formulating a literature review
Formulating a literature reviewFormulating a literature review
Formulating a literature review
 
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
 
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
Assignment 2 social control and criminal deviance bullying (2)
 

More from drennanmicah

Case Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docx
Case Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docxCase Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docx
Case Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Rubric  Directly respond to each questi.docx
Case Study Rubric   Directly respond to each questi.docxCase Study Rubric   Directly respond to each questi.docx
Case Study Rubric  Directly respond to each questi.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docx
Case Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docxCase Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docx
Case Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docx
Case Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docxCase Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docx
Case Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case study RubricCriterionOutstanding 3.75Very Good 3 .docx
Case study RubricCriterionOutstanding  3.75Very Good  3 .docxCase study RubricCriterionOutstanding  3.75Very Good  3 .docx
Case study RubricCriterionOutstanding 3.75Very Good 3 .docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Rubric .docx
Case Study Rubric                                                 .docxCase Study Rubric                                                 .docx
Case Study Rubric .docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docx
Case Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docxCase Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docx
Case Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docx
Case Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docxCase Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docx
Case Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docx
Case Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docxCase Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docx
Case Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docx
Case Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docxCase Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docx
Case Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docx
Case Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docxCase Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docx
Case Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docxCase Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Project Part I Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docx
Case Study Project Part I   Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docxCase Study Project Part I   Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docx
Case Study Project Part I Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docxCase Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docx
Case Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docxCase Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docx
Case Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docx
Case Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docxCase Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docx
Case Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docx
Case Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docxCase Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docx
Case Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docx
Case Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docxCase Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docx
Case Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docx
Case Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docxCase Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docx
Case Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docxdrennanmicah
 
Case Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docx
Case Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docxCase Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docx
Case Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docxdrennanmicah
 

More from drennanmicah (20)

Case Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docx
Case Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docxCase Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docx
Case Study RubricCriterionStrongAverageWeakInt.docx
 
Case Study Rubric  Directly respond to each questi.docx
Case Study Rubric   Directly respond to each questi.docxCase Study Rubric   Directly respond to each questi.docx
Case Study Rubric  Directly respond to each questi.docx
 
Case Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docx
Case Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docxCase Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docx
Case Study Scenario Part 3IntroductionThis media piece exp.docx
 
Case Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docx
Case Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docxCase Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docx
Case Study RubricYour case study will be assessed as follows•.docx
 
Case study RubricCriterionOutstanding 3.75Very Good 3 .docx
Case study RubricCriterionOutstanding  3.75Very Good  3 .docxCase study RubricCriterionOutstanding  3.75Very Good  3 .docx
Case study RubricCriterionOutstanding 3.75Very Good 3 .docx
 
Case Study Rubric .docx
Case Study Rubric                                                 .docxCase Study Rubric                                                 .docx
Case Study Rubric .docx
 
Case Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docx
Case Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docxCase Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docx
Case Study ReflectionWrite a 4-5 page paper. Your written assi.docx
 
Case Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docx
Case Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docxCase Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docx
Case Study Questions (Each question is worth 6 marks)1. Defi.docx
 
Case Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docx
Case Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docxCase Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docx
Case Study Reorganizing Human Resources at ASP SoftwareRead the.docx
 
Case Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docx
Case Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docxCase Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docx
Case Study Report Rubric CriterionWeakAverageStrongIdent.docx
 
Case Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docx
Case Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docxCase Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docx
Case Study Project (A) Hefty Hardware - Be sure to address each .docx
 
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docxCase Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions (.docx
 
Case Study Project Part I Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docx
Case Study Project Part I   Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docxCase Study Project Part I   Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docx
Case Study Project Part I Declared JurisdictionTemplate Sta.docx
 
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docxCase Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docx
Case Study Proposing a Data Gathering Approach at TLG Solutions.docx
 
Case Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docx
Case Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docxCase Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docx
Case Study Peer Comments In each case study, you are expected.docx
 
Case Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docx
Case Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docxCase Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docx
Case Study ProblemLeadership appears as a popular agenda it.docx
 
Case Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docx
Case Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docxCase Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docx
Case Study Planning for GrowthKelly’s Sandwich Stop is one of t.docx
 
Case Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docx
Case Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docxCase Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docx
Case Study People v. Smith, 470 NW2d 70, Michigan Supreme Court (19.docx
 
Case Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docx
Case Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docxCase Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docx
Case Study OneBMGT 464 Portfolio Activity TwoPurposeIn thi.docx
 
Case Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docx
Case Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docxCase Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docx
Case Study on The Wallace GroupConduct a strategic analysis us.docx
 

Recently uploaded

INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptxINTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptxHumphrey A Beña
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxKarra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxAshokKarra1
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomnelietumpap1
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptxBarangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptxCarlos105
 
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptxmary850239
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...Postal Advocate Inc.
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONHumphrey A Beña
 
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfInclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfTechSoup
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 

Recently uploaded (20)

INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptxINTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
INTRODUCTION TO CATHOLIC CHRISTOLOGY.pptx
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxKarra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptxBarangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
 
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
4.18.24 Movement Legacies, Reflection, and Review.pptx
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE GOT EMAIL_FINALS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdfInclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
Inclusivity Essentials_ Creating Accessible Websites for Nonprofits .pdf
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 

CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY For the Case Study assig.docx

