SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  46
The purpose of this chapter

• To develop a theory-based approach to how color should be
  used in design.
THE COLOR-PROCESSING MACHINERY


  There two basic types of light receptors in the retina at the back of
  the eyeball:
  • Rods
  • Cones

  Rods:
  • The most numerous type
  • Specialized for very low light levels.

  Cones:
  • The basis for normal daytime vision
  • Three subtypes—short-wavelength sensitive, middle-wavelength
    sensitive, and long-wavelength sensitive.
Cones in the fovea where there are no rods. Those sensitive to
short, medium, and long wavelengths are colored blue, green, and
red, respectively

(Images from David R. Williams, University of Rochester)
Cone receptors are sensitive to light having wavelengths between
400 and 700 nanometers.

Note: L and M overlap. The brain takes the difference between the
L and M to get useful hue information.

It is impossible to reproduce pure spectral colors with printing inks
so the color bands is only approximate.
A consequence of our having few and weak blue-sensitive cones is that
 it is a mistake to show text or anything else with detail using blue on a
 dark background. The result is illegible.




A related problem—Yellow
Yellow can be almost as light as white, and small yellow text on a white
background is extremely hard to read, but pure yellow is very distinct on
a black background.
OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY

 Neural networks add and subtract the cone signals in different
 ways, transforming them into what are called the color-opponent
 channels.

 There are three channels:
    • red-green (diff. bet M- and L-wavelength)
    • yellow-blue (diff. bet luminance and blue)
    • black-white (or luminance, combine M and L)
OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY

Red-green channel
• represents the difference between the signal from the middle-and
  long-wavelength-sensitive cones.

Yellow-blue channel
• represents the differences between the luminance channel and
  the blue cone signals.

Black-white channels
• Combines two type of cone information
• Differentiates in a spatial sense.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES




 Contrast.
 Simultaneous contrast occurs in each of the channels.

 Lightness/Brightness contrast—occurs in the black-white channel.
 Chromatic contrast—occurs in either the red-green or yellow-blue
 channels. Its effects are much more complex and harder to predict
 than grey-scale contrast.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES




 Unique hues.
 Black, white, red, green, yellow, and blue
 • occurs when there is a strong signal on one of the channels
   and a neutral signal on the other two channels.
 • the most commonly used no matter what the language.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES




Sensitivity to spatial detail.
The luminance channel has much greater capacity to convey detailed
information than the chromatic channel.
The fine pattern is much harder to see expressed through chromatic
differences than when expressed through black-white differences.

Motion.
The luminance channel can convey motion information much more
effectively than the chromatic channels.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES

 Stereoscopic depth.
 This is the kind of depth information we get from having two eyes.
 The brain’s processing of stereoscopic depth information is done
 via the luminance channel.



Shape-from-shading.
The luminance channel can
process shape-from-shading, whereas
the chromatic channels cannot.
In the illustration right, a black and white photograph of a shaded
surface has been doctored so that the black-white sequence has
been transformed into a red-green sequence. In this way, the
topography of the shape has become virtually meaningless.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES


Saturation.
Saturated colors
• Vivid
• Have strong signals on one or both chromatic channels.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES

 Maximum saturation varies with luminance.
 When colors are dark
 • there is a smaller difference signal on the chromatic channels
 • saturations are lower for dark colors.

 When colors are light
 • there is also usually a reduction in saturation for reasons having to
   do with color reproduction technology rather than perception.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES

Luminance, lightness, brightness.
   Luminance
       • physical amount of light.
   Lightness
       • used when discussing surface colors
   Brightness
       • generally used when discussing emitted light.

Lightness and brightness are both
• the amount of light coming from a particular part of the visual field
• signals that affect the luminance channel.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES
Luminance nonlinearity.
The luminance channel is nonlinear. We are more sensitive to dark gray
differences than to light differences.

