SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/298799146
There But for the Grace of God: Counterfactuals Influence Religious Belief and
Images of the Divine
Article  in  Social Psychological and Personality Science · April 2016
DOI: 10.1177/1948550616632576
CITATIONS
3
READS
149
3 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Social Media Analytics for Well-being View project
Big data insights on stress: The language of stress in individuals and communities View project
Anneke Buffone
University of Pennsylvania
17 PUBLICATIONS   102 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
Michael Poulin
University at Buffalo, The State University of New York
38 PUBLICATIONS   2,023 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Anneke Buffone on 31 August 2016.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
Article
There But for the Grace of God:
Counterfactuals Influence Religious
Belief and Images of the Divine
Anneke Buffone1,2
, Shira Gabriel2
, and Michael Poulin2
Abstract
Past research suggests that thinking counterfactually leads to a perception that major life events are fated or predetermined. We
hypothesized that a perception that events are fated will activate perceptions that God played a role in the event, which will
increase feelings of religiousness. Because most people view God as a positive influence, we hypothesized that this would only
happen when imagining how events might have turned out worse (i.e., downward counterfactuals). Across two experiments, we
examined the hypothesis that thinking counterfactually increases religiosity. The studies supported our predictions. Generating
counterfactuals increased belief in God and religiosity across several variables. Furthermore, evidence was found for the proposed
pathway. In summary, the studies provided strong and consistent empirical support for counterfactual thinking’s role in religious
faith and for this effect to be due to increased perceptions of God’ role in the event. Implications for religion, cognition, and
counterfactual thinking are discussed.
Keywords
counterfactual thinking, religion, attitudes, attribution, interpersonal relationships, counterfactual direction, fate
Psalm 124
2 If the Lord had not been on our side when men attacked us,
3 when their anger flared against us, they would have swal-
lowed us alive; ( . . . )
Despite historical trends toward less religiosity, most people
still believe in God (Gallup, 2011). The current research is
concerned with the age-old question of how believers find
evidence in their lives for the existence of God. We propose
that counterfactual thinking, (i.e., imagining how life would
be different if a given event had not occurred) allows
believers to find evidence that God plays a role in their
lives.
Counterfactual thinking is defined as ‘‘if-then’’ statements
about how a series of events could have had an altered outcome
if an event had not occurred or had occurred differently (Roese,
1997; Roese & Olson, 1995). For example, Jane might think,
‘‘if I hadn’t taken the bus that day, I never would have met
my husband.’’ Thinking counterfactually is a commonly used
reflection strategy, with implications for problem solving and
affect management (Epstude & Roese, 2008; Roese, 1994).
In addition, counterfactual thinking about past autobiographi-
cal events increases awareness of unknown causes and surpris-
ing connections (Koo, Algoe, Wilson, & Gilbert, 2008) and
perceptions of the event being fated (Kray et al., 2010; McA-
dams, Reynolds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001; Sanna &
Turley, 1996).
It is the influence of counterfactual thinking on perceptions
of fatedness that we propose plays an integral role in increasing
religiosity. Merriam-Webster defines fate as ‘‘a power that is
believed to control what happens in the future’’ (‘‘Fate,’’
2015). Thus, the question arises, if fate implies that some power
is controlling the events in one’s life, what is that power? Sur-
vey research suggests that, for the majority of people in the
world today, power is thought to be God (Gallup, 2011). Thus,
we propose that when counterfactual thinking leads to an
increase in fatedness, that increase in fatednesss will lead to
an increased perception that God is playing a role in one’s life.
Finally, a perception that God is playing a role in one’s life
should lead to increased religiosity.
Support for the role of counterfactual thinking in increasing
religiosity comes from practices encouraged by major religions
(Bender, 2008). For example, in Catholicism, the Examen
encourages reflective prayer each night in order to perceive
divine influence in the day’s events. Protestant religions have
1
Department of Psychology, Positive Psychology Center, University of
Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
2
Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Buffalo, NY, USA
Corresponding Author:
Anneke Buffone, Department of Psychology, Positive Psychology Center,
University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA.
Email: buffonea@sas.upenn.edu
Social Psychological and
Personality Science
2016, Vol. 7(3) 256-263
ª The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1948550616632576
spps.sagepub.com
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
developed programs encouraging believers to journal each
night about the day’s events to deduce evidence for divine
responsiveness (Bender, 2008). The Jewish Passover song and
prayer ‘‘Dayenu’’ encourages Jews to find evidence of God’s
presence by imagining how life would have been different if
God had not intervened. In Islam, Mohammed encourages
believers to reflect on ordinary events, such as the occurrence
of rain, to recognize Allah’s benevolence. In Hinduism, believ-
ers are encouraged to see everyday occurrences as manifesta-
tions of the Divine. In sum, all the current major theistic
religions encourage believers to reflect on the events in their
lives to find evidence for the existence of God. By imagining
how their days might have been different, religious practi-
tioners are able to perceive the ways that God intervened.
The Present Research
Two studies were run to test the hypothesis that downward
counterfactual thinking leads to an increase in religiosity.1
In
both studies, a model was tested in which generating counter-
factuals leads to a perception of fatedness which leads to a
belief in God’s role that leads to increased religiosity (see
Figure 1).
Pretesting suggested that our participants tend to see God as
a benevolent, helpful presence. Specifically, we surveyed 434
participants and found that 71% of participants agreed with
‘‘I believe that if there is a God, he is most likely loving and
caring and tries to make the best happen for people.’’ In addi-
tion, most religions appear to hold up an image of a benevolent
deity, the Bible states that ‘‘the Lord is kind and merciful’’
(Psalm 103); throughout the Quran, Allah is constantly referred
to as ‘‘the merciful, the compassionate’’; and in Hinduism, God
is the source of happiness and love. Thus, one would predict
that evidence for God’s hand in believers’ lives should be
found when imagining how one’s life would be worse if an
event had not happened (i.e., downward counterfactuals) and
not when imagining how one’s life would have been if the
event had not happened (i.e., upward counterfactuals). Study
1 manipulated counterfactual direction to examine that
hypothesis.
Study 1 also examined both positive and negative events.
Prior research on counterfactuals most often limits the focus
on either positive or, more commonly, negative events, as these
are more likely to elicit counterfactual thought (Burrus &
Roese, 2005; Mandel & Dhami, 2005; Roese, 1997). Some
work however has found that valence affects counterfactual
reflection: Upward counterfactuals are more common in
response to negative events and vice versa (Koo et al., 2008;
White & Lehman, 2005). Yet downward counterfactuals can
sometimes be generated in response to negative events, espe-
cially if individuals are motivated to make sense of a negative
event by benefit finding (Helgeson, Reynolds, & Tomich,
2006)—that is, focusing on an event’s unexpected positive out-
comes or its ‘‘silver lining.’’ In fact, religiosity is associated
with greater levels of spontaneous benefit finding (McIntosh,
Silver, & Worman, 1993), suggesting that downward counter-
factual thinking about negative events may fit well with reli-
gious belief. Given the conceptual and empirical separability
of counterfactual direction and event valence, we expected that
imagining alternate—specifically, worse—realities to positive
or negative life events should equally make individuals per-
ceive a benevolent God’s influence.
Study 1
Method
Participants
We recruited 292 (92 female) undergraduate students2
diverse
in ethnicity (19 African/African American, 48 Asian/Asian
American, 11 Hispanic/Hispanic American, 4 Arab/Arab
American, 2 Native American, and 208 European/European
American) and belief (22 Jewish, 18 Protestant, 104 Catholic,
4 Lutheran, 32 Other Christian, 10 Muslim, 8 Hindu, 26 Athe-
ist, 22 Nonbeliever, 21 Agnostic, and 23 Spiritual/Nonreli-
gious). Twelve individuals had to be excluded from the
analyses due to not following instructions.
Materials and Procedures
While seated in individual cubicles, unaware that the experi-
ment examined religion, participants wrote about a positive
or negative life event either upward or downward counterfac-
tually or without the use of counterfactuals, completed a mea-
sures of religious faith, measures of fate and whether
participants felt the ‘‘hand of God’’ in the event.
Essay task. Participants were instructed to select and describe
either an important negative or an important positive event
from their past (Kray et al., 2010). Subsequently, one third of
participants per group were instructed to write a coherent story
about how life would be better (upward) and one third about
how life would be worse (downward) if this event had never
Downward
Counterfactuals
Perceived
Fatedness
PercepƟons of
God's Role
Religious Faith
Figure 1. Proposed model for the effects of downward counterfactual thinking on religious faith by way of perceptions of fate and the role of
God.
Buffone et al. 257
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
occurred. A final third was asked to simply describe the event
in more detail.
Questionnaires. Subsequently participants completed the follow-
ing questionnaires.
Strength of Religious Faith. Participants completed the Santa
Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire (Plante &
Boccaccini, 1997) assessed on a 7-point scale (1 ¼ completely
disagree; 7 ¼ completely agree), featuring items such as ‘‘I
look at my faith as a source of inspiration.’’3
Comparative Strength of Religious Faith. Participants were
asked ‘‘How strongly do you believe in God compared to most
other people you know’’ (1 ¼ not at all strongly; 7 ¼ extremely
strongly).
Perceptions of own religious behavior. We also assessed per-
ceived religious behavior by asking how often participants
recalled attending a place of worship (1 ¼ never; 7 ¼ daily) and
how frequently they prayed (1 ¼ never; 7 ¼ several times a
day).
Image of God. We included a shortened version of the God
Image Scales (Lawrence, 1997) assessing perceptions of God
as an attachment figure.
Fatedness. Further participants completed an item identical
to the one used to measure fatedness by Kray and col-
leagues (2010), asking participants to indicate whether they
perceived the event as ‘‘a product of fate’’ as well as an
item asking how much the event appeared ‘‘meant to be,’’
both assessed on a 7-point scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree;
7 ¼ strongly agree).
Hand of God in Experience. We assessed the degree to which
participants felt the influence of God in their event by ask-
ing them to indicate, on a 7-point scale, their agreement
with the single item ‘‘I felt the hand of God in this life
experience.’’
Results and Discussion
Event Valence
Our primary hypothesis was that downward counterfactual
thinking would increase strength of religious faith, perceptions
of fatedness, and perceptions of God’s causal influence in the
life event compared to both upward counterfactual and factual
reflection. We included event valence in our design to see
whether effects would differ depending on whether events were
positive or negative. Thus, all results were examined with 3
(reflection type: downward counterfactual vs. upward counter-
factual vs. factual) Â 2 (event valence: positive vs. negative
event) analyses of variance. However, we were only interested
in the effects for reflection type, and none of our analyses
showed any significant main effects or interactions for event
valence. Therefore, for clarity of report, only results for reflec-
tion type are reported.
Counterfactuals and Religious Faith
Given the high degree of conceptual similarity among all of the
religion items measured (tapping strength of religious faith,
comparative strength of religious faith, perceptions of own reli-
gious behavior, and images of God), we first examined whether
it was defensible to combine all of these items into a single
index to avoid capitalizing on Type-I error. We computed a sin-
gle Religious Faith Scale that was the mean of all of the above
items, and this scale had excellent internal consistency
(a ¼ .98) so was used as the single religiosity variable in
the following analyses. However, analyses conducted using
the above scales separately yielded substantively similar results
for each.
We examined effects of the manipulations on our religious
faith scale, and in line with predictions, a significant main