  • 1. CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY For the Case Study assignment the current pathogen selections may be requested by sending an email to your instructor! Assigned Case Study Problem: You will create a case study for a microbial infection selected from the current pathogen list. Your case study will be assembled using a detailed rubric (see below). Upon completion, you will submit your case study to the Blackboard gradebook in Unit 5 and to SafeAssign. How to create a case study The case studies are meant to be an enjoyable, interesting, and informative assignment. This is your chance to show that you understand the key teaching points about a microbe and to communicate these points in a written format. What information belongs in my case study? Have at least 3-4 key referenced points in each of the five areas shown in the Case Study Information Chart (see below). The left-hand heading in the chart suggests the type of information requested for the pathogen. Outlines can be in whatever form you prefer (bullets/charts/outlines/diagrams or a mix). Be sure to include two discussion questions (and provide complete answers) that you can incorporate into your case study (place them at the end of your write-up).
  • 2. These questions should help connect your case to other material in the course. For example, what other microbes have an A-B toxin? What other viruses are transmitted by fecal-oral spread? How much information should I provide for my case study? For the Case Study, you are asked to provide at least the information requested in the chart below. The boxed questions are suggestions for the minimum amount of information within each category. The more detailed the information, the better the study. You may consult your textbook, CDC, WHO, Access Medicine, Google Scholar, NCBI, WebMD, etc. to find the information. For example, if you perform a Google search using the name of the pathogen and the word ‘vaccine’, you will find information on current vaccines (if any), those in clinical trials, vaccines used only in animals, etc. Case Study Information Chart Typical Case What does a typical case look like? Use the standard format for a patient presentation with chief complaint (CC), history of present illness (HPI), key physical exam details (PE), lab findings, signature signs, and any other important findings. Description of the infectious agent If it is a bacterium, how is it classified? If it is a virus, what kind of
  • 3. nucleic acid does it have? Does it target specific cellular types (tropism)? Does it form a spore? Is it aerobic? Is it intracellular? Can it only be grown in a specific type of media? How is it distinguished from other members of the species? Does the pathogen have a significant history with humans or animals? Epidemiology What do you feel are the most important points about the epidemiology of the disease? Incidence? Portal of entry? Source? Is it a normal microbiota component in the human body? Does it only occur in certain populations or certain geographical areas? Is there a vector involved? What and who is the vector? Is it zoonotic? Does it appear CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY seasonally? Are there currently any outbreaks of this pathogen? What is its ecological niche? Is there a reservoir? Are there currently any outbreaks or epidemics of disease from this pathogen? Pathogenesis What is the range of diseases caused by the agent? What organs are affected? What symptoms might the patient have? What is the disease course? Will the patient recover? Are there any long-term
  • 4. sequelae of infection? Latency? Prophylaxis/Treatment Is there an antitoxin? Specific antibiotics or a class of antibiotics that are used? Is there a vaccine available? Is treatment curative? Does infection make you immune? Is this immunity life-long? Is there drug resistance? Are there novel treatments? Discussion Questions With Responses Two (2) discussion questions with responses and scientific literature references are required to complete the Case Study; These questions should help connect your case to other material in the course. For example, what other microbes have an A-B toxin? What other viruses are transmitted by fecal-oral spread? Is this pathogen a candidate for use as a biological weapon? How should I format my references for my case study? You will need references for all factual information in your case study. Pertinent references can be listed at the bottom of your outline in a small font. Your references must include at least two current peer- reviewed publications from the scientific literature post-2010. Use APA Sixth Edition Style format for all references (a PDF document is provided with the case study directions).
  • 5. For example, your text book in-text citation would look like this (Anderson, Salm, & Allen, 2016) and your textbook would be shown on your reference page as: Anderson, D., Salm, S., & Allen, D. (2016). Microbiology: A Human Perspective. New York: McGraw-Hill. Addenda 1. The written case presentations are submitted through Blackboard in Unit 5 of the course. When you reach Unit 5, be sure to e-mail your instructor to receive a list of the Case Study selections. 2. Please name the files in the following manner “LastnameFirstinitial CS Y”. For example, if I am submitting a Case Study on tularemia, I would name the case study file “Frisardi M CS tularemia”. This will ensure that you receive appropriate credit and that I will be able to find your Case Study document easily when grading. Be sure to provide the two (2) required discussion questions (as well as provide answers) with your case study. 3. Your library privileges from UNE give you access to all of the scientific literature so you are able to find references, abstracts, and complete articles in most cases. Your library privileges from UNE also give you access to the medical textbooks. Do this: http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources
  • 6. Resources (the page will be here: http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources/databases-title ) http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources http://www.une.edu/library/e-resources/databases-title CASE STUDY RUBRIC MICROBIOLOGY it. -in page and you may use your UNE e-mail log-in information. bar in the middle of the page. Type in your Case Study pathogen and get started. 4. Finally, use Google Scholar. Make a noun string of your pathogen’s Latin name and a qualifying term like “vaccines” or “virulence factors”. The site is: http://scholar.google.com/ Be sure to consider the directions carefully and include all the requested information (especially the two discussion questions with responses and your scientific literature references). By the time you provide all information for your pathogen (including any figures, illustrations, maps) the case study will be several pages in length. Use APA format (or
  • 7. any format you are used to), in-text citations, and a reference list at the end. So: • Read the rubric carefully, provide all information, discussion questions/responses, and references in APA format. • Submit to SafeAssign to check that you have referenced everything and avoid any inadvertent plagiarism, then Remember that the Case Study is a teaching tool and you are, essentially, teaching me about the pathogen you selected. http://scholar.google.com/ Running Head: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators Title: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators Student’s Name:
  • 8. Institution: Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators According to the chapters on PAS, the conceptual framework has been effectively presented with the main points analyzed in an effective manner. The conceptual framework of the chapters on PAS has analyzed the challenges experienced in the learning institutions and the manner in which the challenges should be addressed to bring about the required success in the schools. However, the conceptual framework has given me a chance to identify evaluation of the challenges as a model to address the problems experienced. This model will make it easy for me to understand the problems experienced hence coming up with a strategy in which problems are addressed in an effective manner. The working title of my PAS is the challenges of charter schools and the public schools: perceptions of Teachers and the administrators. This is because different challenges are encountered when running the charter schools as well as the public schools in a country. For the purposes of ensuring that both the charter schools and the public schools do not experience the problems, different strategies should be put in place by the government o protect the learning institutions from the challenges experienced in their efforts to teach the young people in the society. The teachers and the administrators in the learning institutions should change their perceptions and hence develop mechanisms in which the challenges are overcome. However, the challenges
  • 9. should be addressed in a way that does not affect the learning of the students in both the charter schools and the public schools. The challenges experienced by the teacher, administrators and the learners in the learning institutions should be analyzed and hence the right solutions identified for the purposes of making the learning and the teaching process effective. The administrators in both the charter schools and the public schools should use their ability in addressing the challenges experienced by the teachers and the learners. This is because they have the authority to carry out different activities for the sake of bringing about the success of the charter schools and the public schools. Moreover, consultations should be carried so as to ensure that the charter schools and the public schools do not experience the challenges as they might bring about the unnecessary failures n place especially when not effectively controlled. (Dunn, 2018) The stakeholders in charter schools and the public schools should be consulted before different solutions are achieved as they might give out ideas which might help in the solving and addressing the challenges in an effective manner. Moreover, it is the responsibility of the stakeholders to support the charter schools and the public schools so as to ensure that the learners receive a quality education. (Cookson, 2018) As a result, both material support and psychological support is required to enable the administrators and teachers to carry out different activities for the success of the learning institution. The support provided by the stakeholders will help in addressing some of the challenges hence enabling the administrators to come up with different projects which will make the learning process easy. Finally, the challenges experienced should be addressed in time before they affect the learning process of the students. (Maranto, 2018) As far as the research questions are concerned, some of the research questions identified are: · What should be done to reduce the challenges experienced in both charter schools and the public schools?
  • 10. · What are the roles of the teachers and the administrators in addressing of the challenges experienced in the learning institutions? · How have the perceptions of the teachers and the administrators changed as a result of the challenges experienced in both the charter schools and the public schools? · What efforts have been carried so far to have the problems addressed? References Cookson, P. (2018). Expect miracles: Charter schools and the politics of hope and despair. Routledge. Dunn, M. E., Katsiyannis, A., & Ryan, J. B. (2018). Charter schools and students with disabilities: legal and practical considerations. Intervention in School and Clinic, 53(4), 252- 255. Maranto, R. (2018). The death of one best way: Charter schools as reinventing government. In School choice in the real world (pp. 39-57). Routledge.
  • 11. Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators Name of student Institution Date Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators The topic that I will be handling in this study is “Challenges of Charter Schools and Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators.” This therefore, is the working title of my Professional Administrative study (PAS). To pursue this topic in detail, I believe that the application of the qualitative approach will be key in ensuring that the study obtains more information from the teachers and administrators who will be the respondents. I believe that application of a theory will also be key in establishing relationships between various variables in the study design. The reason why a study has to rely on a theory is because “theories and conceptual frameworks are developed to account for or describe abstract phenomena that occur under similar conditions” (Rudestam & Newton, 2015). We expect that the responses that are given by the participants and which make a key part of the findings of the study should be reproducible under similar conditions. The problem statement in this study is that there are many challenges that charter schools and public schools face and the perceptions of teachers and administrators are key in reaching a solution. According to Rudestam & Newton, 2015, a professional scientific study should be based on a method of inquiry that a community of scholars have tested and conformed to be reliable and valid. It is in this connection that I chose to use the qualitative approach because its scientifically proven. Another advantage of using this approach lies in the fact that I