With a dark gray background, our sensitivity is increased to gray
differences in the midrange.
With a light background, our sensitivity is increased to differences near
white. This effect is sometimes called sharpening.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES

 Color segmentation.
 The visual system has a strong tendency to segment smoothly
 changing colors into regions consisting of the unique hues.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES

Color blindness.
People who are color blind (about 8 percent of males)
• missing the red-green channel
• they have a two-dimensional rather than a three-dimensional color
  space.
• they can still distinguish colors that differ in the yellow-blue direction
• their ability to see gray-scale is unaffected.
Yellow-blue color blindness is much less common.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES

Chromatic color as a property of surfaces.
Major function of visual processing
• let us perceive the properties of surfaces in the environment, rather
  than the amount of light coming from those surfaces.

Adaptation in the receptors means that we are relatively insensitive to
overall changes in light levels.
Contrast mechanisms make us more sensitive to local changes in
surface colors, rather than differences in light and shade across a scene.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES




Color and attention.
The pattern-finding mechanisms of the brain can be tuned for color.
This allows for rapid visual searches for things that are of particular
colors.

But color search also depends how many other colors there are in the
environment. If all the world is gray, a patch of vivid color pops out. If
the world is a riot of color then a visual search for a particular hue
becomes difficult.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES
 Color solids.
 The three-dimensional nature of color space is a straight-forward
 consequence of having three cone types in the retina.
   But if we wish to view this three-dimensional space, how exactly
   should the colors be laid out?
   Here are just two of the many possible solutions.




This is a set of calibrated color             This is the outside of the cube of colors
samples. Each is designed to be               that can be represented on a computer
equally different from the adjacent           monitor by combinations of the outputs
sample, making it an example of a             of the red, green, and blue guns. The
uniform color space. (From Science            space is not perceptually uniform.
Museum/Science and Society Picture Library,
London.)
CHANNEL PROPERTIES




Color appearance.
Color appearance has more than three dimensions. The reason is
spatial. A patch of color is never seen in isolation, and its appearance is
affected by the colors that surround it, its orientation with respect to the
light source, whether is it perceived as lying in or out of shadow, the
texture of the surface on which it lies, and so on.
Colors like brown and olive green only occur through contrast with
surrounding colors. A color patch that seems brown when viewed on a
white background will be perceived as orange when viewed in isolation
in a dark room.
CHANNEL PROPERTIES
CHANNEL PROPERTIES
PRINCIPLES FOR DESIGN
SHOWING DETAIL




 luminance contrast is necessary. Of course, black and white
 give the most extreme contrast possible.

 Especially critical for small text.

 When design elements are very large, for example in a
 poster title, luminance contrast is less critical and design can
 be given free rein.
PRINCIPLES FOR DESIGN
SHOWING DETAIL

The red-green and yellow-blue channels do not convey fine
detail, but they are just as effective as black and white for
making large patterns distinct.
COLOR-CODING INFORMATION




Perhaps the most important use of color is to indicate
categories of information.
COLOR-CODING INFORMATION




The two primary considerations in the design of color codes
• visual distinctness, to support visual search operations
• learnability, so that particular colors come to “stand for” particular
   entities.
When learnability is the issue, it is important to use the unique hues
first—red, green, yellow, and blue—followed by other colors that have
relatively consistent names: pink, brown, orange, grey, and purple.
COLOR-CODING INFORMATION

If a design is complex and the symbols are quite small, then no more
than a dozen codes can be used with complete reliability.
The main reason why we cannot use more colors
• the backgrounds can distort the appearance of a small patch of
    symbol color leading to the confusion of one symbol with another.

To effectively support visual search the colors of the background and
the other symbols in the set are as important as the symbol being
searched.

A strong pop-out effect depends on
• the other colored objects in the scene
• the background color.
LARGE AND SMALL AREAS

 More complex designs often include color-coded background
 regions as well as color-coded symbols.
 Small areas should be
 • strongly colored
 • have black-white channel contrast with larger background
   areas if they are to be distinct.
LARGE AND SMALL AREAS
LARGE AND SMALL AREAS

When small symbols must be displayed against a colored background it
can be difficult to come up with symbol colors that are both distinct from
each other and from the background.

Using low-saturation colors for large areas generally makes the task
easier.