effect emerged for kind of reflection, F(2, 274) ¼ 3.88,
p ¼ .02, Z2
p ¼ .03. Downward counterfactual reflection
(M ¼ 3.90, SD ¼ 1.42) produced marginally higher religious
faith than factual reflection (M ¼ 3.50, SD ¼ 1.47; p ¼ .06),
mean difference 0.40, 95% confidence interval [CI] ¼
[À0.01, 0.82], and significantly higher religious faith than
upward counterfactual reflection (M ¼ 3.31, SD ¼ 1.47;
p ¼ .007), mean difference ¼ À0.59, 95% CI ¼ [À0.17,
À1.01]. Upward and factual reflection did not differ from
one another ( p ¼ .39), mean difference ¼ À0.19, 95%
CI ¼ [À0.61, 0.24].
Mediation by Fatedness and Hand of God
In order to test the hypothesis that downward counterfac-
tuals increased religious faith by increasing fatedness per-
ceptions, which in turn increased belief in God’s role in
the event (see Figure 1), we conducted the following three
steps. First, we tested whether the contrast of downward
counterfactuals versus other conditions predicted each of the
proposed mediators, fatedness perceptions, and the hand of
God. Second, we tested the indirect effect corresponding
to each of the intermediate mediational paths (downward
counterfactuals ! fatedness ! hand of God; fatedness !
hand of God ! religious faith). Finally, we estimated the
overall indirect effect corresponding to the total mediated
effect (downward counterfactuals ! fatedness ! hand of
God ! religious faith).
Downward counterfactuals affecting Fatedness and hand of God. As
predicted, downward counterfactuals predicted higher levels
of fatedness perceptions (M ¼ 4.01, SD ¼ 1.67) versus
other reflection conditions (upward counterfactuals and
factual reflection; M ¼ 3.56, SD ¼ 1.77; p ¼ .04; mean differ-
ence ¼ À0.45, 95% CI ¼ [À0.88, À0.01]). In addition, down-
ward counterfactuals predicted higher perceptions of the hand
of God in the event (M ¼ 3.60, SD ¼ 1.92) versus other
reflection conditions (M ¼ 2.82, SD ¼ 1.79; p < .001; mean
difference ¼ À0.79, 95% CI ¼ [À1.24, À0.32]).
258 Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(3)
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Fatedness mediating the effect of downward counterfactuals on
hand of God. Fatedness and hand of God perceptions were sig-
nificantly correlated (r ¼ .44, p < .001). Following guidelines
recommended by Preacher and Hayes (2004; see also Zhao,
Lynch, & Chen, 2010), we performed bootstrapped estimates
of the indirect effect between downward counterfactuals and
hand of God by way of fatedness using Stata sgmediation and
bootstrap modules. Results (performed with 5,000 bootstrap
samples) indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect
B ¼ 0.20, 95% CI ¼ [0.001, 0.41], z ¼ 1.97, p ¼ .049). These
results suggest that downward counterfactuals may increase
hand of God perceptions by leading to perceptions that the life
event is fated.
Hand of God mediating the association between fatedness and
religious faith. Fatedness perceptions and religious faith were
associated (r ¼ .30, p < .001), and unsurprisingly, religious
faith and hand of God perceptions were also significantly cor-
related (r ¼ .68, p < .001). As above, we performed boot-
strapped estimates of the indirect effect between fatedness
and religious faith by way of hand of God. Results (performed
with 5,000 bootstrap samples) indicated an indirect slope
greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.25, 95% CI ¼ [0.19, 0.32],
z ¼ 7.55, p < .001). These results suggest that fatedness percep-
tions may predict religious faith specifically by leading to per-
ceptions of the hand of God.
Testing two-step mediation. Finally, we obtained the overall indi-
rect effect corresponding to the total-mediated effect (down-
ward counterfactuals ! fatedness ! hand of God !
religious faith). To do so, we used Stata’s sureg (seemingly
unrelated regression) command to obtain estimates of all direct
and indirect effects and then used the bootstrap command to
obtain 5,000 bootstrapped estimates of the overall indirect
effect. Results indicated an indirect slope marginally greater
than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.12, 95% CI ¼ [À0.01, À0.25],
z ¼ 1.86, p ¼ .06). These results lend support to the hypothesis
that downward counterfactuals predict religious faith specifi-
cally by leading to perceptions of fatedness, which in turn pre-
dict perceptions of the hand of God and, in turn, religious faith.
In summary, Study 1 found consistent support for the
hypothesis that counterfactual thinking plays an important role
in belief in God. Thinking about downward counterfactuals led
to an increased perception of fate that led to an increased per-
ception of the hand of God, which led to an increase in religious
faith. In other words, Study 1 suggested that counterfactual
thinking leads believers to the belief that an event did not occur
by chance alone, then individuals search for a likely source of
this fate, which leads them to God, which then increases their
religious faith. This occurred regardless of whether the event
was positive or negative in valence.
The effects were limited to downward counterfactuals, or
thinking about the ways that life might have been worse. This
makes sense, given the positive view most of our participants
have about God. Furthermore, the prominence of downward
counterfactuals makes it unlikely that uncertainty reduction
played a role in the link between counterfactuals and faith. It
is possible that participants in Study 1 felt less certain of the
world due to the counterfactual thinking and the uncertainty
(and not perceptions of the hand of God) led to the increased
religiosity. However, if anything, one might expect upward
counterfactuals to be more related to uncertainty than down-
ward because they are more closely related to negative emo-
tions and less closely related to positive emotions (e.g.,
Epstude & Roese, 2008; Inzlicht, McGregor, Hirsh, & Nash,
2009; Inzlicht & Tullet, 2010; McGregor, Nash, & Prentice,
2010; Roese, 1994; Sanna & Turley, 1996). Thus, the finding
that upward counterfactuals were completely unrelated to
increased religiosity makes it much less likely that uncertainty
reduction played a causal role.
In Study 2, we hoped to replicate the results of Study 1 with
a noncollege student sample. In addition, Study 2 utilized dif-
ferent measures of perception of God’s influence on the event
and fatedness to ensure that results were not limited to spe-
cific items. In addition, because the effects were only found
for downward counterfactuals in Study 1 and did not differ
based on the valence of the event, only downward counterfac-
tuals and positive events were utilized to increase statistical
power.
Study 2
Method
Participants
We recruited 108 (61 female; 1 questioning) diverse individu-
als (Mage ¼ 35.7 years) via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (13
African/African American, 5 Asian/Asian American, 6 Hispa-
nic/Hispanic American, 1 Arab/Arab American, 1 Native
American, and 82 European/European American) and belief
(2 Jewish, 20 Protestant, 15 Catholic, 1 Muslim, 0 Hindu, 13
Nondenominational, 14 Other Christian, 16 Agnostic, 14 Athe-
ist, 5 Nonbeliever, 5 Spiritual/Nonreligious, and 3 Other).
Users were restricted to be based in the United States, a hit
approval rate of .99 as well as a minimum of 1,000 approved
hits via Mturk’s settings. The final sample included 99 partici-
pants, after 11 had to be excluded from the analyses, because
they did not describe a (positive) life event (n ¼ 7) or the life
event they chose to write about was becoming religious itself
(n ¼ 2).
Materials and Procedures
Participants completed a writing prompt of either writing in
downward counterfactuals or writing factually about a positive
life event and then completed some potential mediator items
followed by the religious faith-dependent measures.
Essay task. The essay task was identical to Study 1 but only
included the positive event condition and the downward coun-
terfactual and no counterfactual conditions.
Buffone et al. 259
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Questionnaires. The religious faith questions from Study 1 were
used, with the exception that the God Image scales were not
added, as differing beliefs about God’s character are of less
importance to the question of divine influence. As in Study
1, the resulting scale had excellent internal consistency
(a ¼ .98).4
Fate’s role. ‘‘How much would you say was the event con-
trolled by fate?’’ as well as ‘‘How much was fate responsible
for the event’s occurrence?’’ were each assessed on 7-point
scales (a ¼ .75).
God’s role. How much did you feel that God took an active
role in how the life event unfolded?’’ ‘‘How much did you per-
ceive God’s guidance in the life event?’’ ‘‘I relied on God for
guidance in this life event,’’ ‘‘God worked through me or
through others in making this positive life event happen for
me,’’ ‘‘God worked together with me to make this life event
happen,’’ ‘‘How much was the event you described controlled
by God?’’ ‘‘How much was God responsible for the event’s
occurrence?’’ were each assessed on 7-point scales (a ¼ .98).
Further mediators. In addition to fatedness and God’s role, we
assessed further potential mediators including meaning, how
unlikely participants found the event; how easy it would be
to imagine alternate outcomes to the life event; whether the
event was controlled or caused by the self, chance, or other peo-
ple; and if other people or chance were responsible for the
event’s occurrence. None of these variables proved to be media-
tors of the observed effect linking downward counterfactuals to
increased faith using a bootstrapping approach. We also assessed
alternative outcome variables, such as optimism, death anxiety,
life satisfaction, self-esteem, and flourishing, which were not
predicted by counterfactual thinking in these data.
Results and Discussion
Counterfactuals and Religious Faith
Downward counterfactual reflection (M ¼ 4.04, SD ¼ 2.12)
produced higher religious faith than factual reflection
(M ¼ 3.11, SD ¼ 2.24; p ¼ .04), mean difference ¼ À0.93,
95% CI ¼ [À1.81, À0.05].
Mediation by Fate’s Role and God’s Role
In order to test the hypothesis that downward counterfactuals
increased religious faith by increasing fatedness perceptions,
which in turn increased belief in God’s role in the event, we
conducted the same three steps as in Study 1. We tested
(1) whether downward counterfactuals predicted each of the
proposed mediators, fate’s role, and God’s role; (2) the indirect
effect corresponding to each of the intermediate mediational
paths; and (3) the overall indirect effect corresponding to the
total-mediated effect.
Downward counterfactuals affecting fate’s role and God’s role. As
predicted, downward counterfactuals predicted higher
perceptions of fate’s role (M ¼ 4.62, SD ¼ 1.82) versus factual
reflection (M ¼ 3.63, SD ¼ 1.85; p ¼ .01; mean difference ¼
À0.98, 95% CI ¼ [À1.72, À0.24]). In addition, downward
counterfactuals predicted marginally higher perceptions of
God’s role in the event (M ¼ 3.74, SD ¼ 2.23) versus factual
reflection (M ¼ 2.93, SD ¼ 2.20; p ¼ .08; mean difference
¼ À0.81, 95% CI ¼ [À1.70, 0.09]).
Fatedness mediating the effect of downward counterfactuals on
hand of God. Perceptions of fate’s role and God’s role were
indeed significantly correlated (r ¼ .45, p < .001). Bootstrapped
estimates of the indirect effect (using 5,000 bootstrap samples)
indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.50,
95% CI ¼ [0.09, 0.91], z ¼ 2.41, p ¼ .02). Downward counter-
factuals may increase perceptions of God’s role in an event spe-
cifically by leading to perceptions of fate’s role in the event.
Hand of God mediating the association between fatedness and
religious faith. Perceptions of fate’s role and religious faith were
indeed associated (r ¼ .34, p < .001), and religious faith and
perceptions of God’s role were also significantly correlated
(r ¼ .85, p < .001). Bootstrapped estimates of the indirect effect
between fate’s role and religious faith by way of God’s role
(using 5,000 bootstrap samples) indicated an indirect slope
greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.46, 95% CI ¼ [0.29, 0.64],
z ¼ 5.17, p < .001). These results suggest that perceptions of
fate’s role may predict religious faith specifically by leading
to perceptions of God’s role in an event.
Testing two-step mediation. Finally, we obtained the overall indi-
rect effect corresponding to the total mediated effect (down-
ward counterfactuals ! fate’s role ! God’s role ! religious
faith), using Stata’s sureg and bootstrap commands as in Study
1. Results indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect
B ¼ 0.48, 95% CI ¼ [0.08, 0.88], z ¼ 2.37, p ¼ .02). These
results suggest that downward counterfactuals may predict reli-
gious faith specifically by leading to perceptions of fate’s role
in an event, which in turn predicts perceptions of God’s role in
the event and, in turn, religious faith.
In Summary, the results of Study 2 were highly consistent
with those of Study 1 utilizing a nonstudent sample and differ-
ent measures. Writing about downward counterfactuals from a
positive life event led participants to feel as if fate played a role
in the event, which led to a feeling that God played a role in the
event that led to increased religiosity.
General Discussion
Across two studies, we found support for the hypothesis that
counterfactual thinking leads to an increase in religious faith.
Furthermore, we found that this effect is driven particularly
by downward counterfactuals (Study 1). In addition, across
both studies, generating downward counterfactuals about the
life event led to a perception that the event was fated which led