  • 12. will be able to get detailed responses which I believe will be in a descriptive manner. I would also rely on an exploratory model to ensure that I have the best methods for data collection with an aim of minimising the possibility of bias. According to Rudestam & Newton 2015, “The research question is the precisely stated form of the researcher’s intent and may be accompanied by one or more specific hypotheses”. Looking at the study topic in question, it is important to consider the use of short interviews and factor the necessity of an exploratory element with theoretical implications. The research questions that will govern the study include the following; i. What are the three major challenges that charter and public schools face? ii. In the view of teachers which are the three greatest challenges facing charter and public schools? iii. In the view of administrators which are the three greatest challenges facing charter and public schools? iv. What should be done to rectify the situation? References Rudestam, K. E., & Newton, R. R. (2015). Surviving your dissertation: A comprehensive guide to content and process. Sage Publications. This is a 4 page paper. The PAS Prospectus is a preliminary document that serves as the foundation for the proposal document for the PAS. It is a relatively short document and should not exceed approximately 6 pages of text, excluding appendices and references. This paper is a qualitative study using a case study, using a survey questionnaireNOT interviews or observations. · Title Page(The qualitative study of Charter Schools vs. Public Schools: Perceptions of Teachers and Administrators)
  • 13. · Problem Statement(What is the problem that needs to be solved from the title page) · Purpose Statement(What am I going to solve the problem? How am I going to identify it from the problem statement)? · Nature of the Study · Research Question(s)(Must align with the problem statement and purpose statement) · Conceptual Framework · Significance of the Study · References(Need to be included in the body of the paper and in the reference section as well) 1 Surviving Your Dissertation 4 2 Surviving Your Dissertation A Comprehensive Guide to Content and Process 4 Kjell Erik Rudestam Fielding Graduate University Rae R. Newton Fielding Graduate University
  • 14. 3 FOR INFORMATION: SAGE Publications, Inc. 2455 Teller Road Thousand Oaks, California 91320 E-mail: [email protected] SAGE Publications Ltd. 1 Oliver’s Yard 55 City Road London EC1Y 1SP United Kingdom SAGE Publications India Pvt. Ltd. B 1/I 1 Mohan Cooperative Industrial Area Mathura Road, New Delhi 110 044 India SAGE Publications Asia-Pacific Pte. Ltd. 3 Church Street
  • 15. #10-04 Samsung Hub Singapore 049483 4 Copyright © 2015 by SAGE Publications, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Printed in the United States of America Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Rudestam, Kjell Erik. Surviving your dissertation : a comprehensive guide to content and process / Kjell Erik Rudestam, Fielding Graduate University, Rae R. Newton, Fielding Graduate University. —Fourth edition. pages cm. Includes bibliographical references. ISBN 978-1-4522-6097-6 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Dissertations, Academic—United States. 2. Report writing. 3.
  • 16. Research—United States. I. Newton, Rae R. II. Title. LB2369.R83 2015 378.2—dc23 2014002905 This book is printed on acid-free paper. 14 15 16 17 18 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Acquisitions Editor: Vicki Knight Editorial Assistant: Yvonne McDuffee Production Editors: Laura Barrett, David C. Felts Copy Editor: Paula L. Fleming Typesetter: C&M Digitals (P) Ltd. Proofreader: Sarah J. Duffy Indexer: Joan Shapiro 5 Cover Designer: Rose Storey Marketing Manager: Nicole Elliott 6
  • 17. Contents Preface About the Authors Part I: Getting Started 1. The Research Process 2. Selecting a Suitable Topic 3. Methods of Inquiry: Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches Part II: Working With Content: The Dissertation Chapters 4. Literature Review and Statement of the Problem 5. The Method Chapter: Describing Your Research Plan 6. Presenting the Results of Quantitative Research 7. Presenting the Results of Qualitative Research 8. Discussion Part III: Working With Process: What You Need to Know to Make the Dissertation Easier 9. Overcoming Barriers: Becoming an Expert While Controlling Your Own Destiny 10. Writing 11. How to Complete Your Dissertation Using Online Data Access and Collection 12. Guidelines for the Presentation of Numbers in the Dissertation 13. Informed Consent and Other Ethical Concerns References Name Index Subject Index 7
  • 18. Preface We are pleased to present the fourth edition of Surviving Your Dissertation. As with previous editions, we have sought to answer questions that students and faculty have at every stage of the dissertation process. In past editions, we have illustrated the challenge of engaging in such a notable project with book covers that depict a bridge leading into an impenetrable jungle and life buoys close at hand to negotiate the stormy seas. The cover of this edition offers a different, but equally relevant image: the pride of victory achieved by scaling a lofty peak. In many ways, these images also reflect our own experience in writing the book. The field of research in the social and behavioral sciences has expanded rapidly over the past several years, and we have frequently felt as if we are scrambling to keep up. This edition reflects our experience. We have maintained the overall structure of the book, which has been well received so far, while updating content on those topics that are indispensable to the dissertation process: the selection of an appropriate research topic; the review of the literature; the description of the methodology and research design; the collection and analysis of data; and the interpretation, presentation, and discussion of the results
  • 19. and implications of the study. Within this updated material, we have once again attempted to provide sufficient detail to enable the reader to know exactly what goes into each section and chapter of the dissertation and how to format that information. In addition, we continue to include topics that are not always present in sources of this kind: the many types of quantitative and qualitative research models and approaches that are available to the student, the principles of good scholarly and academic writing, suggestions for how to select and work with committees, and tips for overcoming task and emotional blocks that may impede progress. Throughout, we have replaced older references with newer, more contemporary ones, including many new dissertation examples taken from our students and colleagues. We have also added significant new content to the fourth edition. We note that the traditional null hypothesis significance-testing model is being challenged and augmented by an emphasis on clinical or practical significance and a corresponding use of measures of effect size and 8
  • 20. confidence intervals. We have explained and illustrated this new approach to the presentation of statistical results. Similarly, we have acknowledged an emerging emphasis on theoretical “models” and the influence of model building on the design and presentation of research. We provide several examples of studies that incorporate this approach. We are increasingly impressed by the implications of the Internet for the entire research enterprise. We have expanded our discussion of the Internet as a source of data, an opportunity for data collection, and a vehicle for data analysis, as well as providing recommendations of potentially helpful websites and software programs that may be unfamiliar to the average reader. We also acknowledge the parallel expansion of available data sources in all their varied forms, including data archives, social media, and what is currently known as “big data.” Finally, we have expanded the sections on qualitative and multimodal methods of research, which have an inductive, theory- building focus. With respect to all these topics, we have tried to explain the concepts, illustrate them with new tables and figures, and, in many cases, provide very specific details about how to incorporate them into a research study.
  • 21. We believe that this book is suitable for a large academic and professional audience. Of course, it is primarily directed at the graduate student who envisions or is involved in writing a research dissertation. Thus, there is significant focus on material that is best suited for the beginning doctoral student—for example, how to develop a research question, how to construct a table or figure, how to report a statistical finding, how to use American Psychological Association formatting conventions, and so on. However, there is also content directed at the more advanced student—for example, how to conceptualize and illustrate a mediation model, how to report multiple regression findings, and how to code text for a grounded theory study. Moreover, we have become increasingly aware that Surviving Your Dissertation serves also as a resource for researchers and practitioners who have either forgotten important details or are motivated to keep abreast of evolving research practices in their fields. Perhaps more important, we view the book as a convenient source of information for faculty who are currently supervising graduate students’ dissertations or research projects. We remain deeply indebted to our own students, who continue
  • 22. to thrill us with their creativity and force us to keep learning in order to stay a step ahead. We hope that they, and you, find this volume a helpful and steady 9 companion in your research and writing endeavors. A large number of individuals have contributed to the completion of this project. We called upon many faculty colleagues to nominate student dissertations that exemplify high levels of scholarship and have sprinkled references to these dissertations throughout the book to illustrate important principles and recommendations. We are very appreciative of these relatively recent graduates for allowing us to share their first major research endeavors in this venue. We also benefitted from the critical reflections and insight of the following reviewers of the third edition of Surviving Your Dissertation: Anne J. Hacker, Bernie Kerr, Karin Klenke, Kaye Pepper, and Udaya R. Wagle. Their observations and suggestions were both reinforcing and helpful in crystallizing changes for this edition. We are also grateful to be part of the SAGE family, a
  • 23. collaborative, dedicated group of professionals who have facilitated our writing careers in so many ways. The following individuals were notable contributors: Vicki Knight, publisher and senior editor, has always provided us with a balance of inspiring leadership and nurturing support. Her editorial assistant, Jessica Miller, has been consistently responsive to our frequent requests for assistance. Laura Barrett and David Felts, project editors, have gracefully guided the editorial process from start to finish. And Paula Fleming, our copy editor, is truly a paragon in her craft. Her grammatical acuity, common sense, and work ethic cannot be overestimated. Thank you all. Finally, we must thank our partners in life, Jan and Kathy, for their continuing patience and support as we have devoted our energy and attention to four editions of this volume. 10 About the Authors Kjell Erik Rudestam is Professor of Psychology at Fielding Graduate University, Santa
  • 24. Barbara, California, where he served as Associate Dean of Academic Affairs for many years. He was previously a psychology professor at York University, Toronto, and Miami University, Oxford, Ohio, after receiving his PhD in Psychology (Clinical) from the University of Oregon. He is the author of Your Statistical Consultant: Answers to Your Data Analysis Questions, 2nd edition (also with Rae R. Newton), Handbook of Online Learning, 2nd edition (with Judith Schoenholtz-Read), and eight other books, as well as numerous articles in professional journals on topics including suicide, psychotherapy, and family and organizational systems. He is a Fellow of the American Psychological Association (Division 12), a Diplomate of the American Board of Examiners in Professional Psychology (Clinical), a Diplomate of the American Academy of Experts in Traumatic Stress, and holds an Honorary Doctorate of Science from The Professional School of Psychology. Rae R. Newton is Professor of Sociology Emeritus at California State University, Fullerton. He recently joined the faculty of the School of Psychology at Fielding Graduate University where he serves as a research consultant and statistical advisor to doctoral students and faculty. He received his PhD in sociology from the University of California, Santa Barbara, and completed postdoctoral training in mental health
  • 25. measurement at Indiana University. His primary interests include longitudinal modeling of outcomes for high risk youth and foster care populations, family violence and statistics education. He is author, with Kjell Erik Rudestam, of Your Statistical Consultant: Answers to Your Data Analysis Questions, now in its second edition and numerous articles in professional journals on topics including family violence, child maltreatment, and measurement. In semi- retirement he enjoys traveling with his wife in their RV and surfing throughout Mexico and Central America. 11 Part I Getting Started The Research Process Selecting a Suitable Topic Methods of Inquiry: Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches 12 1 The Research Process There is a story about a Zen Buddhist who took a group of monks into the forest. The group soon lost its way. Presently one of the monks
  • 26. asked the leader where they were going. The wise man answered, “To the deepest, darkest part of the forest so that we can all find our way out together.” Doctoral research for the graduate student in the social sciences is often just such an experience—trekking into a forest of impenetrable density and making many wrong turns. Over the years, our students have used various metaphors to describe the dissertation process, metaphors that convey the feeling of being lost in the wilderness. One student compared the process to the Sisyphean struggle of reaching the top of a hill, only to discover the presence of an even higher mountain behind it. Another student experienced the task as learning a Martian language, known to the natives who composed her committee but entirely foreign to her. A third student offered perhaps the best description when she suggested that it was like waiting patiently in a seemingly interminable line to gain admission to a desirable event, then finally reaching the front only to be told to return to the rear of the line. One reason that students become more exasperated than necessary on the dissertation journey is that they fail to understand the procedures and practices that form the foundation for contemporary social science