Also, color contrast will distort the perceived color of small symbols less
if the background colors are low in saturation.
EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING




A strong signal on any of the opponent channels will attract attention
better than a weak signal. Also, the strength of the signal depends on
the difference between a visual object and its background. Objects that
have reduced contrast with their backgrounds are naturally less salient.
EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING




GOOD. This example uses high               BAD. This example shows what
saturation (strong colors) to code small   happens when the colors used for
areas such as symbols and lines.           small areas and large areas are
Larger background areas                    switched. The color codes
are all light and low saturation.          used for the symbols and lines are
                                           difficult to discriminate because of
                                           contrast effects.
EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING
A common mistake, often seen in PowerPoint slides




The highlighted text shown above is less distinct than the other text
even though it uses a saturated hue.




It is often more effective to emphasize text by reducing the contrast of the
other text. Care must be taken to maintain sufficient luminance contrast for
clarity.
EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING




This illustration of a lumbar spinal fusion technique, by medical
illustrator Christy Krames, has highlighted the critical area of interest by
de-emphasizing the background through the use of neutral tones with
some low-saturation blues. The critical area of interest is rendered in
higher contrast and with the color red appearing only in this area.
(Image courtesy of Christy Krames.)
COLOR SEQUENCES




Color is most effective as a highlighter when there is only a single
color used, other than black and white. (Image courtesy of National
Park Service.)
COLOR SEQUENCES



There are two primary tasks supported by color sequences used with
maps.
• It allows people to perceive the spatial layout of patterns in the data,
  such as the shape and location of the hills, ridges, and valleys.
• we can read quantitative values from the map; for example, judging
  the height or energy level at a particular location.
COLOR SEQUENCES

Because shape perception is based mainly on luminance channel information,
color sequences that vary mainly in luminance will be the most effective in
revealing patterns in the data. Different colors in the sequence along with a
color key makes for more accurate readings.

The best solution may be to provide a sequence that spirals up through color
space, starting off with a dark color and selecting a series of colors, each of
which has higher luminance than the previous one.




                                     A black-white (top left) sequence reveals
                                     the features of the map much better than a
                                     red-green sequence (top right). Even better
                                     is a sequence of colors that consistently
                                     increases in lightness while cycling through
                                     a range of hues (bottom left). A stepped
                                     sequence allows for more precision,
                                     especially when reading from a key,
                                     although less detail is revealed (bottom
                                     right).
COLOR SEQUENCES

Zero is often an important number in data analysis, and sometimes a
color sequence is needed that represents zero in an intuitive way.




 This map shows temperature differences (from normal) for the
 Pacific Ocean at 100 meters depth during an El Nin~o period.
 Warmer than usual is shown as red. Cooler than usual is shown as
 blue. Zero difference is shown as white.
COLOR SEQUENCES




 One kind of color sequence that is often used by scientists is an
 approximation to the spectrum of light (purple-blue-cyan-green-
 yellow-red).
 Because this represents a sequence of wavelengths of light, it is
 often erroneously presumed to be a perceptual sequence.

 Parts of the spectrum are perceptually ordered, but the whole
 spectrum is not. Although the spectrum may be useful for
 reading values from data using a color key, it will not be the best
 for revealing patterns.
COLOR ON SHADED SURFACES
What does it take to see both the shape of a shaded surface and the
colored patterns that are painted on that surface?
Shape from shading information is conveyed by the luminance channel,
and any colored pattern that interferes with luminance channel
information destroys our ability to see the shape of the surface.


                                         Adding the light-colored
                                         pattern (top center) to a
                                         shaded surface (top left)
                                         interferes little with our
                                         perception of surface shape
                                         as the combined image
                                         (lower left) shows.

                                         Adding a dark-colored
                                         pattern with strongly
                                         saturated colors (top right) to
                                         the same shaded surface
SEMANTIC OF COLOR


Colors are often used symbolically. Some of these meanings are
well entrenched. Changing stop signs to the color green would have
disastrous consequences.


Other semantic mappings relating to quantity may be more universal.
Quantities that are larger should generally be mapped to colors that
are more distinct. This visually means that more saturated colors
should be used to represent greater quantity. Similarly, darker colors
on a light background or lighter colors on a dark background can be
used to indicate greater quantity.
SEMANTIC OF COLOR




Six different ways of showing greater quantity. All involve increasing
the relative difference from the background color.