to a perception that God played a role in the event which led to
increased religiosity.
260 Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(3)
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
The current research has important implications for views of
religious cognition. The predominant view on cognitive styles
and religiosity is that intuitive thinking is associated with reli-
giosity (Shenhav, Rand, & Greene, 2012), whereas analytic
reasoning is associated with nonreligiosity (Gervais & Noren-
zayan, 2012; Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli, Koehler, & Fugelsang,
2012). In other words, belief in God is initiatively appealing,
but rationally unlikely, so individuals are most likely to believe
in God when they rely on their intuitions at the expense of more
rational cognition. However, such differences in intuitive ver-
sus deliberative processing cannot fully account for individual
differences in belief in God because humans rarely hold beliefs
for which they are unable to generate at least some deliberative
evidence (Heider, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965; Weiner, 1974,
1980). The current work suggests a mechanism by which indi-
viduals can generate, test, and find evidence for hypotheses
about God—by seeing the way that God has made a difference
in their lives.
It is possible that the current findings may be truer among
some individuals than others and among some religions more
than others. In the psychology of religion literature, the form
in which believers perceive God to take control more generally
has been found to vary on a continuum of all or specific life
events being completely controlled by a deity or the deity tak-
ing control collaboratively together with the self (Berrenberg,
1987; Krause, 2005). Furthermore, while some believers view
God as not interfering in life events at all, some believe that
God is controlling all life events, and again others view God
as a collaborative force (Soenens et al., 2012). In addition,
believers sometimes perceive God as supportive, but other
times as absent (e.g., Phillips, Pargament, Lynn, & Crossley,
2004). Finally, some major religions do not believe in a deity
at all. Thus, it may be possible that the effects of downward
counterfactuals on religious beliefs may be moderated by kind
of religious orientation. Future work would be necessary to
examine this interesting possibility.
In addition, different results may have been found if differ-
ent kinds of counterfactual thoughts had been utilized. Parti-
cipants in our studies were asked to think about major life
events. Previsions research suggests that thinking counterfac-
tually about those kinds of events is particularly likely to lead
to beliefs about fatedness (Burrus & Roese, 2005). Thus, if
participants had generated counterfactuals about more mun-
dane or recent events, it is less likely that they would perceive
the role of fate and in turn feel more religious. Again, future
research would be necessary to examine that possible
moderator.
The current work suggests not only how religious faith can
be built and bolstered; it may also shed light on how it may fal-
ter. Individuals who have experienced prolonged adversity that
did not yield any apparent benefit or silver lining may have dif-
ficulty generating downward counterfactuals and thus be
unable to find evidence for God’s role in their lives. This inabil-
ity to find evidence for God’s presence, in particular when they
fell that they need God the most, may lead to a decrease in faith.
Thus, counterfactuals may play a role in the dissolution of
religious faith as well. Further research would be necessary
to examine that hypothesis.
This work also increases understanding of the importance of
counterfactual thinking, by demonstrating how important coun-
terfactual thinking may be in daily life, providing a means by
which people not only perceive the role of fate but find evi-
dence for the existence of God in their lives. Thus, this work
builds on an already strong literature that suggests the impor-
tance of counterfactual thinking.
In summary, our research suggests that people find evidence
for divine intervention in their lives through counterfactual
thinking, consistent with a view of all humans as naive scien-
tists, testing their theories of the world against their everyday
experience. It takes us one step closer to understanding reli-
gious cognition and the importance of counterfactual thinking
in the human experience.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article.
Notes
1. In an initial study (n ¼ 118), we found evidence for counterfactual
thinking (regardless of direction) to increase religious faith. Since
this study did not add anything substantially and due to length lim-
itations for the article, we excluded it from this article. The study
does however provide a conceptual, more general replication of the
effects that are reported here.
2. Given that these studies are the first to demonstrate an associ-
ation between counterfactuals and religious belief, the effect
size for this association, if any, was previously unknown. In
focal analyses across both studies, this effect size was estimated
to be Z2
¼.02 and .04, respectively. Post hoc analyses suggest
that the power of these studies to detect these effect sizes was
.54 and .55, respectively.
3. We also assessed several control measures including need for cog-
nition (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982), self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965),
and need to belong (Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, & Schreindorfer,
2013). Also, in order to ensure that our effects would not be due
to direct impacts of the life events on faith, we assessed whether
participants felt that the event had directly affected their religious
beliefs (‘‘The life event made me more/less religious,’’ ‘‘The life
event made me less religious,’’ and ‘‘My faith helped me deal with
my life experience’’).
References
Bender, C. (2008). How does God answer back? Poetics, 36, 476–492.
Berrenberg, J. L. (1987). The belief in personal control scale: A mea-
sure of God-mediated and exaggerated control. Journal of person-
ality assessment, 51, 194–206.
Buffone et al. 261
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Burrus, J., & Roese, N. J. (2006). Long ago it was meant to be: The
interplay between time, construal, and fate beliefs. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1050–1058.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.
Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counter-
factual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12,
168–192.
Fate. (2015). Merriam-Webster. Retrieved from http://www.merriam-
webster.com.com
Gallup. (2011). More than 9 in 10 Americans continue to believe in
god. Retrieved from http://www.gallup.com/poll/147887/ameri-
cans-continue-believe-god.aspx
Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking pro-
motes religious disbelief. Science, 336, 493–496.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New
York, NY: Wiley.
Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., & Tomich, P. L. (2006). A meta-
analytic review of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 74, 797–816.
Inzlicht, M., & Tullett, A. M. (2010). Reflecting on God: Religious
primes can reduce neurophysiological response to errors. Psycho-
logical Science, 21, 1184–1190.
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: the attri-
bution process in social psychology. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 219–266). New
York, NY: Academic Press.
Koo, M., Algoe, S. B., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). It’s a
wonderful life: Mentally subtracting positive events improves peo-
ple’s affective states, contrary to their affective forecasts. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1217–1224.
Kray, L. J., George, L. G., Liljenquist, K. A., Galinsky, A. D., Tetlock,
P. E., & Roese, N. J. (2010). From what might have been to what
must have been: Counterfactual thinking creates meaning. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 106–118.
Krause, N. (2005). God-mediated control and psychological well-
being in late life. Research on Aging, 27, 136–164.
Lawrence, R. T. (1997). Measuring the image of God: The God image
inventory and the God image scales. Journal of Psychology and
Theology, 25, 214–226.
Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. A., & Schreindorfer, L. S.
(2013). Construct validity of the need to belong scale: Mapping the
nomological network. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95,
610–624.
Mandel, D. R., & Dhami, M. K. (2005). ‘‘What I did’’ versus ‘‘what I
might have done’’: Effect of factual versus counterfactual thinking
on blame, guilt, and shame in prisoners. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 41, 627–635.
McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman,
P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad:
Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and
their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in
students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 474–485.
McGregor, I., Nash, K., & Prentice, M. (2010). Reactive approach
motivation (RAM) for religion. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 99, 148.
McIntosh, D. N., Silver, R. C., & Wortman, C. B. (1993). Religion’s
role in adjustment to a negative life event: Coping with the loss of a
child. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 812–821.
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang,
J. A. (2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and para-
normal belief. Cognition, 123, 335–346.
Phillips, R. E. III, Pargament, K. I., Lynn, Q. K., & Crossley, C. D.
(2004). Self-directing religious coping: A deistic God, abandoning
God, or no God at all? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,
43, 409–418.
Plante, T. G., & Boccaccini, M. (1997). The Santa Clara strength of
religious faith questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 45, 375–387.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for
estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior
Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731.
Roese, N. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805–818.
Roese, N. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin,
121, 133–148.
Roese, N., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Outcome controllability and coun-
terfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
21, 620–628.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Prince-
ton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Sanna, L. J., & Turley, K. J. (1996). Antecedents to spontaneous
counterfactual thinking: Effects of expectancy violation and
outcome valence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
22, 906–919.
Shenhav, A. S., Rand, D. G., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Divine intuition:
Cognitive style influences belief in God. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 141, 423–428.
Soenens, B., Neyrinck, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Dezutter, J., Hutsebaut,
D., & Duriez, B. (2012). How do perceptions of God as autonomy
supportive or controlling relate to individuals’ social-cognitive
processing of religious contents? The role of motives for religious
behavior. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 22,
10–30.
Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory.
Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Weiner, B. (1980). Human Motivation. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart
& Winston.
White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2005). Looking on the bright side:
Downward counterfactual thinking in response to negative life
events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31,
1413–1424.
Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Truths about mediation
analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 197–206.
Author Biographies
Anneke Buffone currently is the Lead Research Scientist and a post-
doctoral fellow at the University of Pennsylvania’s World Well-Being
Project. Dr. Buffone’s research specializes in other-focused motiva-
tions, emotions, and cognitions and its effects on social interactions.
As a researcher, Dr. Buffone gains insights into these processes via var-
ious methods, such as psychophysiology, neuroendocrinology, and
262 Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(3)
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
theory and data-driven linguistic analysis in ‘‘big data’’, such as social
media.
Shira Gabriel is an Associate Professor of Psychology at SUNY, Uni-
versity at Buffalo, Social Psychologist (PhD, Northwestern Univer-
sity), and internationally renowned expert on Social Surrogacy—the
tendency for humans to form psychological relationships with nonhu-
man (on nonavailable) entities. Professor Gabriel has published papers
in prominent, peer-reviewed papers on the importance and nature of
psychological bonds with celebrities, favorite television shows, and
comfort foods. In addition to expanding her research program on
Social Surrogacy, Professor Gabriel also studies social comparison,
friendship processed, and gratitude, and serves as the editor of the
journal Self & Identity.
Michael Poulin is an Associate Professor of Social Psychology in the
Department of Psychology at the University at Buffalo. His research
focuses on stress, coping, altruism, prosocial behavior, and trauma.
Buffone et al. 263
at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
View publication statsView publication stats