  • 27. research. Many students who are attracted to their field of interest out of an applied concern are apprehensive about making the leap from application to theory, an indispensable part of the research enterprise. What may not be so evident is that many of the skills that go into being a consummate practitioner are the same ones demanded of a capable researcher. It is well known that curiosity and hypothesis testing are the bedrock of empirical research. In a similar fashion, experienced psychotherapists, to take an example from clinical psychology, are sensitive and keen observers of client behavior. They are persistent hypothesis testers. They are curious about the relationship between family history variables and current functioning. They draw on theory and experience to help select a particular intervention for a particular client problem or moment in therapy. Dispassionate logic and clear and organized thinking are as necessary for effectiveness in the field as they are for success in research. In fact, the 13 bridge between research and just plain living is much shorter than most
  • 28. people think. All of us gather data about the world around us, wonder what will happen if we or others behave in particular ways, and test our pet hunches through deliberate action. To a large extent, the formal research enterprise consists of thinking systematically about these same issues. The procedures outlined in this book are intended to assist the doctoral student in planning and writing a research dissertation, but the suggestions are equally applicable to writing a master’s thesis. In fact, there is considerable overlap between these two challenging activities. For most students, the master’s thesis is the first rigorous research project they attempt. This means that, in the absence of strong, supportive faculty consultation, the student often concludes the thesis with considerable relief and an awareness of how not to do the study the next time! With a doctoral dissertation, it is generally expected, sometimes as an act of faith, that the student is a more seasoned and sophisticated researcher. The consensus opinion is that dissertations are generally longer than theses, that they are more original, that they rely more heavily on theoretically based arguments, and that they make a greater contribution to the field. In most graduate programs, the prelude to conducting a
  • 29. dissertation study is presenting a dissertation proposal. A research proposal is an action plan that justifies and describes the proposed study. The completion of a comprehensive proposal is a very important step in the dissertation process. The proposal serves as a contract between the student and his or her dissertation or thesis committee that, when approved by all parties, constitutes an agreement that data may be collected and the study may be completed. As long as the student follows the steps outlined in the proposal, committee members should be discouraged from demanding significant changes to the study after the proposal has been approved. Naturally, it is not uncommon to expect small changes, additions, or deletions as the study progresses because one can never totally envision the unpredictable turns that research can take. There is no universally agreed-on format for the research proposal. To our way of thinking, a good proposal contains a review of the relevant literature, a statement of the problem and the associated hypotheses, and a clear delineation of the proposed method and plan for data analysis. In our experience, an approved proposal means that a significant percentage of the work on the dissertation has been completed. As such, this
  • 30. book is 14 intended to help students construct research proposals as well as complete dissertations. The Research Wheel One way to think about the phases of the research process is with reference to the so-called research wheel (see Figure 1.1). The wheel metaphor suggests that research is not linear but is rather a recursive cycle of steps. The most common entry point is some form of empirical observation. In other words, the researcher selects a topic from the infinite array of possible topics. The next step is a process of inductive logic that culminates in a proposition. The inductive process serves to relate the specific topic to a broader context and begins with some hunches in the form “I wonder if . . . .” These hunches typically are guided by the values, assumptions, and goals of the researcher, which need to be explicated. Figure 1.1 The Research Wheel Stage 2 of the research wheel is a developed proposition, which is
  • 31. expressed as a statement of an established relationship (e.g., “the early bird is more likely than the late bird to catch the worm”). The proposition exists within a conceptual or theoretical framework. The role of the researcher is to clarify the relationship between a particular proposition and the broader context of theory and previous research. This is probably the most challenging and creative aspect of the dissertation process. Theories and conceptual frameworks are developed to account for or describe abstract phenomena that occur under similar conditions. A theory 15 is the language that allows researchers to move from observation to observation and make sense of similarities and differences. A conceptual framework, which is simply a less-developed form of a theory, consists of statements that link abstract concepts (e.g., motivation, role) to empirical data. If not placed within such a context, the proposed study has a “So what?” quality. This is one of the main objections to the research proposals of novice researchers: The research question may be inherently interesting but ultimately meaningless. For instance, the question “Are
  • 32. there more women than men in graduate school today?” is entirely banal as a research question unless the answer has conceptual or theoretical implications that are developed within the study. Although a study may be worthwhile primarily for its practical implications (e.g., “Should we start recruiting more men into graduate schools?”), a purely applied study may not be acceptable as a dissertation. Kerlinger and Lee (1999), authors of a highly respected text on research methodology, noted that “the basic purpose of scientific research is theory” (p. 5). Generally speaking, a research dissertation is expected to contribute to the scholarly literature in a field and not merely solve an applied problem. Thus, developing a proposition for one’s dissertation typically involves immersing oneself in the research and theoretical literature of the field to identify a conceptual framework for the study. Having stated our position on the role of theory in dissertation research, we now need to take a step back. As a psychologist and a sociologist, respectively, we are most familiar with research conventions within these two disciplines. Other branches of the social sciences have their own standards of what constitutes an acceptable dissertation topic.
  • 33. We have attempted to keep this book as generalizable as possible and to infuse it with examples from other fields. Ultimately, of course, you will need to follow the rules and conventions that pertain to your discipline as well as to your university and department. For example, a few major universities allow a doctoral student to submit one or more published articles as the equivalent of a dissertation. Many others encourage studies that consist of secondary data analyses derived from national databases, such as U.S. Census data or the General Social Survey, or data obtained from a larger study. Some fields— notably social work, education, policy evaluation, and professional psychology—may encourage dissertations that solve applied problems rather than make 16 distinct theoretical contributions. Studies that evaluate the effectiveness of programs or interventions are a case in point because they sometimes contribute little to validating a theory. Political science and economics are examples of fields that are diverse enough to accommodate both theoretically based studies and purely applied studies. Within
  • 34. the subspecialty of international relations, for instance, one could imagine a survey and analysis of security agreements of European nations after the unraveling of the North Atlantic Treaty Organization (NATO) that rely on interviews with foreign policy makers and are largely descriptive and applied. In contrast, a study of the role of a commitment to ideology in the success of political parties in the United States, based on an analysis of historical documents and voting records, might be grounded in a theory of how ideology attracts or alienates the voting public. Moving forward along the research wheel, the researcher uses deductive reasoning to move from the larger context of theory to generate a specific research question. The research question is the precisely stated form of the researcher’s intent and may be accompanied by one or more specific hypotheses. The first loop is completed as the researcher seeks to discover or collect the data that will serve to answer the research question. The data collection process is essentially another task of empirical observation, which then initiates another round of the research wheel. Generalizations are made on the basis of the particular data that have been observed (inductive process), and the generalizations are tied to a
  • 35. conceptual framework, which then leads to the elucidation of further research questions and implications for additional study. The kinds of skills called for at the various points of the research wheel are reminiscent of the thoughts about learning presented by Bertrand Russell many years ago. Russell noted that there are two primary kinds of knowledge acquisition: knowledge by description and knowledge by acquaintance. Knowledge by description is learning in a passive mode, such as by reading a book on how to change the oil in one’s car or hearing a lecture on Adam Smith’s theory of economics. This type of learning is especially well suited to mastering abstract information; in other words, it is better for learning about economics than about changing the oil. Knowledge by acquaintance, on the other hand, is learning by doing—the kind of skill training that comes from practicing a tennis serve, driving an automobile, and playing with a computer. This is concrete knowledge acquisition, oriented to solving problems. 17 The research process demands both skills. First, the researcher
  • 36. needs to apply clear, logical thinking to working with concepts and ideas and building theories. It is our impression that many graduate students, particularly those who have experience as practitioners in their fields, are weaker in this abstract conceptualization, and honing this skill may be the major challenge of the dissertation. Second, the researcher must engage in the practical application of ideas, including by systematically planning a study and then collecting and analyzing data. The ability to focus, problem solve, and make decisions will help bring the study to completion. 18 2 Selecting a Suitable Topic The selection of an appropriate topic is the first major challenge in conducting research. In many academic settings, this task is simplified by working with a faculty mentor who is already familiar with an interesting area of study, may have an extensive program of research in that area, and may even have defined one or more researchable questions. It is quite common for students interested in a particular area of research to not only
  • 37. select their doctoral institution but also select their dissertation chair, with the goal of joining the research program of a noted scholar in that field. On the other hand, you may not be blessed with a faculty role model who is actively engaged in research in an area of interest to you. There are no simple rules for selecting a topic of interest, but there are some considerations with respect to appropriateness. It is generally unwise to define something as important as a dissertation topic without first obtaining a broad familiarity with the field. This implies a large amount of exploring the literature and studying the experts. Without this initial exploration, you can neither know the range of possibilities of interesting topics nor have a clear idea of what is already known. Most students obtain their research topics from the loose ends they discover in reading within an area, from an interesting observation they have made (“I notice that men shut up when a beautiful woman enters the room; I wonder what the effect of physical attractiveness is on group process?”), or from an applied focus in their lives or professional work (“I have a difficult time treating these alcoholics and want to discover how best to work with them”). In short, there is no substitute for immersing oneself in a field of study by having