The most distinctive hues should be reserved for the most commonly
accessed symbols and patterns in a design.
CONCLUSION

Based on color-opponent theory, the functional aesthetics of color
can be reduced to a remarkably small number of guidelines.
Colors are changed in appearance by adjacent colors. This is
called simultaneous lightness or chromatic contrast.

contrast effects
• colors will be confused.
• the appearance of a specific hue can only be judged in the context
  of a particular set of surrounding colors.


It is generally better if large areas have subdued colors. Also, to make
small areas distinct they should be strongly colored to counter the
lack of spatial resolution in the red-green and yellow-blue channels.
CONCLUSION


Red, green, yellow, blue, black, and white are special. They each
produce the most extreme signal on one channel while being neutral
with respect to the other channels. These are the most distinct and
easily identified colors and form the ideal labels, all other
considerations being equal.

Only the black-white channel is
• capable of conveying much detail and so dark-light contrast is
  essential if detail is to be shown.
• essential to show the shape of curved surfaces through shading.

Smooth graduations of darkness and lightness will be perceived largely
as shading differences.
CONCLUSION




Unlike gray-scale gradients, smooth graduations of color tend to be
perceptually categorized into regions of red, yellow, green, and
blue. If it is necessary to present a hue so that it can be properly
judged it is useful to isolate it with a neutral black, white, or gray
border. Even better is to see it in context.

Contenu connexe

Similaire à W8 color

Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...
Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...
Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...Elsa von Licy
 
Color vision โอม
Color vision โอมColor vision โอม
Color vision โอมPanit Cherdchu
 
Colour schemes lesson 1
Colour schemes lesson 1Colour schemes lesson 1
Colour schemes lesson 1michael mcewan
 
Color vision and physiological processes
Color vision and physiological processesColor vision and physiological processes
Color vision and physiological processesRaju Kaiti
 
IMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of survey
IMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of surveyIMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of survey
IMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of surveyKaran Patel
 
color and optical effects in dentistry
color and optical effects in dentistrycolor and optical effects in dentistry
color and optical effects in dentistryMohamed M. Abdul-Monem
 
Color and human vision
Color and human visionColor and human vision
Color and human visionsaniacorreya
 
colour vision.pptx
colour vision.pptxcolour vision.pptx
colour vision.pptxMoniCaNeGi2
 
Color Study Journal
Color Study JournalColor Study Journal
Color Study JournalJOYCE TEOH
 
Color theorykareemebrahim
Color theorykareemebrahimColor theorykareemebrahim
Color theorykareemebrahimKareem .
 
Shade selection seminar - Copy.pptx
Shade selection seminar - Copy.pptxShade selection seminar - Copy.pptx
Shade selection seminar - Copy.pptxSAROJINIBISWAL
 
Design Element 4 - Color
Design Element 4 - ColorDesign Element 4 - Color
Design Element 4 - Colormjb77ny
 
Bruce Block's Visual Components For Filmmakers
Bruce Block's Visual Components For FilmmakersBruce Block's Visual Components For Filmmakers
Bruce Block's Visual Components For Filmmakersthomasjcastillo
 
Colour vision with lvm
Colour vision with lvmColour vision with lvm
Colour vision with lvmsurendra74
 

Similaire à W8 color (20)

Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...
Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...
Visual thinking colin_ware_lectures_2013_5_color theory and color for informa...
 
color vision.ppt
color vision.pptcolor vision.ppt
color vision.ppt
 
Color vision โอม
Color vision โอมColor vision โอม
Color vision โอม
 
Colour 1 theory 2014
Colour 1 theory 2014Colour 1 theory 2014
Colour 1 theory 2014
 
Light and Dark Adaptation
Light and Dark AdaptationLight and Dark Adaptation
Light and Dark Adaptation
 
Colour schemes lesson 1
Colour schemes lesson 1Colour schemes lesson 1
Colour schemes lesson 1
 
Colour vision
Colour visionColour vision
Colour vision
 
Color vision and physiological processes
Color vision and physiological processesColor vision and physiological processes
Color vision and physiological processes
 
IMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of survey
IMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of surveyIMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of survey
IMAGE INTERPRETATION TECHNIQUES of survey
 
Color vision
Color visionColor vision
Color vision
 
color and optical effects in dentistry
color and optical effects in dentistrycolor and optical effects in dentistry
color and optical effects in dentistry
 