More Related Content

What's hot

Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9
Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9
Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9
Joep Oomen
 
Concepts and dimensions of conversion
Concepts and dimensions of conversionConcepts and dimensions of conversion
Concepts and dimensions of conversion
Domenic Marbaniang
 
001123 mt533animistictaiwan
001123 mt533animistictaiwan001123 mt533animistictaiwan
001123 mt533animistictaiwan
Laura Sun
 
Does god want us to believe miracles
Does god want us to believe miraclesDoes god want us to believe miracles
Does god want us to believe miracles
KenPulliam
 

What's hot (11)

1 instructions reply to 2 of your peers below.  must be 250 wo
1 instructions reply to 2 of your peers below.  must be 250 wo1 instructions reply to 2 of your peers below.  must be 250 wo
1 instructions reply to 2 of your peers below.  must be 250 wo
 
Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9
Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9
Victimhood and theodicy in Christianity, Islam and Hinduism v9
 
Gospel nonviolence
Gospel nonviolenceGospel nonviolence
Gospel nonviolence
 
Concepts and dimensions of conversion
Concepts and dimensions of conversionConcepts and dimensions of conversion
Concepts and dimensions of conversion
 
Atheist
AtheistAtheist
Atheist
 
THE IDEOLOGY OF GOD MANIFESTED THROUGHOUT TIME BY HUMANKIND
THE IDEOLOGY OF GOD MANIFESTED THROUGHOUT TIME BY HUMANKINDTHE IDEOLOGY OF GOD MANIFESTED THROUGHOUT TIME BY HUMANKIND
THE IDEOLOGY OF GOD MANIFESTED THROUGHOUT TIME BY HUMANKIND
 
001123 mt533animistictaiwan
001123 mt533animistictaiwan001123 mt533animistictaiwan
001123 mt533animistictaiwan
 
Does god want us to believe miracles
Does god want us to believe miraclesDoes god want us to believe miracles
Does god want us to believe miracles
 
Religious Convocation - The Equally Yoked Couple - Liberal Arts and Humanities
Religious Convocation - The Equally Yoked Couple - Liberal Arts and HumanitiesReligious Convocation - The Equally Yoked Couple - Liberal Arts and Humanities
Religious Convocation - The Equally Yoked Couple - Liberal Arts and Humanities
 
Rene girard & genesis
Rene girard & genesisRene girard & genesis
Rene girard & genesis
 
Religion sin dios dwokin ingles
Religion sin dios dwokin inglesReligion sin dios dwokin ingles
Religion sin dios dwokin ingles
 

Similar to There But for the Grace of God: Counterfactuals Influence Religious Belief and Images of the Divine

Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx
                 Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx                 Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx
Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx
joyjonna282
 
Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01
Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01
Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01
derryck100
 

Similar to There But for the Grace of God: Counterfactuals Influence Religious Belief and Images of the Divine (9)

Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx
                 Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx                 Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx
Is religion a force for goodWhen.docx
 
Spirituality
SpiritualitySpirituality
Spirituality
 
Sociology of religion(Crime and Spirituality)
Sociology of religion(Crime and Spirituality)Sociology of religion(Crime and Spirituality)
Sociology of religion(Crime and Spirituality)
 
Review Paper – Power Point Presentation
Review Paper – Power Point PresentationReview Paper – Power Point Presentation
Review Paper – Power Point Presentation
 
Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01
Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01
Encyclopediaofpsychologyandreligion 110225130507-phpapp01
 
Research-1
Research-1Research-1
Research-1
 
CogSci poster
CogSci posterCogSci poster
CogSci poster
 
Thewindsbeneathyouthswings
ThewindsbeneathyouthswingsThewindsbeneathyouthswings
Thewindsbeneathyouthswings
 
week7assignment2reviewpaperpowerpointpresentation-12717908726025-phpapp02_rec...
week7assignment2reviewpaperpowerpointpresentation-12717908726025-phpapp02_rec...week7assignment2reviewpaperpowerpointpresentation-12717908726025-phpapp02_rec...
week7assignment2reviewpaperpowerpointpresentation-12717908726025-phpapp02_rec...
 

More from Jonathan Dunnemann

The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...
The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...
The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...
Jonathan Dunnemann
 
Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...
Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...
Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...
Jonathan Dunnemann
 
Appropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s Spirituality
Appropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s SpiritualityAppropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s Spirituality
Appropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s Spirituality
Jonathan Dunnemann
 

More from Jonathan Dunnemann (20)

"Black Power Statement"
"Black Power Statement""Black Power Statement"
"Black Power Statement"
 
BOS City Guide to Sacred Spaces in Boston © Sacred Space International Sacred...
BOS City Guide to Sacred Spaces in Boston © Sacred Space International Sacred...BOS City Guide to Sacred Spaces in Boston © Sacred Space International Sacred...
BOS City Guide to Sacred Spaces in Boston © Sacred Space International Sacred...
 
NYC City Guide to Sacred Spaces in New York City © Sacred Space International...
NYC City Guide to Sacred Spaces in New York City © Sacred Space International...NYC City Guide to Sacred Spaces in New York City © Sacred Space International...
NYC City Guide to Sacred Spaces in New York City © Sacred Space International...
 