  • 38. conversations with leading scholars, advisers, and peers; critically reading the existing literature; and reflecting upon the implications of professional and personal experiences. Some Guidelines for Topic Selection Here are some guidelines for deciding whether a topic is appropriate as a dissertation subject. 1. A topic needs to sustain your interest over a long period of time. A 19 study on learning nonsense syllables under two sets of environmental conditions may sound appealing in its simplicity, but remember Finagle’s first law of research: If something can go wrong, it will go wrong! Dissertations usually take at least twice as much time as anticipated, and there are few worse fates than slaving for hour after hour on a project that you abhor. Remember, too, that all dissertations are recorded and published by the Library of Congress, and you will always be associated with your particular study. 2. At the other extreme, it is wise to avoid a topic that is overly ambitious
  • 39. and overly challenging. Most students want to graduate, preferably within a reasonable period of time. Grandiose dissertations have a way of never being completed, and even the best dissertations end up being compromises among your own ambition, the wishes of your committee, and practical circumstances. It is not realistic for a dissertation to say everything there is to say about a particular topic (e.g., the European Union), and you need to temper your enthusiasm with pragmatism. As one student put it, “There are two types of dissertations: the great ones and those that are completed!” Sometimes it makes sense to select a research topic on the basis of convenience or workability and use the luxury of the postgraduate years to pursue more esoteric topics of personal interest. 3. We suggest that you avoid topics that may be linked too closely with emotional issues in your own life. It always makes sense to choose a topic that is interesting and personally meaningful. Some students, however, try to use a dissertation to resolve an emotional issue or solve a personal problem. For example, even if you think you have successfully overcome the personal impact of the death of your child, this is a topic to be avoided. It will necessarily stir up emotional issues that may get in the way of
  • 40. completing the dissertation. 4. A related issue is selecting a topic in which you have a personal ax to grind. Remember that conducting research demands ruthless honesty and objectivity. If you initiate a study to demonstrate that men are no damned good, you will be able neither to allow yourself the sober reflections of good research nor to acknowledge the possibility that your conclusions may contradict your expectations. It is much better to begin with a hunch (“I’ve noticed that men don’t do very well with housekeeping; I wonder if that has something to do with being pampered as children”) and to regard the research as an adventurous exploration to shed light on this topic rather 20 than as a polemical exercise to substantiate your point of view. 5. Finally, you need to select a topic that has the potential to make an original contribution to the field and allow you to demonstrate your independent mastery of subject and method. In other words, the topic must be worth pursuing. At the very least, the study must generate or help validate theoretical understanding in an area or, in those fields
  • 41. where applied dissertations are permissible, contribute to the development of professional practice. Some students are put off when they discover that a literature review contains contradictory or puzzling results or explanations for a phenomenon. However, such contradictions should be taken not as reasons to steer away from a topic but rather as opportunities to resolve a mystery. When people disagree or when existing explanations seem inadequate, there is often room for a critical study to be conducted. An opportunity to design a study that resolves theoretical contradictions within a discipline should not be overlooked. From Interesting Idea to Research Question Let us assume that you have identified a general area of research and that your choice is based on curiosity and may involve resolving a problem, explaining a phenomenon, uncovering a process by which something occurs, demonstrating the truth of a hidden fact, building on or reevaluating other studies, or testing some theory in your field. To know whether or not the topic is important (significant), you must be familiar with the literature in the area. In Chapter 4, we present a number of suggestions for conducting a good review and assessment of the literature. In the meantime, we have noticed that many students have
  • 42. difficulty transforming an interesting idea into a researchable question, and we have designed a simple exercise to help in that endeavor. Researchable questions almost invariably involve a relationship between two or more variables, phenomena, concepts, or ideas. The nature of that relationship may vary. Research studies generally consist of methods to explicate the nature of the relationship. Research in the social sciences rarely consists of explicating a single construct (e.g., “I will look at everything there is to know about the ‘imposter phenomenon’”) or a single variable (e.g., voting rates in presidential elections).1 Even the presence of two variables is apt to be limiting, and oftentimes it is only when a third 21 “connecting” variable is invoked that an idea becomes researchable. As a caveat, however, we acknowledge that research questions that are qualitative rather than quantitative in nature might not be as focused on the relationship between variables as on “how” processes develop or are experienced. We will have more to say about this distinction in forthcoming chapters.
  • 43. An example might help to demonstrate how the introduction of an additional variable can lead to the birth of a promising study. Let us assume that I am interested in how members of a younger generation perceive the elderly. At this level, a study would be rather mundane and likely to lead to a “So what?” response. So far, it implies asking people what they think of the elderly, perhaps using interviews or tests or even behavioral observations. But we really won’t learn much of value about the nature of perceptions of the elderly in contemporary society and what influences those perceptions. Introducing a second variable, however, can lead to a set of questions that have promising theoretical (as well as practical) implications: I wonder what the role of the media is in shaping social perceptions of the elderly? I wonder if living with a grandparent makes any difference in how the elderly are viewed? I wonder how specific legislation designed to benefit the elderly has changed our perception of them? I wonder if there is a relationship between how middle-aged adults deal with their aging parents and how they view the elderly? The new variables introduced in these potential research questions are, respectively, the slant of the media, presence or absence of
  • 44. a grandparent, type of legislation, and treatment of one’s own parents. These variables impart meaning to the research because they offer suggestions as to what accounts for variability in perceptions of the elderly. As an example of generating a research question using three primary variables, let’s say that you have inferred that many women lose interest in sexual relations with their husbands after the birth of a child. At this level, the proposed study would consist of checking out this hunch by assessing the sexual interest of women (Variable 1) before and after childbirth (Variable 2). But what would this finding mean? The introduction of a third variable or construct could lead to a much more sophisticated and conceptually meaningful study. An investigator might ask, “I wonder if the partner’s involvement in parenting makes a difference? What’s the role of his sexual initiative? How about childbirth complications? Father’s involvement in the birthing? The length of time they have been married? 22 Presence of other children in the home? Mother’s level of fatigue? Her
  • 45. body image?” There is no end to the number of interesting questions that can be raised simply by introducing another variable into the proposed study. This variable would then help to explain the nature of the relationship between the primary variables. In fact, one could brainstorm a whole list of third variables that could contribute to a better understanding of the relationship between childbirth and sexuality. Note that the precise function of the third, or connecting, variable depends on the logic of the conceptual model or theory underlying the study. In this regard, a distinction can be made between two terms, mediator and moderator, which play important roles in research questions. A moderator variable pinpoints the conditions under which an independent variable exerts its effects on a dependent variable. Strictly speaking, a moderator effect is an interaction effect in which the influence of one variable depends upon the level of another variable (Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004). One commonly employed moderator variable is gender, which has two levels, male and female. The relationship between provocation and aggression, for example, may be very different for men and women. The role of context can also be conceptualized as a moderator variable. The famous Kinsey report on sexual behavior would certainly have
  • 46. generated very different results if the interviews with participants about their sex lives had taken place in the presence of family members. Identification of relevant contextual variables has important implications for the design of a study because such variables will affect the generalizability of research findings. A mediating variable, on the other hand, tries to describe how or why rather than when or for whom effects will occur by accounting for the relationship between the independent variable (the predictor) and the dependent variable (the criterion). The mediator is the mechanism through which the predictor affects the outcome.2 As such, one can think of mediators as process variables. For example, in the counseling psychology field, maladaptive perfectionism can be regarded as either a moderating variable or a mediating variable (Wei, Mallinckrodt, Russell, & Abraham, 2004). Conceptualized as a moderator, attachment anxiety could be seen to exert negative effects on depressive mood only under conditions of high maladaptive perfectionism (i.e., there is a statistical interaction between maladaptive perfectionism and attachment anxiety). Conceptualized as a mediator, maladaptive perfectionism acts as an intervening
  • 47. variable 23 between attachment anxiety and depressive mood (i.e., there is an indirect relationship between anxiety and depression). As Wei et al. stated, It is possible for maladaptive perfectionism to serve as both an intermediate link in the causal chain leading from attachment insecurity to depressive mood (i.e., as a mediator) and as a variable that alters the strength of association between attachment insecurity and depressive mood (i.e., as a moderator). (p. 203) The diagram in Figure 2.1 captures the distinction between moderating and mediating variables in a theoretical model. In the case of mediation, the mediating variable (maladaptive perfectionism) is placed between attachment anxiety and depressed mood. In the case of moderation, the arrow from maladaptive perfectionism points to another arrow, that from attachment anxiety to depressed mood, indicating that the relationship between attachment anxiety and depressed mood depends on the level of maladaptive perfectionism. Figure 2.1 The Distinction Between Moderator and Mediator
  • 48. Variables, Represented in a Causal Diagram 24 Figure 2.2 Classification Plot Representing Moderation of Relationship Between Attachment Anxiety and Depressed Mood, Moderated by Maladaptive Perfectionism 25 Source: Author created using data from Wei, W., Mallinckrodt, B., Russell, D., & Abraham, W. T. (2004). Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between adult attachment and depressive mood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(2), 201–212. We have illustrated one potential moderated outcome in the classification plot shown in Figure 2.2. Note that under conditions of low maladaptive perfectionism, there is only a small difference in depression between those with low and high attachment anxiety; however, under conditions of high maladaptive perfectionism, those with high levels of attachment anxiety are more likely to be depressed than those with low levels. In other words, maladaptive perfectionism moderates the relationship between
  • 49. attachment anxiety and depression, or, put another way, maladaptive perfectionism and attachment anxiety interact. One research study is not likely to establish and verify all of the important elements of a complex conceptual model. As one of our colleagues puts it, you would need a video camera to capture the entire Grand Canyon on film, whereas the dissertation is more like a snapshot, perhaps of a mule 26 and rider descending one small section of one canyon trail. Yet the proposed model can provide a useful context for current and future research studies. Most ambitious research studies rely heavily on just such theoretical models. As you might imagine, a researcher is in no position to test a model of this scope in a single study. For example, Gerald Patterson and his colleagues (Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989) spent many years developing and testing a model to explain aggressive and deviant behavior among young males. The model hypothesizes that such antisocial behavior can be
  • 50. causally linked to disrupted parental discipline and poor family management skills. Moreover, the relationship between these two sets of variables is not direct but is mediated by a network of other variables. The process is thought to begin with parents “training” a child to behave aggressively by relying on aversive behaviors in both punishment and negative reinforcement contingencies. The inability of the parents to control coercive exchanges among family members constitutes “training for fighting,” which leads, in turn, to aggressive behavior and poor peer relationships. This lack of social skills generalizes to antisocial behavior in the classroom, which makes it next to impossible for the youth to obtain basic academic skills, thus preparing him poorly to cope with life outside school. Ultimately, this set of factors leads to high rates of delinquent behavior. An abbreviated summary of one version of the model is shown in Figure 2.3. Figure 2.3 A Model of Antisocial Behavior 27 Source: From Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American