Color and human vision
Color and human visionColor and human vision
Color and human vision
 
colour vision.pptx
colour vision.pptxcolour vision.pptx
colour vision.pptx
 
Color Study Journal
Color Study JournalColor Study Journal
Color Study Journal
 
Color theorykareemebrahim
Color theorykareemebrahimColor theorykareemebrahim
Color theorykareemebrahim
 
Shade selection seminar - Copy.pptx
Shade selection seminar - Copy.pptxShade selection seminar - Copy.pptx
Shade selection seminar - Copy.pptx
 
Design Element 4 - Color
Design Element 4 - ColorDesign Element 4 - Color
Design Element 4 - Color
 
shade selection seminar.pptx
shade selection seminar.pptxshade selection seminar.pptx
shade selection seminar.pptx
 
Bruce Block's Visual Components For Filmmakers
Bruce Block's Visual Components For FilmmakersBruce Block's Visual Components For Filmmakers
Bruce Block's Visual Components For Filmmakers
 
Colour vision with lvm
Colour vision with lvmColour vision with lvm
Colour vision with lvm
 

Plus de visualization, ID, NCKU

G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭
G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭
G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥
G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥
G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼
G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼
G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪
G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪
G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_
G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_
G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳
G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳
G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡visualization, ID, NCKU
 
G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡visualization, ID, NCKU
 

Plus de visualization, ID, NCKU (20)

G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭
G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭
G4 c2 何元杰陳禹安陳虹誼鍾承庭
 
G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 c2 許哲豪_孫佳筠_吳典陽_黃玟慧
 
G2 c2 許瑋芸_張耀仁
G2 c2 許瑋芸_張耀仁G2 c2 許瑋芸_張耀仁
G2 c2 許瑋芸_張耀仁
 
G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥
G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥
G1 c2 王柔堯_吳思儀_張緹葳_丁星顥
 
G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 c2 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
 
G6 c1 雅筑_怡嘉_昱仁
G6 c1 雅筑_怡嘉_昱仁G6 c1 雅筑_怡嘉_昱仁
G6 c1 雅筑_怡嘉_昱仁
 
Pasta
PastaPasta
Pasta
 
G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼
G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼
G4 c1 何元杰陳禹安鍾承庭陳虹誼
 
G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪
G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪
G3 c1-吳典陽 黃玟慧-孫佳筠_許哲豪
 
G2 c1 張耀仁_許瑋芸
G2 c1 張耀仁_許瑋芸G2 c1 張耀仁_許瑋芸
G2 c1 張耀仁_許瑋芸
 
G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_
G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_
G5 c1 佳穗、若瑄、凱駿、怡凡_
 
1122 雅筑怡嘉昱仁
1122 雅筑怡嘉昱仁 1122 雅筑怡嘉昱仁
1122 雅筑怡嘉昱仁
 
1206 雅筑怡嘉昱仁
1206 雅筑怡嘉昱仁1206 雅筑怡嘉昱仁
1206 雅筑怡嘉昱仁
 
G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧
G3 b4-許哲豪 孫佳筠-吳典陽_黃玟慧
 
G2 b4 許瑋芸張耀仁
G2 b4 許瑋芸張耀仁G2 b4 許瑋芸張耀仁
G2 b4 許瑋芸張耀仁
 
G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳
G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳
G1 b4 丁星顥王柔堯吳思儀張緹葳
 
G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b4-徐佳穗 劉若瑄-鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
 