The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...
The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...
The Theology of Spirituality: It's Growing Importance Amid the Transformation...
 
How children describe the fruits of meditation
How children describe the fruits of meditationHow children describe the fruits of meditation
How children describe the fruits of meditation
 
ASSESSMENT OF CHARACTER STRENGTHS AMONG YOUTH: THE VALUES IN ACTION INVENTORY...
ASSESSMENT OF CHARACTER STRENGTHS AMONG YOUTH: THE VALUES IN ACTION INVENTORY...ASSESSMENT OF CHARACTER STRENGTHS AMONG YOUTH: THE VALUES IN ACTION INVENTORY...
ASSESSMENT OF CHARACTER STRENGTHS AMONG YOUTH: THE VALUES IN ACTION INVENTORY...
 
Youth Living A Life Of Purpose
Youth Living A Life Of Purpose Youth Living A Life Of Purpose
Youth Living A Life Of Purpose
 
African American Spirituality: Through Another Lens
African American Spirituality: Through Another LensAfrican American Spirituality: Through Another Lens
African American Spirituality: Through Another Lens
 
Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...
Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...
Strengths Building, Resilience, and the Bible: A Story-Based Curriculum for A...
 
African American Interpretation
African American InterpretationAfrican American Interpretation
African American Interpretation
 
Appropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s Spirituality
Appropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s SpiritualityAppropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s Spirituality
Appropriating Universality: The Coltranes and 1960s Spirituality
 
God of the Oppressed
God of the OppressedGod of the Oppressed
God of the Oppressed
 
Faith and the Sacred in African American Life
Faith and the Sacred in African American LifeFaith and the Sacred in African American Life
Faith and the Sacred in African American Life
 
Transformative Pedagogy, Black Theology and Participative forms of Praxis
Transformative Pedagogy, Black Theology and Participative forms of PraxisTransformative Pedagogy, Black Theology and Participative forms of Praxis
Transformative Pedagogy, Black Theology and Participative forms of Praxis
 
A Biblical Basis for Social Justice
A Biblical Basis for Social JusticeA Biblical Basis for Social Justice
A Biblical Basis for Social Justice
 
Development of a Program for the Empowerment of Black Single Mother Families ...
Development of a Program for the Empowerment of Black Single Mother Families ...Development of a Program for the Empowerment of Black Single Mother Families ...
Development of a Program for the Empowerment of Black Single Mother Families ...
 
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological IdentitiesBlack Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
 
The Impact and Legacy of the BSU at 50
The Impact and Legacy of the BSU at 50The Impact and Legacy of the BSU at 50
The Impact and Legacy of the BSU at 50
 
Oblate School of Theology
Oblate School of TheologyOblate School of Theology
Oblate School of Theology
 
The Redemptive Self
The Redemptive SelfThe Redemptive Self
The Redemptive Self
 

Recently uploaded

Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...
Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...
Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...
baharayali
 
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...
baharayali
 
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...
baharayali
 
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...
baharayali
 
Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...
Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...
Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...
makhmalhalaaay
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
ZurliaSoop
 
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...
baharayali
 
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...
baharayali
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Zulu - The Epistle of Ignatius to Polycarp.pdf
Zulu - The Epistle of Ignatius to Polycarp.pdfZulu - The Epistle of Ignatius to Polycarp.pdf
Zulu - The Epistle of Ignatius to Polycarp.pdf
 
Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...
Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...
Top Kala Jadu, Bangali Amil baba in Lahore and Kala jadu specialist in Lahore...
 
"The Magnificent Surah Rahman: PDF Version"
"The Magnificent Surah Rahman: PDF Version""The Magnificent Surah Rahman: PDF Version"
"The Magnificent Surah Rahman: PDF Version"
 
NoHo First Good News online newsletter May 2024
NoHo First Good News online newsletter May 2024NoHo First Good News online newsletter May 2024
NoHo First Good News online newsletter May 2024
 
St. Louise de Marillac and Abandoned Children
St. Louise de Marillac and Abandoned ChildrenSt. Louise de Marillac and Abandoned Children
St. Louise de Marillac and Abandoned Children
 
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic specialist in Rawalpindi and Bangali Amil baba ...
 
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala ilam specialist in USA and Bangali Amil baba in Saudi...
 
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...
Popular Kala Jadu, Kala jadu Expert in Islamabad and Kala jadu specialist in ...
 
Jude: The Acts of the Apostates (Jude vv.1-4).pptx
Jude: The Acts of the Apostates (Jude vv.1-4).pptxJude: The Acts of the Apostates (Jude vv.1-4).pptx
Jude: The Acts of the Apostates (Jude vv.1-4).pptx
 
St. Louise de Marillac and Care of the Sick Poor
St. Louise de Marillac and Care of the Sick PoorSt. Louise de Marillac and Care of the Sick Poor
St. Louise de Marillac and Care of the Sick Poor
 
Codex Singularity: Search for the Prisca Sapientia
Codex Singularity: Search for the Prisca SapientiaCodex Singularity: Search for the Prisca Sapientia
Codex Singularity: Search for the Prisca Sapientia
 
Deerfoot Church of Christ Bulletin 5 12 24
Deerfoot Church of Christ Bulletin 5 12 24Deerfoot Church of Christ Bulletin 5 12 24
Deerfoot Church of Christ Bulletin 5 12 24
 
Genesis 1:5 - Meditate the Scripture Daily bit by bit
Genesis 1:5 - Meditate the Scripture Daily bit by bitGenesis 1:5 - Meditate the Scripture Daily bit by bit
Genesis 1:5 - Meditate the Scripture Daily bit by bit
 
The_Chronological_Life_of_Christ_Part_99_Words_and_Works
The_Chronological_Life_of_Christ_Part_99_Words_and_WorksThe_Chronological_Life_of_Christ_Part_99_Words_and_Works
The_Chronological_Life_of_Christ_Part_99_Words_and_Works
 
Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...
Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...
Professional Amil baba, Black magic specialist in Lahore and Kala ilam expert...
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Ponorogo ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
 
St. Louise de Marillac and Galley Prisoners
St. Louise de Marillac and Galley PrisonersSt. Louise de Marillac and Galley Prisoners
St. Louise de Marillac and Galley Prisoners
 
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...
Certified Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in Faisalabad and Kala ilam specialis...
 
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...
Famous Kala Jadu, Black magic expert in UK and Kala ilam expert in Saudi Arab...
 
Lesson 6 - Our Spiritual Weapons - SBS.pptx
Lesson 6 - Our Spiritual Weapons - SBS.pptxLesson 6 - Our Spiritual Weapons - SBS.pptx
Lesson 6 - Our Spiritual Weapons - SBS.pptx
 

There But for the Grace of God: Counterfactuals Influence Religious Belief and Images of the Divine