  • 51. Psychologist, 44. Copyright © 1989, American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission. Over the years that Patterson and his colleagues (Patterson et al., 1989) spent elaborating the nature of these relationships, they conducted numerous studies that each constituted a “snapshot” of one aspect of this complex model, perhaps focusing on a particular set of relationships. For instance, the investigator might ask whether a relationship exists between physical fighting and poor peer relationships. Each variable would have to be operationalized, probably by obtaining more than one measure of both fighting and peer relationships. In Patterson’s work, he asked mothers, peers, and teachers to rate levels of physical fighting because their perspectives might differ. Likewise, peers, teachers, and self- reports are used to obtain measures of peer relations. The objective of the study—that is, to determine the nature and form of the relationship between the primary variables—determines the research method that is employed. In the early years of his career, Patterson focused on the relationships among contextual variables, parental beliefs, parenting practices, and child outcomes. He concluded that parenting practices—such as discipline, monitoring, problem solving, involvement, and positive
  • 52. reinforcement— serve as mediating variables between parenting beliefs and attitudes and the child’s behavior. Once this model was supported by sufficient data, Patterson proceeded to establish links between children’s behavior problems and subsequent chronic juvenile and adult offending (Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002). Whether or not a particular dissertation is designed to test a theory or model derived from the research literature, we believe that the creation of a visual model, which shows how the network of relevant variables and constructs may be related to one another, can serve as a powerful tool for guiding the study. Arranging your ideas spatially helps to organize your thinking, which in turn helps position your proposed study within a larger framework. Research models are developed to account for the relationships among variables at a conceptual level and then used to guide the construction of research designs by which the relationships will be tested, usually, but not always, using contemporary statistics. The process is iterative so that the 28
  • 53. models are modified on the basis of data and then reevaluated in further studies. Two primary types of relationships can be identified and explored within a causal model (Jaccard & Jacoby, 2010): predictive relationships and causal relationships. A predictive relationship implies that an association or correlation exists between two (or more) variables without assuming that one causes the other. For example, we may determine that traveling frequently as a child is related to (predicts) being more proficient in languages as an adult, without knowing whether travel actually causes improvement in language skills. Above, we used the terms independent variables and predictor variables more or less interchangeably. Strictly speaking, however, when the issue is prediction, the relationship is between one or more predictor variables and a criterion variable (Jaccard & Jacoby, 2010). Causal relationships imply that one variable “causes” another; that is, changes in the primary variable, usually referred to as the independent variable, elicit changes in the second variable, the dependent variable or outcome variable. Although the principle of causation is the foundational
  • 54. bedrock for the experimental method in social science research, philosophers of science have argued for centuries whether causality can ever be truly demonstrated. Jaccard and Jacoby (2010) made a persuasive argument that the concept of causality is ultimately a heuristic that enables us to maintain an organized view of our world and of human behavior. By inferring causality, we can identify systematic relationships between variables and produce socially significant changes by manipulating some variables to influence others. Whether or not causality can be definitively demonstrated, much of contemporary research is conducted to give us confidence in theoretical models that purport causal relationships. There are several types of causal relationships, and each type can play a role in developing a causal model. How causal models can be employed and evaluated in dissertations is a subject for a later chapter (Chapter 6). How to think about and construct causal models as a way of describing research ideas constitutes much of Jaccard and Jacoby’s (2010) very useful book, and the following discussion is stimulated by their work. Jaccard and Jacoby observed that most researchers begin by identifying an outcome variable that they want to understand better. An example might
  • 55. be the level of concern people express for the environment, including by engaging in behaviors that are environmentally sensitive, such as recycling waste or reducing pollution. The next challenge is to identify some 29 variables that could potentially influence or relate to environmentally sensitive behavior. One could imagine a study, for example, that seeks to determine which interventions would increase the motivation to recycle trash (or, more modestly, just to understand differences between those who readily recycle and those who do not). Perhaps we predict that having a neighborhood trash collection system that mandates sorting trash into recyclable and nonrecyclable categories will directly affect environmentally pro-social behavior. Of course, not all studies begin by identifying a dependent variable; it is also possible to choose an independent variable and speculate about its effects. For instance, a study might address the implications of working in a highly polluting industry for health, socioeconomic status, and social relationships. Indirect causal relationships have an effect through the influence of an intermediary variable, which we have referred to above as a
  • 56. mediating variable. Moderated causal relationships are a third type of causal relationship, again defined earlier in the chapter. Most causal models contain a combination of the various types of relationships. Models can get very complex because of the number of variables and their subtle relationships with one another. Thus, simple path diagrams evolve into sophisticated theoretical networks. Many of these models have been developed over the course of a career by dedicated researchers, such as Gerald Patterson, who started with studies that explored subsets of variables within a model that then evolved in complexity. Because computer software allows for the manipulation of multiple variables simultaneously and relatively effortlessly, researchers usually begin by proposing one or more theoretical models and evaluating them empirically. One example of a research model comes from the dissertation of Bill MacNulty (2004), one of our doctoral students. MacNulty generated this model from existing research literature and then tested it empirically using a number of well-validated self-report scales. The study employed the schema-polarity model of psychological functioning to assess how self-
  • 57. schemas (cognitive representations of self and others) influence the experience of gratitude and forgiveness and whether these variables mediate relationships between self-schemas and physical health and well- being. The model is summarized in Figure 2.4. The plus and minus signs refer to the direction of the hypothesized relationships among the variables. Although the results supported most of the initial hypotheses, the proposed model needed to be amended to accommodate the data. This 30 is typical of the research enterprise, in which theories and conceptual models are continually tested and refined to serve as increasingly sophisticated representations of real-life phenomena. Another dissertation example comes from Jenny Knetig’s (2012) study of active duty military personnel who are at risk of experiencing symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Knetig speculated that being psychologically minded (a component of what is referred to as having a mentalizing capacity) allows some soldiers to perceive and interpret cognitive and affective states of themselves and others in a way
  • 58. that might facilitate resilience and help-seeking behaviors, which, in turn, mitigate how they are affected by severe stress. At the outset of her study, on the basis of the available literature and her own experience, Knetig postulated that the relationships among these variables might look something like the diagram in Figure 2.5. Figure 2.4 A Theoretical Framework Presented as a Causal Diagram Source: From Self-Schemas, Forgiveness, Gratitude, Physical Health, and Subjective Well-Being, by W. MacNulty, 2004, unpublished doctoral dissertation, Fielding Graduate University, Santa Barbara, CA. Copyright 2004 by W. MacNulty. Reprinted with permission of the author. At the conclusion of the study, after collecting her data and performing a number of statistical analyses (canonical correlation analysis), Knetig amended her proposed model, as shown in Figure 2.6, to reflect the experiences of her participants more accurately. 31 Figure 2.5 Proposed Mediational Model Relating Psychological
  • 59. Mindedness to PTSD Source: Knetig, 2012, p. 52. Reprinted with permission of the author. In short, the data suggested that soldiers who are more psychologically minded are less apt to conceal their thoughts and feelings. It also suggested that the relationship between psychological mindedness and symptoms is mediated by self-concealment. Figure 2.6 Final Mediation Model Relating Psychological Mindedness to PTSD Source: Knetig, 2012, p. 54. Reprinted with permission of the author. Generating Researchable Questions To help students generate researchable questions from their interesting ideas, we use a brainstorming exercise that begins with labeling one or two variables and generating a second or third. Brainstorming consists of 32 openly and noncritically listing all possible ideas in a given period of time. Later you can return to a more critical analysis of each idea and delete those that are uninteresting, not meaningful, or impractical.
  • 60. Ultimately, of course, it is contact with the literature that determines whether or not a research question is viable, because the literature houses the scholarly inquiry that goes beyond the limits of your own knowledge. We suggest that you do this brainstorming exercise in a small group so that the person receiving the consultation merely serves as a scribe to record the ideas thrown out by the other group members (see Box 2.1). After 5 or 10 minutes, move on to the next person’s partially formed research topic. We generally use this exercise in groups of three or four so that group members can frequently shift groups and draw on the spontaneous reactions of a larger number of peers, uncontaminated by prior ideas or a particular mind-set. The exercise involves suspending critical thinking and allowing new ideas to percolate. It should especially suit divergent thinkers, who will find the demand to be expansive in their thinking exciting and creative. Convergent thinkers may experience the exercise as a bit overwhelming, but they will find fulfillment in other stages of the research process that demand compulsivity, care, and precision. Every chapter of a dissertation contains both divergent and convergent elements.
  • 61. Note that not all worthwhile research studies focus on three (or more) primary variables. Many studies look at the relationship between two variables or concepts, and a few descriptive studies make do with one variable or construct. The latter generally occurs in the early stages of research in an area, when little is known about a topic. Some investigators are pathfinders in terms of opening up new topics of research by trying to understand as much as possible about a phenomenon and generating more informed hypotheses for others to test in the future. Nevertheless, we believe that most students underestimate what is currently known about most topics and that the most interesting, practical, and theoretically meaningful studies are likely to consider relationships among several variables. Our exercise for generating research questions is only one option. Jaccard and Jacoby (2010) have listed 26 different heuristics for how to think creatively about questions or phenomena that may be of interest to you. Items on their list include analyzing your own experiences, using case 33
  • 62. studies, interviewing experts in the field, role-playing, and conducting thought experiments. The adoption of thought experiments might be particularly helpful. Jaccard and Jacoby viewed these as experiments that you conduct in your mind as if you are really obtaining the data and analyzing the results. You can then play with the possibilities of adding new variables or introducing different contingencies into the situation. One contingency is the introduction of so-called counterfactuals into the thought experiment (Tetlock & Belkin, 1996). Counterfactuals refer to “what-might-have-been” scenarios, such as what might happen if parents rather than administrators ran the public schools. Researchers can use this strategy to address theoretical alternatives that might otherwise not be raised or appreciated. We conclude this chapter with the outline in Box 2.2, which asks you to look at the kinds of issues that need to be considered and responded to during the course of developing the research proposal. By and large, your dissertation committee will need to be convinced of three things to be comfortable with your proposal:
  • 63. 1. Is the question clear and researchable, and will the answer to the question extend knowledge in your field of study? 2. Have you located your question within the context of previous study such that you have demonstrated a mastery and consideration of the relevant background literature? 3. Is the proposed method suitable for exploring your question? Box 2.1 Brainstorming Exercise Begin by defining one or two variables (or constructs) of interest. Then generate a list of additional variables (or constructs) that in some way amplify the original variables or illuminate the relationship between them. The new variables you list may be independent variables, dependent variables, moderating variables, or mediating variables in the research questions you eventually select. After brainstorming this list, go back and eliminate those variables that do not interest you or do not seem promising to pursue. Finally, see if you can now define one or more research questions that speak to the relationship among the two or three variables (or constructs) you have specified. Ultimately, each of these variables will need to be operationally defined as you develop your research study. Here are some examples of the application of this brainstorming exercise to topics from different disciplines. Political Science Begin with an interest in citizen participation in city council meetings. List variables or phenomena that might influence, be influenced by, or be related to this variable. A
  • 64. sample research question is “What is the impact of citizen participation in city council meetings on legislative decision making?” 34 Education Begin with an interest in single mothers who return to school while receiving Aid to Families with Dependent Children (AFDC). List variables or phenomena that might influence, be influenced by, or be related to this variable. A sample research question is “What is the effect of the availability of child care on whether single mothers receiving AFDC return to school?” Criminal Justice Begin with an interest in the relationship between neighborhood crime watch programs and robbery rates. List variables that might influence or amplify the relationship between these two variables. A sample research question is “What is the effect of neighborhood crime watch programs, in both urban and rural environments, on the rate of burglaries?” Psychology Begin with an interest in the relationship between physical attractiveness and self-esteem. List variables that might amplify or influence the relationship between these two variables. A sample research question is “What is the role of body image and physical attractiveness in self- esteem?” Another sample research question is “What is the role of body image in mediating the relationship between physical attractiveness and self-esteem?”
  • 65. Selecting a Suitable Topic: Student Suggestions 35 Over the years, our students have provided many useful suggestions for completing a dissertation. Here are some of the suggestions they have offered to one another that pertain to the earliest stages of developing a dissertation. Other suggestions are noted at the appropriate places in subsequent chapters. 1. Start a computer file where you can store good ideas for future reference. Use the file for noting books and articles to get from the library, good quotations, inspirations for future studies, half-baked notions that might be useful in the future, and so on. 2. Think of your topic as a large jigsaw puzzle with a piece missing. That piece is what you want to research to fill in the gap in your field. To discover which piece is missing, you must read as much of the literature as possible in your field. 3. Before you begin, read several well-written dissertations recommended by your chairperson. 4. As you progress through the dissertation process and your question shrinks due to the necessity of maintaining a manageable project for one person, don’t lose heart. Even very small questions can serve much larger purposes. 5. To keep the perspective of meaningfulness throughout, keep imagining an audience of individuals who
  • 66. would want to know the results of your work. Even if you can imagine only 25 people in the world who would care, keep that group alive in front of your eyes. 6. A few students and I have found that beginning dissertation work early on and setting “mile marker dates” is very helpful. This has helped us keep each other accountable by regular “check-ins” with each other. Sharing these dates with friends and family as well as posting them in a conspicuous place so you see them pretty much daily has helped as well. 7. Make contact with researchers who may be of interest to you. It cannot hurt, and it might be quite helpful. 8. Travel to at least one professional meeting where research in your area of interest is being presented. I went to a paper presentation in my area of family violence. I realized that I knew more about my topic than I thought and shared my research ideas with one of the presenters. I came home reinvigorated and anxious to complete my dissertation. 36 Box 2.2 Outline of Issues for a Student Researcher to Complete in the Development of the Research Proposal Review of the Literature The classic, definitive, or most influential pieces of research in this area are . . . The journals that specialize in the kind of research in which I
  • 67. propose to engage are . . . The body(ies) of research to which I wish to add is (are) . . . The experts in the field of my research are . . . Statement of the Problem The intellectual problem(s) I may help solve through this research is (are) . . . The moral, political, social, or practical problem(s) I may help alleviate through this research is (are) . . . Method The method I propose to use to answer my question, prove my point, or gain more detailed and substantive knowledge is . . . An alternative way to do this study would be . . . Three important research studies that have been carried out using the method I propose are . . . The reason(s) this method is a good one for my question, proposition, or subject is (are) . . . Possible weaknesses of this method are . . . The skills I will need to use this method are . . . Of these skills, I still need to acquire . . . I propose to acquire these skills by . . . Notes 1. A construct is a concept used for scientific purposes in building theories. Constructs (e.g., self-esteem), like concepts, are abstractions formed by generalizing from specific behaviors or manipulations. When constructs are operationalized in such a way that they can be “scored” to take on different numerical values, they are referred to as variables.
  • 68. 2. There has been extensive discussion in recent literature regarding the definition of, appropriate statistical analyses for, and interpretation of moderating and mediating effects. We recommend the following discussions: Frazier, Tix, and Barron, 2004; Hayes, 2009; Jaccard and Jacoby, 2010; Kazdin, 2007; Kim, Kaye, and Wright, 2001; and Preacher and Hayes, 2008. 37 3 Methods of Inquiry Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches The principal characteristic of scholarly and scientific inquiry— as opposed to informal, intuitive kinds of inquiry—is the use of rationally grounded procedures to extend knowledge that a community of scholars regards as reliable and valid. The dissertation process is a ritual of socialization into that community of scholars, so it is necessary for you, as a student, to master the scholarly procedures within your discipline. The specific methods chosen to attack a problem will depend on your discipline and the nature of the specific problem. There is no universally accepted approach within the social sciences, although there are rich research traditions that
  • 69. cannot be ignored, as well as a common understanding that chosen methods of inquiry must rest on rational justification. This means that scientific methods differ from more informal methods of inquiry by their reliance on validated public procedures that have been determined to produce reliable knowledge. Currently, there are many disagreements in the social sciences regarding what constitutes knowledge and the procedures for gaining it. One way to think about how research generally contributes to the knowledge base of a discipline is in terms of the following three-level hierarchy of knowledge, suggested by our colleague Marilyn Freimuth. Axiologic/Epistemic Level. This is the underlying level of basic world hypotheses that form the foundation for content and method within a field of inquiry. Epistemology refers to the study of the nature of knowledge, whereas axiology refers to the study of ethics, values, and aesthetics. Examples of constructs at this level include the explanatory principle of cause and effect and the notion of open systems. Theoretical Level.
  • 70. This is the level of models and theories. Theories are premises to account for data or, more informally, explanations of how things work based on 38 data. Examples are the theory of loss aversion in economics (Tversky & Kahneman, 1991) and the five-factor theory of personality in psychology (McCrae & Costa, 2003). The distinction between theories and models is murky because these terms are often used interchangeably within the social sciences. At the most basic level, both theories and models refer to relationships between concepts. For our purposes, the term model refers to a higher-order theory, that is, a representational system at a higher level of abstraction that can inform and be informed by alternative theories. (This concept is similar to the framework or worldview that guides researchers, identified as a “paradigm” by Thomas Kuhn [1996].) Thus, psychoanalysis could be seen as a model, a wide lens with which to view and understand the mysteries of human behavior. Each model carries with it certain sets of assumptions. In the case of psychoanalysis, these assumptions include the unifying importance of causal determinism and unconscious
  • 71. motivation. Note that this use of the term model differs somewhat from that in the discussion of working models in Chapter 2. Empirical Level. In the field of epistemology, empiricism refers to a commitment to obtaining knowledge through sense experience (literally, “based on experience” in Greek). Empiricism is frequently contrasted with rationalism, which refers to knowledge derived purely through thought and reason, and to more natural philosophical and religious traditions of reaching conclusions. In the present context, the empirical level includes hypotheses and methods and data of scientific research. Hypotheses are tentative answers to questions, generally based on theory. The primary role of research within this three-level schema is to link the theoretical and the empirical. Theories need the support of data to remain viable, whereas methods carry assumptions that are theoretical in nature. Note that research findings do not contribute directly to the axiologic/epistemic level or even to basic models. Those levels reflect fundamental value commitments and personal preferences that are rarely modified on the basis of additional data, especially the kind of data generated by scholarly research. It is hard to imagine a
  • 72. psychoanalyst becoming a behaviorist or a Republican joining the Democrats without a significant shift in values unlikely to be compelled by the accumulated wisdom imparted by a series of research studies. Because most researchers strongly identify with particular values and carry many personal 39 preferences into their work, it becomes especially important to learn to discriminate between beliefs and opinions, on the one hand, and verifiable, data-inspired support for ideas, on the other hand. A brief look at the history of science is a humbling experience that should put to rest the misguided notion that research discovers truth. Drilling holes in the skull (trephining) used to be an acceptable way to dismiss the demons responsible for mental illness, and it wasn’t that long ago that the sun was thought to circle the earth. One wonders what remnants of contemporary scientific truth will be regarded as equally ludicrous tomorrow. Instead, what research contributes is a series of thoughtful observations that support or question the validity of theories, which are in turn based on a set of largely untestable beliefs and
  • 73. assumptions. Every once in a while, at opportunistic moments of scholarly upheaval, a new paradigm appears that seems to do a better job of explaining the available data and guiding further inquiry. Each social science discipline and set of investigators seems to have its own favored approach to generating knowledge. For instance, public opinion studies usually rely on survey research methods, psychoanalytic studies of infants make use of observational techniques, studies of organizational effectiveness may employ action research methods and case studies, historical investigations of political and social events rely on archival records and content analysis, and laboratory studies of perceptual processes stress experimental manipulation and hypothesis testing. Within your chosen field, it is important to ask how a piece of research acquires legitimacy as reliable knowledge. No doubt part of the answer comes down to underlying epistemological assumptions and values. Certainly research strategies will differ in terms of the problems they address and the outcomes they produce. As we later show, one important distinction in the choice of method seems to be the nature of the relationship between the researcher and the topic of study.