G2 b3 許瑋芸、張耀仁
G2 b3 許瑋芸、張耀仁G2 b3 許瑋芸、張耀仁
G2 b3 許瑋芸、張耀仁
 
G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
G5 b3 徐佳穗_劉若瑄_鄭凱駿_鄧怡凡
 
G4 b3 庭安杰虹
G4 b3 庭安杰虹G4 b3 庭安杰虹
G4 b3 庭安杰虹
 

W8 color

  • 1. The purpose of this chapter • To develop a theory-based approach to how color should be used in design.
  • 2. THE COLOR-PROCESSING MACHINERY There two basic types of light receptors in the retina at the back of the eyeball: • Rods • Cones Rods: • The most numerous type • Specialized for very low light levels. Cones: • The basis for normal daytime vision • Three subtypes—short-wavelength sensitive, middle-wavelength sensitive, and long-wavelength sensitive.
  • 3. Cones in the fovea where there are no rods. Those sensitive to short, medium, and long wavelengths are colored blue, green, and red, respectively (Images from David R. Williams, University of Rochester)
  • 4. Cone receptors are sensitive to light having wavelengths between 400 and 700 nanometers. Note: L and M overlap. The brain takes the difference between the L and M to get useful hue information. It is impossible to reproduce pure spectral colors with printing inks so the color bands is only approximate.
  • 5. A consequence of our having few and weak blue-sensitive cones is that it is a mistake to show text or anything else with detail using blue on a dark background. The result is illegible. A related problem—Yellow Yellow can be almost as light as white, and small yellow text on a white background is extremely hard to read, but pure yellow is very distinct on a black background.
  • 6. OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY Neural networks add and subtract the cone signals in different ways, transforming them into what are called the color-opponent channels. There are three channels: • red-green (diff. bet M- and L-wavelength) • yellow-blue (diff. bet luminance and blue) • black-white (or luminance, combine M and L)
  • 7. OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY Red-green channel • represents the difference between the signal from the middle-and long-wavelength-sensitive cones. Yellow-blue channel • represents the differences between the luminance channel and the blue cone signals. Black-white channels • Combines two type of cone information • Differentiates in a spatial sense.
  • 8. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Contrast. Simultaneous contrast occurs in each of the channels. Lightness/Brightness contrast—occurs in the black-white channel. Chromatic contrast—occurs in either the red-green or yellow-blue channels. Its effects are much more complex and harder to predict than grey-scale contrast.
  • 9. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Unique hues. Black, white, red, green, yellow, and blue • occurs when there is a strong signal on one of the channels and a neutral signal on the other two channels. • the most commonly used no matter what the language.
  • 10. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Sensitivity to spatial detail. The luminance channel has much greater capacity to convey detailed information than the chromatic channel. The fine pattern is much harder to see expressed through chromatic differences than when expressed through black-white differences. Motion. The luminance channel can convey motion information much more effectively than the chromatic channels.
  • 11. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Stereoscopic depth. This is the kind of depth information we get from having two eyes. The brain’s processing of stereoscopic depth information is done via the luminance channel. Shape-from-shading. The luminance channel can process shape-from-shading, whereas the chromatic channels cannot. In the illustration right, a black and white photograph of a shaded surface has been doctored so that the black-white sequence has been transformed into a red-green sequence. In this way, the topography of the shape has become virtually meaningless.
  • 12. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Saturation. Saturated colors • Vivid • Have strong signals on one or both chromatic channels.
  • 13. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Maximum saturation varies with luminance. When colors are dark • there is a smaller difference signal on the chromatic channels • saturations are lower for dark colors. When colors are light • there is also usually a reduction in saturation for reasons having to do with color reproduction technology rather than perception.
  • 14. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Luminance, lightness, brightness. Luminance • physical amount of light. Lightness • used when discussing surface colors Brightness • generally used when discussing emitted light. Lightness and brightness are both • the amount of light coming from a particular part of the visual field • signals that affect the luminance channel.
  • 15. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Luminance nonlinearity. The luminance channel is nonlinear. We are more sensitive to dark gray differences than to light differences. With a dark gray background, our sensitivity is increased to gray differences in the midrange. With a light background, our sensitivity is increased to differences near white. This effect is sometimes called sharpening.
  • 16. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Color segmentation. The visual system has a strong tendency to segment smoothly changing colors into regions consisting of the unique hues.
  • 17. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Color blindness. People who are color blind (about 8 percent of males) • missing the red-green channel • they have a two-dimensional rather than a three-dimensional color space. • they can still distinguish colors that differ in the yellow-blue direction • their ability to see gray-scale is unaffected. Yellow-blue color blindness is much less common.