  • 1. See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/298799146 There But for the Grace of God: Counterfactuals Influence Religious Belief and Images of the Divine Article  in  Social Psychological and Personality Science · April 2016 DOI: 10.1177/1948550616632576 CITATIONS 3 READS 149 3 authors, including: Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Social Media Analytics for Well-being View project Big data insights on stress: The language of stress in individuals and communities View project Anneke Buffone University of Pennsylvania 17 PUBLICATIONS   102 CITATIONS    SEE PROFILE Michael Poulin University at Buffalo, The State University of New York 38 PUBLICATIONS   2,023 CITATIONS    SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Anneke Buffone on 31 August 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
  • 2. Article There But for the Grace of God: Counterfactuals Influence Religious Belief and Images of the Divine Anneke Buffone1,2 , Shira Gabriel2 , and Michael Poulin2 Abstract Past research suggests that thinking counterfactually leads to a perception that major life events are fated or predetermined. We hypothesized that a perception that events are fated will activate perceptions that God played a role in the event, which will increase feelings of religiousness. Because most people view God as a positive influence, we hypothesized that this would only happen when imagining how events might have turned out worse (i.e., downward counterfactuals). Across two experiments, we examined the hypothesis that thinking counterfactually increases religiosity. The studies supported our predictions. Generating counterfactuals increased belief in God and religiosity across several variables. Furthermore, evidence was found for the proposed pathway. In summary, the studies provided strong and consistent empirical support for counterfactual thinking’s role in religious faith and for this effect to be due to increased perceptions of God’ role in the event. Implications for religion, cognition, and counterfactual thinking are discussed. Keywords counterfactual thinking, religion, attitudes, attribution, interpersonal relationships, counterfactual direction, fate Psalm 124 2 If the Lord had not been on our side when men attacked us, 3 when their anger flared against us, they would have swal- lowed us alive; ( . . . ) Despite historical trends toward less religiosity, most people still believe in God (Gallup, 2011). The current research is concerned with the age-old question of how believers find evidence in their lives for the existence of God. We propose that counterfactual thinking, (i.e., imagining how life would be different if a given event had not occurred) allows believers to find evidence that God plays a role in their lives. Counterfactual thinking is defined as ‘‘if-then’’ statements about how a series of events could have had an altered outcome if an event had not occurred or had occurred differently (Roese, 1997; Roese & Olson, 1995). For example, Jane might think, ‘‘if I hadn’t taken the bus that day, I never would have met my husband.’’ Thinking counterfactually is a commonly used reflection strategy, with implications for problem solving and affect management (Epstude & Roese, 2008; Roese, 1994). In addition, counterfactual thinking about past autobiographi- cal events increases awareness of unknown causes and surpris- ing connections (Koo, Algoe, Wilson, & Gilbert, 2008) and perceptions of the event being fated (Kray et al., 2010; McA- dams, Reynolds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001; Sanna & Turley, 1996). It is the influence of counterfactual thinking on perceptions of fatedness that we propose plays an integral role in increasing religiosity. Merriam-Webster defines fate as ‘‘a power that is believed to control what happens in the future’’ (‘‘Fate,’’ 2015). Thus, the question arises, if fate implies that some power is controlling the events in one’s life, what is that power? Sur- vey research suggests that, for the majority of people in the world today, power is thought to be God (Gallup, 2011). Thus, we propose that when counterfactual thinking leads to an increase in fatedness, that increase in fatednesss will lead to an increased perception that God is playing a role in one’s life. Finally, a perception that God is playing a role in one’s life should lead to increased religiosity. Support for the role of counterfactual thinking in increasing religiosity comes from practices encouraged by major religions (Bender, 2008). For example, in Catholicism, the Examen encourages reflective prayer each night in order to perceive divine influence in the day’s events. Protestant religions have 1 Department of Psychology, Positive Psychology Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 2 Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Buffalo, NY, USA Corresponding Author: Anneke Buffone, Department of Psychology, Positive Psychology Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA. Email: buffonea@sas.upenn.edu Social Psychological and Personality Science 2016, Vol. 7(3) 256-263 ª The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1948550616632576 spps.sagepub.com at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 3. developed programs encouraging believers to journal each night about the day’s events to deduce evidence for divine responsiveness (Bender, 2008). The Jewish Passover song and prayer ‘‘Dayenu’’ encourages Jews to find evidence of God’s presence by imagining how life would have been different if God had not intervened. In Islam, Mohammed encourages believers to reflect on ordinary events, such as the occurrence of rain, to recognize Allah’s benevolence. In Hinduism, believ- ers are encouraged to see everyday occurrences as manifesta- tions of the Divine. In sum, all the current major theistic religions encourage believers to reflect on the events in their lives to find evidence for the existence of God. By imagining how their days might have been different, religious practi- tioners are able to perceive the ways that God intervened. The Present Research Two studies were run to test the hypothesis that downward counterfactual thinking leads to an increase in religiosity.1 In both studies, a model was tested in which generating counter- factuals leads to a perception of fatedness which leads to a belief in God’s role that leads to increased religiosity (see Figure 1). Pretesting suggested that our participants tend to see God as a benevolent, helpful presence. Specifically, we surveyed 434 participants and found that 71% of participants agreed with ‘‘I believe that if there is a God, he is most likely loving and caring and tries to make the best happen for people.’’ In addi- tion, most religions appear to hold up an image of a benevolent deity, the Bible states that ‘‘the Lord is kind and merciful’’ (Psalm 103); throughout the Quran, Allah is constantly referred to as ‘‘the merciful, the compassionate’’; and in Hinduism, God is the source of happiness and love. Thus, one would predict that evidence for God’s hand in believers’ lives should be found when imagining how one’s life would be worse if an event had not happened (i.e., downward counterfactuals) and not when imagining how one’s life would have been if the event had not happened (i.e., upward counterfactuals). Study 1 manipulated counterfactual direction to examine that hypothesis. Study 1 also examined both positive and negative events. Prior research on counterfactuals most often limits the focus on either positive or, more commonly, negative events, as these are more likely to elicit counterfactual thought (Burrus & Roese, 2005; Mandel & Dhami, 2005; Roese, 1997). Some work however has found that valence affects counterfactual reflection: Upward counterfactuals are more common in response to negative events and vice versa (Koo et al., 2008; White & Lehman, 2005). Yet downward counterfactuals can sometimes be generated in response to negative events, espe- cially if individuals are motivated to make sense of a negative event by benefit finding (Helgeson, Reynolds, & Tomich, 2006)—that is, focusing on an event’s unexpected positive out- comes or its ‘‘silver lining.’’ In fact, religiosity is associated with greater levels of spontaneous benefit finding (McIntosh, Silver, & Worman, 1993), suggesting that downward counter- factual thinking about negative events may fit well with reli- gious belief. Given the conceptual and empirical separability of counterfactual direction and event valence, we expected that imagining alternate—specifically, worse—realities to positive or negative life events should equally make individuals per- ceive a benevolent God’s influence. Study 1 Method Participants We recruited 292 (92 female) undergraduate students2 diverse in ethnicity (19 African/African American, 48 Asian/Asian American, 11 Hispanic/Hispanic American, 4 Arab/Arab American, 2 Native American, and 208 European/European American) and belief (22 Jewish, 18 Protestant, 104 Catholic, 4 Lutheran, 32 Other Christian, 10 Muslim, 8 Hindu, 26 Athe- ist, 22 Nonbeliever, 21 Agnostic, and 23 Spiritual/Nonreli- gious). Twelve individuals had to be excluded from the analyses due to not following instructions. Materials and Procedures While seated in individual cubicles, unaware that the experi- ment examined religion, participants wrote about a positive or negative life event either upward or downward counterfac- tually or without the use of counterfactuals, completed a mea- sures of religious faith, measures of fate and whether participants felt the ‘‘hand of God’’ in the event. Essay task. Participants were instructed to select and describe either an important negative or an important positive event from their past (Kray et al., 2010). Subsequently, one third of participants per group were instructed to write a coherent story about how life would be better (upward) and one third about how life would be worse (downward) if this event had never Downward Counterfactuals Perceived Fatedness PercepƟons of God's Role Religious Faith Figure 1. Proposed model for the effects of downward counterfactual thinking on religious faith by way of perceptions of fate and the role of God. Buffone et al. 257 at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 4. occurred. A final third was asked to simply describe the event in more detail. Questionnaires. Subsequently participants completed the follow- ing questionnaires. Strength of Religious Faith. Participants completed the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire (Plante & Boccaccini, 1997) assessed on a 7-point scale (1 ¼ completely disagree; 7 ¼ completely agree), featuring items such as ‘‘I look at my faith as a source of inspiration.’’3 Comparative Strength of Religious Faith. Participants were asked ‘‘How strongly do you believe in God compared to most other people you know’’ (1 ¼ not at all strongly; 7 ¼ extremely strongly). Perceptions of own religious behavior. We also assessed per- ceived religious behavior by asking how often participants recalled attending a place of worship (1 ¼ never; 7 ¼ daily) and how frequently they prayed (1 ¼ never; 7 ¼ several times a day). Image of God. We included a shortened version of the God Image Scales (Lawrence, 1997) assessing perceptions of God as an attachment figure. Fatedness. Further participants completed an item identical to the one used to measure fatedness by Kray and col- leagues (2010), asking participants to indicate whether they perceived the event as ‘‘a product of fate’’ as well as an item asking how much the event appeared ‘‘meant to be,’’ both assessed on a 7-point scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree; 7 ¼ strongly agree). Hand of God in Experience. We assessed the degree to which participants felt the influence of God in their event by ask- ing them to indicate, on a 7-point scale, their agreement with the single item ‘‘I felt the hand of God in this life experience.’’ Results and Discussion Event Valence Our primary hypothesis was that downward counterfactual thinking would increase strength of religious faith, perceptions of fatedness, and perceptions of God’s causal influence in the life event compared to both upward counterfactual and factual reflection. We included event valence in our design to see whether effects would differ depending on whether events were positive or negative. Thus, all results were examined with 3 (reflection type: downward counterfactual vs. upward counter- factual vs. factual) Â 2 (event valence: positive vs. negative event) analyses of variance. However, we were only interested in the effects for reflection type, and none of our analyses showed any significant main effects or interactions for event valence. Therefore, for clarity of report, only results for reflec- tion type are reported. Counterfactuals and Religious Faith Given the high degree of conceptual similarity among all of the religion items measured (tapping strength of religious faith, comparative strength of religious faith, perceptions of own reli- gious behavior, and images of God), we first examined whether it was defensible to combine all of these items into a single index to avoid capitalizing on Type-I error. We computed a sin- gle Religious Faith Scale that was the mean of all of the above items, and this scale had excellent internal consistency (a ¼ .98) so was used as the single religiosity variable in the following analyses. However, analyses conducted using the above scales separately yielded substantively similar results for each. We examined effects of the manipulations on our religious faith scale, and in line with predictions, a significant main effect emerged for kind of reflection, F(2, 274) ¼ 3.88, p ¼ .02, Z2 p ¼ .03. Downward counterfactual reflection (M ¼ 3.90, SD ¼ 1.42) produced marginally higher religious faith than factual reflection (M ¼ 3.50, SD ¼ 1.47; p ¼ .06), mean difference 0.40, 95% confidence interval [CI] ¼ [À0.01, 0.82], and significantly higher religious faith than upward counterfactual reflection (M ¼ 3.31, SD ¼ 1.47; p ¼ .007), mean difference ¼ À0.59, 95% CI ¼ [À0.17, À1.01]. Upward and factual reflection did not differ from one another ( p ¼ .39), mean difference ¼ À0.19, 95% CI ¼ [À0.61, 0.24]. Mediation by Fatedness and Hand of God In order to test the hypothesis that downward counterfac- tuals increased religious faith by increasing fatedness per- ceptions, which in turn increased belief in God’s role in the event (see Figure 1), we conducted the following three steps. First, we tested whether the contrast of downward counterfactuals versus other conditions predicted each of the proposed mediators, fatedness perceptions, and the hand of God. Second, we tested the indirect effect corresponding to each of the intermediate mediational paths (downward counterfactuals ! fatedness ! hand of God; fatedness ! hand of God ! religious faith). Finally, we estimated the overall indirect effect corresponding to the total mediated effect (downward counterfactuals ! fatedness ! hand of God ! religious faith). Downward counterfactuals affecting Fatedness and hand of God. As predicted, downward counterfactuals predicted higher levels of fatedness perceptions (M ¼ 4.01, SD ¼ 1.67) versus other reflection conditions (upward counterfactuals and factual reflection; M ¼ 3.56, SD ¼ 1.77; p ¼ .04; mean differ- ence ¼ À0.45, 95% CI ¼ [À0.88, À0.01]). In addition, down- ward counterfactuals predicted higher perceptions of the hand of God in the event (M ¼ 3.60, SD ¼ 1.92) versus other reflection conditions (M ¼ 2.82, SD ¼ 1.79; p < .001; mean difference ¼ À0.79, 95% CI ¼ [À1.24, À0.32]). 258 Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(3) at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 5. Fatedness mediating the effect of downward counterfactuals on hand of God. Fatedness and hand of God perceptions were sig- nificantly correlated (r ¼ .44, p < .001). Following guidelines recommended by Preacher and Hayes (2004; see also Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010), we performed bootstrapped estimates of the indirect effect between downward counterfactuals and hand of God by way of fatedness using Stata sgmediation and bootstrap modules. Results (performed with 5,000 bootstrap samples) indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.20, 95% CI ¼ [0.001, 0.41], z ¼ 1.97, p ¼ .049). These results suggest that downward counterfactuals may increase hand of God perceptions by leading to perceptions that the life event is fated. Hand of God mediating the association between fatedness and religious faith. Fatedness perceptions and religious faith were associated (r ¼ .30, p < .001), and unsurprisingly, religious faith and hand of God perceptions were also significantly cor- related (r ¼ .68, p < .001). As above, we performed boot- strapped estimates of the indirect effect between fatedness and religious faith by way of hand of God. Results (performed with 5,000 bootstrap samples) indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.25, 95% CI ¼ [0.19, 0.32], z ¼ 7.55, p < .001). These results suggest that fatedness percep- tions may predict religious faith specifically by leading to per- ceptions of the hand of God. Testing two-step mediation. Finally, we obtained the overall indi- rect effect corresponding to the total-mediated effect (down- ward counterfactuals ! fatedness ! hand of God ! religious faith). To do so, we used Stata’s sureg (seemingly unrelated regression) command to obtain estimates of all direct and indirect effects and then used the bootstrap command to obtain 5,000 bootstrapped estimates of the overall indirect effect. Results indicated an indirect slope marginally greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.12, 95% CI ¼ [À0.01, À0.25], z ¼ 1.86, p ¼ .06). These results lend support to the hypothesis that downward counterfactuals predict religious faith specifi- cally by leading to perceptions of fatedness, which in turn pre- dict perceptions of the hand of God and, in turn, religious faith. In summary, Study 1 found consistent support for the hypothesis that counterfactual thinking plays an important role in belief in God. Thinking about downward counterfactuals led to an increased perception of fate that led to an increased per- ception of the hand of God, which led to an increase in religious faith. In other words, Study 1 suggested that counterfactual thinking leads believers to the belief that an event did not occur by chance alone, then individuals search for a likely source of this fate, which leads them to God, which then increases their religious faith. This occurred regardless of whether the event was positive or negative in valence. The effects were limited to downward counterfactuals, or thinking about the ways that life might have been worse. This makes sense, given the positive view most of our participants have about God. Furthermore, the prominence of downward counterfactuals makes it unlikely that uncertainty reduction played a role in the link between counterfactuals and faith. It is possible that participants in Study 1 felt less certain of the world due to the counterfactual thinking and the uncertainty (and not perceptions of the hand of God) led to the increased religiosity. However, if anything, one might expect upward counterfactuals to be more related to uncertainty than down- ward because they are more closely related to negative emo- tions and less closely related to positive emotions (e.g., Epstude & Roese, 2008; Inzlicht, McGregor, Hirsh, & Nash, 2009; Inzlicht & Tullet, 2010; McGregor, Nash, & Prentice, 2010; Roese, 1994; Sanna & Turley, 1996). Thus, the finding that upward counterfactuals were completely unrelated to increased religiosity makes it much less likely that uncertainty reduction played a causal role. In Study 2, we hoped to replicate the results of Study 1 with a noncollege student sample. In addition, Study 2 utilized dif- ferent measures of perception of God’s influence on the event and fatedness to ensure that results were not limited to spe- cific items. In addition, because the effects were only found for downward counterfactuals in Study 1 and did not differ based on the valence of the event, only downward counterfac- tuals and positive events were utilized to increase statistical power. Study 2 Method Participants We recruited 108 (61 female; 1 questioning) diverse individu- als (Mage ¼ 35.7 years) via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (13 African/African American, 5 Asian/Asian American, 6 Hispa- nic/Hispanic American, 1 Arab/Arab American, 1 Native American, and 82 European/European American) and belief (2 Jewish, 20 Protestant, 15 Catholic, 1 Muslim, 0 Hindu, 13 Nondenominational, 14 Other Christian, 16 Agnostic, 14 Athe- ist, 5 Nonbeliever, 5 Spiritual/Nonreligious, and 3 Other). Users were restricted to be based in the United States, a hit approval rate of .99 as well as a minimum of 1,000 approved hits via Mturk’s settings. The final sample included 99 partici- pants, after 11 had to be excluded from the analyses, because they did not describe a (positive) life event (n ¼ 7) or the life event they chose to write about was becoming religious itself (n ¼ 2). Materials and Procedures Participants completed a writing prompt of either writing in downward counterfactuals or writing factually about a positive life event and then completed some potential mediator items followed by the religious faith-dependent measures. Essay task. The essay task was identical to Study 1 but only included the positive event condition and the downward coun- terfactual and no counterfactual conditions. Buffone et al. 259 at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 6. Questionnaires. The religious faith questions from Study 1 were used, with the exception that the God Image scales were not added, as differing beliefs about God’s character are of less importance to the question of divine influence. As in Study 1, the resulting scale had excellent internal consistency (a ¼ .98).4 Fate’s role. ‘‘How much would you say was the event con- trolled by fate?’’ as well as ‘‘How much was fate responsible for the event’s occurrence?’’ were each assessed on 7-point scales (a ¼ .75). God’s role. How much did you feel that God took an active role in how the life event unfolded?’’ ‘‘How much did you per- ceive God’s guidance in the life event?’’ ‘‘I relied on God for guidance in this life event,’’ ‘‘God worked through me or through others in making this positive life event happen for me,’’ ‘‘God worked together with me to make this life event happen,’’ ‘‘How much was the event you described controlled by God?’’ ‘‘How much was God responsible for the event’s occurrence?’’ were each assessed on 7-point scales (a ¼ .98). Further mediators. In addition to fatedness and God’s role, we assessed further potential mediators including meaning, how unlikely participants found the event; how easy it would be to imagine alternate outcomes to the life event; whether the event was controlled or caused by the self, chance, or other peo- ple; and if other people or chance were responsible for the event’s occurrence. None of these variables proved to be media- tors of the observed effect linking downward counterfactuals to increased faith using a bootstrapping approach. We also assessed alternative outcome variables, such as optimism, death anxiety, life satisfaction, self-esteem, and flourishing, which were not predicted by counterfactual thinking in these data. Results and Discussion Counterfactuals and Religious Faith Downward counterfactual reflection (M ¼ 4.04, SD ¼ 2.12) produced higher religious faith than factual reflection (M ¼ 3.11, SD ¼ 2.24; p ¼ .04), mean difference ¼ À0.93, 95% CI ¼ [À1.81, À0.05]. Mediation by Fate’s Role and God’s Role In order to test the hypothesis that downward counterfactuals increased religious faith by increasing fatedness perceptions, which in turn increased belief in God’s role in the event, we conducted the same three steps as in Study 1. We tested (1) whether downward counterfactuals predicted each of the proposed mediators, fate’s role, and God’s role; (2) the indirect effect corresponding to each of the intermediate mediational paths; and (3) the overall indirect effect corresponding to the total-mediated effect. Downward counterfactuals affecting fate’s role and God’s role. As predicted, downward counterfactuals predicted higher perceptions of fate’s role (M ¼ 4.62, SD ¼ 1.82) versus factual reflection (M ¼ 3.63, SD ¼ 1.85; p ¼ .01; mean difference ¼ À0.98, 95% CI ¼ [À1.72, À0.24]). In addition, downward counterfactuals predicted marginally higher perceptions of God’s role in the event (M ¼ 3.74, SD ¼ 2.23) versus factual reflection (M ¼ 2.93, SD ¼ 2.20; p ¼ .08; mean difference ¼ À0.81, 95% CI ¼ [À1.70, 0.09]). Fatedness mediating the effect of downward counterfactuals on hand of God. Perceptions of fate’s role and God’s role were indeed significantly correlated (r ¼ .45, p < .001). Bootstrapped estimates of the indirect effect (using 5,000 bootstrap samples) indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.50, 95% CI ¼ [0.09, 0.91], z ¼ 2.41, p ¼ .02). Downward counter- factuals may increase perceptions of God’s role in an event spe- cifically by leading to perceptions of fate’s role in the event. Hand of God mediating the association between fatedness and religious faith. Perceptions of fate’s role and religious faith were indeed associated (r ¼ .34, p < .001), and religious faith and perceptions of God’s role were also significantly correlated (r ¼ .85, p < .001). Bootstrapped estimates of the indirect effect between fate’s role and religious faith by way of God’s role (using 5,000 bootstrap samples) indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.46, 95% CI ¼ [0.29, 0.64], z ¼ 5.17, p < .001). These results suggest that perceptions of fate’s role may predict religious faith specifically by leading to perceptions of God’s role in an event. Testing two-step mediation. Finally, we obtained the overall indi- rect effect corresponding to the total mediated effect (down- ward counterfactuals ! fate’s role ! God’s role ! religious faith), using Stata’s sureg and bootstrap commands as in Study 1. Results indicated an indirect slope greater than 0 (indirect B ¼ 0.48, 95% CI ¼ [0.08, 0.88], z ¼ 2.37, p ¼ .02). These results suggest that downward counterfactuals may predict reli- gious faith specifically by leading to perceptions of fate’s role in an event, which in turn predicts perceptions of God’s role in the event and, in turn, religious faith. In Summary, the results of Study 2 were highly consistent with those of Study 1 utilizing a nonstudent sample and differ- ent measures. Writing about downward counterfactuals from a positive life event led participants to feel as if fate played a role in the event, which led to a feeling that God played a role in the event that led to increased religiosity. General Discussion Across two studies, we found support for the hypothesis that counterfactual thinking leads to an increase in religious faith. Furthermore, we found that this effect is driven particularly by downward counterfactuals (Study 1). In addition, across both studies, generating downward counterfactuals about the life event led to a perception that the event was fated which led to a perception that God played a role in the event which led to increased religiosity. 