  • 74. We would argue that researchers in the social sciences have generally been myopic in defining the kinds of studies that might legitimately lend themselves to research dissertations. Most students in the social sciences are taught early on about the difference between independent and dependent variables and how experimental research implies active manipulation of independent variables to observe a subsequent impact on dependent variables. This basic and time-honored strategy has an earthy 40 history in the systematic evaluation of fertilizers for agricultural productivity (Cowles, 2000). It remains a cornerstone in conducting social science research with human subjects. Yet it is certainly not the only way to conduct research. The only universal in scientific knowledge is a general commitment to using logical argument and evidence to arrive at conclusions that are recognized as tentative and subject to further amendment. Good scientists in action often deviate from an “official” philosophy of science and a
  • 75. prescribed methodology. William Bevan (1991), former president of the American Psychological Association, noted, If you want to understand what effective science making is about, don’t listen to what creative scientists say about their formal belief systems. Watch what they do. When they engage in good, effective science making they don’t, as a rule, reflect on their presuppositions; they engage in a practical art form in which their decisions are motivated by the requirements of particular problem solving. (p. 478) The key to evaluating a completed study is to assess whether the selected method is sufficiently rigorous and appropriate to the research question and whether the study is conceptually and theoretically grounded. The more familiar you are with the full range of alternative research strategies, the more enlightened and appropriate your choice of a particular method is apt to be. Too often, students become so enamored with an approach to research that they choose the method before determining the question. Unless the dissertation is designed to illustrate the use of a promising and innovative methodology, this is putting the cart before the horse. In general, the method needs to evolve out of the research question and be determined by it. Quantitative Methods
  • 76. The epistemological foundation of most social science inquiry throughout the 20th century was logical positivism, a school of thought that maintains that all knowledge is derived from direct observation and logical inferences based on direct observation. To a great extent, the notion of 41 objectively studying human beings is derived from a love affair that social scientists have had with the natural sciences, which sought to understand nature by isolating phenomena, observing them, and formulating mathematical laws to describe patterns in nature. Current research in the social sciences is deeply steeped in the empirical and quantitative traditions. Statistical methods are especially useful for looking at relationships and patterns and expressing these patterns with numbers. Descriptive statistics describe these patterns of behavior, whereas inferential statistics draw on probabilistic arguments to generalize findings from samples to populations of interest. Kerlinger (1977) focused on the inferential process when he defined statistics as
  • 77. the theory and method of analyzing quantitative data obtained from samples of observations in order to study and compare sources of variance of phenomena, to help make decisions to accept or reject hypothesized relations between the phenomena, and to aid in making reliable inferences from empirical observations. (p. 185) Note that the focus in the natural science model of research is the study of average or group effects, not of individual differences. The kinds of inferential statements that derive from this model of research refer to groups of people or groups of events; that is, they are probabilistic (e.g., “Surveys find that most people believe that police officers use excessive force in dealing with criminals,” or “Emotional expressiveness is related to coping effectively with natural disasters”). In experimental research, quantitative research designs are used to determine aggregate differences between groups or classes of subjects. Emphasis is placed on precise measurement and controlling for extraneous sources of error. The purpose, therefore, is to isolate a variable of interest (the independent variable) and manipulate it to observe the impact of the manipulation on a second, or dependent, variable. This procedure is facilitated by the “control” of extraneous variables, thus allowing the researcher to infer a causal relationship between the two (or
  • 78. more) variables of interest. 42 Methodological control is generally accomplished by two procedures that rely on the principle of randomness. One is random sampling, which uses subjects that have randomly been drawn from the potential pool of subjects so that each member of the population has an equal chance or known probability of being selected. Random selection of subjects permits the researcher to generalize the results of the study from the sample to the population in question. The second procedure is randomization, which assigns subjects to groups or experimental conditions in such a way that each subject has an equal chance of being selected for each condition. Subject characteristics are thus randomly distributed in every respect other than the experimental manipulation or treatment, allowing the researcher to infer that resultant differences between the groups must be the result of the isolated variable in question. Unfortunately, these efforts at experimental control are often impractical in social science research with human subjects. Psychology, for
  • 79. instance, has an honorable tradition of laboratory research using tight experimental designs, but research in the clinical or social arena may not permit the kind of control stipulated by the experimental method. This dilemma is equally prominent in field studies in disciplines such as sociology, education, and political science. Jared Diamond (2005), Pulitzer Prize–winning geographer and biologist, for example, conducted quantitative “natural experiments” to investigate the problem of deforestation on Pacific islands. He and his colleague Barry Rolett numerically graded the extent of deforestation on 81 Pacific islands and statistically predicted this outcome from a combination of nine input variables, such as the amount of rainfall, isolation from human populations, and restoration of soil fertility. In a different context, one cannot practically conspire to rear children using two distinct parenting styles, nor can one ethically inflict child abuse to study its immediate impact in a controlled fashion. Researchers can, however, study analogs of these variables using pure experimental designs (e.g., one can ask parents to use specific interventions at the onset of particular child behaviors). Change studies, in which a treatment or program is being evaluated for its effectiveness, may also lend themselves
  • 80. well to experimental designs. Even so, it may not be possible to randomize subjects into groups that receive a treatment or intervention and those that do not. A number of ingenious solutions have been proposed to deal with the ethics of denying treatment to those who need it, including the use of placebos and waiting-list controls (Kazdin, 2002). 43 More typically, the research method of choice in the social sciences seems to be a quasi-experimental design that compromises some of the rigor of the controlled experiment but maintains the argument and logic of experimental research (Kline, 2009; Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2001). This kind of research uses a systematic, empirical approach in which the investigator does not employ a control group or does not randomly assign subjects to conditions because events have already occurred or cannot be sufficiently manipulated. So-called causal statements become correlational statements in quasi-experimental research, although it is often possible to infer a sequence of events in causal form. That is one reason why it is crucial to have a theoretical model as a foundation for an empirical study.
  • 81. The model itself informs your attempt to meaningfully interpret the results of the study. However, because it is difficult to ascribe causality with confidence from quasi-experimental designs that lack true experimental manipulation, independent variables are often termed “predictor” variables in these studies (Kline, 2009). Caution is also needed in interpreting the meaning of results whenever subjects assign themselves to groups. A colorful example is the apparent negative correlation that exists between the numbers of mules found in the various states and the number of PhDs living there. The fact that states that have a lot of mules don’t have so many PhDs, and vice versa, is an empirical observation that can be statistically expressed in the form of a correlation coefficient. A researcher would be hard-pressed to argue a causal relationship between these two variables unless he or she drew on an underlying theoretical model that links the two variables through a third (mediating) variable, such as the degree of urbanization. Note that this simple correlational study could, at least theoretically, be transformed into an experimental study by, for example, flooding some states with mules to see if the PhDs leave or wooing the PhDs across state lines to see if the
  • 82. number of mules in the new state of residence decreases. This is not a book on research design, but the adoption of a particular research strategy will affect the final form of your dissertation. Whether a study employs a true experimental design, a quasi-experimental design, or a cross-sectional survey design, the most popular strategy in the social sciences is a comparison between groups. That is, independent (randomly assigned) groups of subjects are used for each experimental or control condition. The best-known variant of this strategy, the pretest- posttest control group design, uses two equivalent groups of subjects that both 44 receive pretests and posttests and differ only in the experimental treatment that is given to one group (see Table 3.1). In this design, it becomes possible to evaluate the impact of an intervention because the control group offers a baseline for comparison. One could use this design to evaluate, for example, whether the inclusion of spouses in an aftercare program for heart bypass surgery patients encourages greater compliance with medical regimens. Or one could
  • 83. design a study to evaluate the effect of introducing air bags in automobiles on the rate of physical injury to passengers. Some automobiles of a given make would receive air bags, some would not, and the change in types and rates of injuries would be the dependent measure. The straightforward pretest–posttest–control group design makes it possible to attribute the effect of experimental interventions to those interventions rather than to some extraneous variable. The interpretation of results of studies using this design may be compromised, however, if the subjects have not been assigned to conditions in a truly random manner. In the proposed air bag study, for example, if automobiles and drivers are not randomly assigned to conditions, inherently safer drivers may well choose automobiles with better safety features. Because randomization is not always possible, it becomes crucial to argue for the “equivalence” of the two groups, even if they do not derive from the identical population of subjects. One way in which researchers attempt to make this argument is by matching the groups on key variables that are critical to the understanding of the study, such as age, sex, symptomatology, or, in the current example, the previous driving records of the participants.
  • 84. The basic pretest–posttest–control group design does not adequately control for any effect that the pretest evaluations might have on the subjects. Some assessments can sensitize subjects by making them aware that they are now participating in a study or by providing a practice 45 experience that contaminates the validity of posttest results. A simple posttest-only design may get around this difficulty and is probably underused (Campbell & Stanley, 2005). In any case, choosing a basic research design does not eliminate the need for you to think carefully and creatively about potential sources of error and alternative explanations to account for findings. Most experimental designs are variants of the treatment and control group format described earlier.1 Such designs permit the researcher to make causal inferences regarding relationships among the variables. In contrast, correlational (or observational) studies do not generally enable the researcher to demonstrate causal relationships among variables. Any conclusions regarding causality must be inferred from the
  • 85. underlying theory rather than from the results of the study. Studies built around experimental or correlational designs generate data that are subsequently analyzed using appropriate inferential statistics. Statistical techniques that are used to evaluate the effectiveness of an intervention or a difference between groups, such as an analysis of variance (ANOVA) or t test, compare the size of between-group differences (e.g., the treatment effect) with the size of within- group differences due to individual variability. These techniques express the experimental tradition. The logic of the correlational paradigm is quite different (Cronbach, 1975). Correlations depend on comparing two distributions of scores that are broadly dispersed along two dimensions, such as longevity and alcohol use. Statistical techniques that emerged from this tradition, such as multiple regression, are especially popular in social science research that relies on questionnaires, surveys, or scales and the relationship between continuous variables. Because correlational studies typically cannot randomly assign subjects to groups, we have a second major type of control in social science research—statistical control. Statistical control attempts to use complex statistical procedures to remove