  • 18. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Chromatic color as a property of surfaces. Major function of visual processing • let us perceive the properties of surfaces in the environment, rather than the amount of light coming from those surfaces. Adaptation in the receptors means that we are relatively insensitive to overall changes in light levels. Contrast mechanisms make us more sensitive to local changes in surface colors, rather than differences in light and shade across a scene.
  • 19. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Color and attention. The pattern-finding mechanisms of the brain can be tuned for color. This allows for rapid visual searches for things that are of particular colors. But color search also depends how many other colors there are in the environment. If all the world is gray, a patch of vivid color pops out. If the world is a riot of color then a visual search for a particular hue becomes difficult.
  • 20. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Color solids. The three-dimensional nature of color space is a straight-forward consequence of having three cone types in the retina. But if we wish to view this three-dimensional space, how exactly should the colors be laid out? Here are just two of the many possible solutions. This is a set of calibrated color This is the outside of the cube of colors samples. Each is designed to be that can be represented on a computer equally different from the adjacent monitor by combinations of the outputs sample, making it an example of a of the red, green, and blue guns. The uniform color space. (From Science space is not perceptually uniform. Museum/Science and Society Picture Library, London.)
  • 21. CHANNEL PROPERTIES Color appearance. Color appearance has more than three dimensions. The reason is spatial. A patch of color is never seen in isolation, and its appearance is affected by the colors that surround it, its orientation with respect to the light source, whether is it perceived as lying in or out of shadow, the texture of the surface on which it lies, and so on. Colors like brown and olive green only occur through contrast with surrounding colors. A color patch that seems brown when viewed on a white background will be perceived as orange when viewed in isolation in a dark room.
  • 24. PRINCIPLES FOR DESIGN SHOWING DETAIL luminance contrast is necessary. Of course, black and white give the most extreme contrast possible. Especially critical for small text. When design elements are very large, for example in a poster title, luminance contrast is less critical and design can be given free rein.
  • 25. PRINCIPLES FOR DESIGN SHOWING DETAIL The red-green and yellow-blue channels do not convey fine detail, but they are just as effective as black and white for making large patterns distinct.
  • 26. COLOR-CODING INFORMATION Perhaps the most important use of color is to indicate categories of information.
  • 27. COLOR-CODING INFORMATION The two primary considerations in the design of color codes • visual distinctness, to support visual search operations • learnability, so that particular colors come to “stand for” particular entities. When learnability is the issue, it is important to use the unique hues first—red, green, yellow, and blue—followed by other colors that have relatively consistent names: pink, brown, orange, grey, and purple.
  • 28. COLOR-CODING INFORMATION If a design is complex and the symbols are quite small, then no more than a dozen codes can be used with complete reliability. The main reason why we cannot use more colors • the backgrounds can distort the appearance of a small patch of symbol color leading to the confusion of one symbol with another. To effectively support visual search the colors of the background and the other symbols in the set are as important as the symbol being searched. A strong pop-out effect depends on • the other colored objects in the scene • the background color.
  • 29. LARGE AND SMALL AREAS More complex designs often include color-coded background regions as well as color-coded symbols. Small areas should be • strongly colored • have black-white channel contrast with larger background areas if they are to be distinct.
  • 31. LARGE AND SMALL AREAS When small symbols must be displayed against a colored background it can be difficult to come up with symbol colors that are both distinct from each other and from the background. Using low-saturation colors for large areas generally makes the task easier. Also, color contrast will distort the perceived color of small symbols less if the background colors are low in saturation.
  • 32. EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING A strong signal on any of the opponent channels will attract attention better than a weak signal. Also, the strength of the signal depends on the difference between a visual object and its background. Objects that have reduced contrast with their backgrounds are naturally less salient.
  • 33. EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING GOOD. This example uses high BAD. This example shows what saturation (strong colors) to code small happens when the colors used for areas such as symbols and lines. small areas and large areas are Larger background areas switched. The color codes are all light and low saturation. used for the symbols and lines are difficult to discriminate because of contrast effects.
  • 34. EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING A common mistake, often seen in PowerPoint slides The highlighted text shown above is less distinct than the other text even though it uses a saturated hue. It is often more effective to emphasize text by reducing the contrast of the other text. Care must be taken to maintain sufficient luminance contrast for clarity.