260 Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(3) at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 7. The current research has important implications for views of religious cognition. The predominant view on cognitive styles and religiosity is that intuitive thinking is associated with reli- giosity (Shenhav, Rand, & Greene, 2012), whereas analytic reasoning is associated with nonreligiosity (Gervais & Noren- zayan, 2012; Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli, Koehler, & Fugelsang, 2012). In other words, belief in God is initiatively appealing, but rationally unlikely, so individuals are most likely to believe in God when they rely on their intuitions at the expense of more rational cognition. However, such differences in intuitive ver- sus deliberative processing cannot fully account for individual differences in belief in God because humans rarely hold beliefs for which they are unable to generate at least some deliberative evidence (Heider, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965; Weiner, 1974, 1980). The current work suggests a mechanism by which indi- viduals can generate, test, and find evidence for hypotheses about God—by seeing the way that God has made a difference in their lives. It is possible that the current findings may be truer among some individuals than others and among some religions more than others. In the psychology of religion literature, the form in which believers perceive God to take control more generally has been found to vary on a continuum of all or specific life events being completely controlled by a deity or the deity tak- ing control collaboratively together with the self (Berrenberg, 1987; Krause, 2005). Furthermore, while some believers view God as not interfering in life events at all, some believe that God is controlling all life events, and again others view God as a collaborative force (Soenens et al., 2012). In addition, believers sometimes perceive God as supportive, but other times as absent (e.g., Phillips, Pargament, Lynn, & Crossley, 2004). Finally, some major religions do not believe in a deity at all. Thus, it may be possible that the effects of downward counterfactuals on religious beliefs may be moderated by kind of religious orientation. Future work would be necessary to examine this interesting possibility. In addition, different results may have been found if differ- ent kinds of counterfactual thoughts had been utilized. Parti- cipants in our studies were asked to think about major life events. Previsions research suggests that thinking counterfac- tually about those kinds of events is particularly likely to lead to beliefs about fatedness (Burrus & Roese, 2005). Thus, if participants had generated counterfactuals about more mun- dane or recent events, it is less likely that they would perceive the role of fate and in turn feel more religious. Again, future research would be necessary to examine that possible moderator. The current work suggests not only how religious faith can be built and bolstered; it may also shed light on how it may fal- ter. Individuals who have experienced prolonged adversity that did not yield any apparent benefit or silver lining may have dif- ficulty generating downward counterfactuals and thus be unable to find evidence for God’s role in their lives. This inabil- ity to find evidence for God’s presence, in particular when they fell that they need God the most, may lead to a decrease in faith. Thus, counterfactuals may play a role in the dissolution of religious faith as well. Further research would be necessary to examine that hypothesis. This work also increases understanding of the importance of counterfactual thinking, by demonstrating how important coun- terfactual thinking may be in daily life, providing a means by which people not only perceive the role of fate but find evi- dence for the existence of God in their lives. Thus, this work builds on an already strong literature that suggests the impor- tance of counterfactual thinking. In summary, our research suggests that people find evidence for divine intervention in their lives through counterfactual thinking, consistent with a view of all humans as naive scien- tists, testing their theories of the world against their everyday experience. It takes us one step closer to understanding reli- gious cognition and the importance of counterfactual thinking in the human experience. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author- ship, and/or publication of this article. Notes 1. In an initial study (n ¼ 118), we found evidence for counterfactual thinking (regardless of direction) to increase religious faith. Since this study did not add anything substantially and due to length lim- itations for the article, we excluded it from this article. The study does however provide a conceptual, more general replication of the effects that are reported here. 2. Given that these studies are the first to demonstrate an associ- ation between counterfactuals and religious belief, the effect size for this association, if any, was previously unknown. In focal analyses across both studies, this effect size was estimated to be Z2 ¼.02 and .04, respectively. Post hoc analyses suggest that the power of these studies to detect these effect sizes was .54 and .55, respectively. 3. We also assessed several control measures including need for cog- nition (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982), self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965), and need to belong (Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, & Schreindorfer, 2013). Also, in order to ensure that our effects would not be due to direct impacts of the life events on faith, we assessed whether participants felt that the event had directly affected their religious beliefs (‘‘The life event made me more/less religious,’’ ‘‘The life event made me less religious,’’ and ‘‘My faith helped me deal with my life experience’’). References Bender, C. (2008). How does God answer back? Poetics, 36, 476–492. Berrenberg, J. L. (1987). The belief in personal control scale: A mea- sure of God-mediated and exaggerated control. Journal of person- ality assessment, 51, 194–206. Buffone et al. 261 at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 8. Burrus, J., & Roese, N. J. (2006). Long ago it was meant to be: The interplay between time, construal, and fate beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1050–1058. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131. Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counter- factual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 168–192. Fate. (2015). Merriam-Webster. Retrieved from http://www.merriam- webster.com.com Gallup. (2011). More than 9 in 10 Americans continue to believe in god. Retrieved from http://www.gallup.com/poll/147887/ameri- cans-continue-believe-god.aspx Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking pro- motes religious disbelief. Science, 336, 493–496. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley. Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., & Tomich, P. L. (2006). A meta- analytic review of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consult- ing and Clinical Psychology, 74, 797–816. Inzlicht, M., & Tullett, A. M. (2010). Reflecting on God: Religious primes can reduce neurophysiological response to errors. Psycho- logical Science, 21, 1184–1190. Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: the attri- bution process in social psychology. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 219–266). New York, NY: Academic Press. Koo, M., Algoe, S. B., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). It’s a wonderful life: Mentally subtracting positive events improves peo- ple’s affective states, contrary to their affective forecasts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1217–1224. Kray, L. J., George, L. G., Liljenquist, K. A., Galinsky, A. D., Tetlock, P. E., & Roese, N. J. (2010). From what might have been to what must have been: Counterfactual thinking creates meaning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 106–118. Krause, N. (2005). God-mediated control and psychological well- being in late life. Research on Aging, 27, 136–164. Lawrence, R. T. (1997). Measuring the image of God: The God image inventory and the God image scales. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 25, 214–226. Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. A., & Schreindorfer, L. S. (2013). Construct validity of the need to belong scale: Mapping the nomological network. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95, 610–624. Mandel, D. R., & Dhami, M. K. (2005). ‘‘What I did’’ versus ‘‘what I might have done’’: Effect of factual versus counterfactual thinking on blame, guilt, and shame in prisoners. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 627–635. McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman, P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 474–485. McGregor, I., Nash, K., & Prentice, M. (2010). Reactive approach motivation (RAM) for religion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 148. McIntosh, D. N., Silver, R. C., & Wortman, C. B. (1993). Religion’s role in adjustment to a negative life event: Coping with the loss of a child. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 812–821. Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and para- normal belief. Cognition, 123, 335–346. Phillips, R. E. III, Pargament, K. I., Lynn, Q. K., & Crossley, C. D. (2004). Self-directing religious coping: A deistic God, abandoning God, or no God at all? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 43, 409–418. Plante, T. G., & Boccaccini, M. (1997). The Santa Clara strength of religious faith questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 45, 375–387. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. Roese, N. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805–818. Roese, N. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 133–148. Roese, N., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Outcome controllability and coun- terfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 620–628. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Prince- ton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Sanna, L. J., & Turley, K. J. (1996). Antecedents to spontaneous counterfactual thinking: Effects of expectancy violation and outcome valence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 906–919. Shenhav, A. S., Rand, D. G., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in God. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 423–428. Soenens, B., Neyrinck, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Dezutter, J., Hutsebaut, D., & Duriez, B. (2012). How do perceptions of God as autonomy supportive or controlling relate to individuals’ social-cognitive processing of religious contents? The role of motives for religious behavior. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 22, 10–30. Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Weiner, B. (1980). Human Motivation. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2005). Looking on the bright side: Downward counterfactual thinking in response to negative life events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1413–1424. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 197–206. Author Biographies Anneke Buffone currently is the Lead Research Scientist and a post- doctoral fellow at the University of Pennsylvania’s World Well-Being Project. Dr. Buffone’s research specializes in other-focused motiva- tions, emotions, and cognitions and its effects on social interactions. As a researcher, Dr. Buffone gains insights into these processes via var- ious methods, such as psychophysiology, neuroendocrinology, and 262 Social Psychological and Personality Science 7(3) at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 9. theory and data-driven linguistic analysis in ‘‘big data’’, such as social media. Shira Gabriel is an Associate Professor of Psychology at SUNY, Uni- versity at Buffalo, Social Psychologist (PhD, Northwestern Univer- sity), and internationally renowned expert on Social Surrogacy—the tendency for humans to form psychological relationships with nonhu- man (on nonavailable) entities. Professor Gabriel has published papers in prominent, peer-reviewed papers on the importance and nature of psychological bonds with celebrities, favorite television shows, and comfort foods. In addition to expanding her research program on Social Surrogacy, Professor Gabriel also studies social comparison, friendship processed, and gratitude, and serves as the editor of the journal Self & Identity. Michael Poulin is an Associate Professor of Social Psychology in the Department of Psychology at the University at Buffalo. His research focuses on stress, coping, altruism, prosocial behavior, and trauma. Buffone et al. 263 at UNIV OF PENNSYLVANIA on March 18, 2016spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from View publication statsView publication stats