  • 35. EMPTHASIS AND HIGHLIGHTING This illustration of a lumbar spinal fusion technique, by medical illustrator Christy Krames, has highlighted the critical area of interest by de-emphasizing the background through the use of neutral tones with some low-saturation blues. The critical area of interest is rendered in higher contrast and with the color red appearing only in this area. (Image courtesy of Christy Krames.)
  • 36. COLOR SEQUENCES Color is most effective as a highlighter when there is only a single color used, other than black and white. (Image courtesy of National Park Service.)
  • 37. COLOR SEQUENCES There are two primary tasks supported by color sequences used with maps. • It allows people to perceive the spatial layout of patterns in the data, such as the shape and location of the hills, ridges, and valleys. • we can read quantitative values from the map; for example, judging the height or energy level at a particular location.
  • 38. COLOR SEQUENCES Because shape perception is based mainly on luminance channel information, color sequences that vary mainly in luminance will be the most effective in revealing patterns in the data. Different colors in the sequence along with a color key makes for more accurate readings. The best solution may be to provide a sequence that spirals up through color space, starting off with a dark color and selecting a series of colors, each of which has higher luminance than the previous one. A black-white (top left) sequence reveals the features of the map much better than a red-green sequence (top right). Even better is a sequence of colors that consistently increases in lightness while cycling through a range of hues (bottom left). A stepped sequence allows for more precision, especially when reading from a key, although less detail is revealed (bottom right).
  • 39. COLOR SEQUENCES Zero is often an important number in data analysis, and sometimes a color sequence is needed that represents zero in an intuitive way. This map shows temperature differences (from normal) for the Pacific Ocean at 100 meters depth during an El Nin~o period. Warmer than usual is shown as red. Cooler than usual is shown as blue. Zero difference is shown as white.
  • 40. COLOR SEQUENCES One kind of color sequence that is often used by scientists is an approximation to the spectrum of light (purple-blue-cyan-green- yellow-red). Because this represents a sequence of wavelengths of light, it is often erroneously presumed to be a perceptual sequence. Parts of the spectrum are perceptually ordered, but the whole spectrum is not. Although the spectrum may be useful for reading values from data using a color key, it will not be the best for revealing patterns.
  • 41. COLOR ON SHADED SURFACES What does it take to see both the shape of a shaded surface and the colored patterns that are painted on that surface? Shape from shading information is conveyed by the luminance channel, and any colored pattern that interferes with luminance channel information destroys our ability to see the shape of the surface. Adding the light-colored pattern (top center) to a shaded surface (top left) interferes little with our perception of surface shape as the combined image (lower left) shows. Adding a dark-colored pattern with strongly saturated colors (top right) to the same shaded surface
  • 42. SEMANTIC OF COLOR Colors are often used symbolically. Some of these meanings are well entrenched. Changing stop signs to the color green would have disastrous consequences. Other semantic mappings relating to quantity may be more universal. Quantities that are larger should generally be mapped to colors that are more distinct. This visually means that more saturated colors should be used to represent greater quantity. Similarly, darker colors on a light background or lighter colors on a dark background can be used to indicate greater quantity.
  • 43. SEMANTIC OF COLOR Six different ways of showing greater quantity. All involve increasing the relative difference from the background color. The most distinctive hues should be reserved for the most commonly accessed symbols and patterns in a design.
  • 44. CONCLUSION Based on color-opponent theory, the functional aesthetics of color can be reduced to a remarkably small number of guidelines. Colors are changed in appearance by adjacent colors. This is called simultaneous lightness or chromatic contrast. contrast effects • colors will be confused. • the appearance of a specific hue can only be judged in the context of a particular set of surrounding colors. It is generally better if large areas have subdued colors. Also, to make small areas distinct they should be strongly colored to counter the lack of spatial resolution in the red-green and yellow-blue channels.
  • 45. CONCLUSION Red, green, yellow, blue, black, and white are special. They each produce the most extreme signal on one channel while being neutral with respect to the other channels. These are the most distinct and easily identified colors and form the ideal labels, all other considerations being equal. Only the black-white channel is • capable of conveying much detail and so dark-light contrast is essential if detail is to be shown. • essential to show the shape of curved surfaces through shading. Smooth graduations of darkness and lightness will be perceived largely as shading differences.
  • 46. CONCLUSION Unlike gray-scale gradients, smooth graduations of color tend to be perceptually categorized into regions of red, yellow, green, and blue. If it is necessary to present a hue so that it can be properly judged it is useful to isolate it with a neutral black, white, or gray border. Even better is to see it in context.