SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  138
Télécharger pour lire hors ligne
Reliable individual
functional networks
and their relationship
to behavior 

Emily S. Finn, PhD

Section on Functional Imaging Methods

Laboratory of Brain & Cognition, NIMH

emily.finn@nih.gov

Taking Connectivity to a Skeptical Future
Educational Workshop | OHBM Annual Meeting

June 25, 2017

@esfinn
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
How to get good
behavioral data
3
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Why study individual differences?
Controls
Patients
Why study individual differences?
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
Why study individual differences?
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
Why study individual differences?
Categorical approach
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
Why study individual differences?
Categorical approach
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
Why study individual differences?
Categorical approach
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
Why study individual differences?
Categorical approach
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
p < 0.05*
Why study individual differences?
Categorical approach Dimensional approach
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
p < 0.05*
Why study individual differences?
Categorical approach Dimensional approach
Controls
Patients
• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
• Eventual clinical applications:
p < 0.05*
Why study individual differences?
Motor
Mt
Emotion
Em
Language
Lg
Working memory
WM
Resting
R1
+
Resting
R2
+
Why study individual differences?
Motor
Mt
Emotion
Em
Language
Lg
Working memory
WM
Resting
R1
Resting
R2
Why study individual differences?
Motor
Mt
Emotion
Em
Language
Lg
Working memory
WM
Resting
R1
Resting
R2
Why study individual differences?
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
Why study individual differences?
0.93 0.84 0.63
0.72 0.79 0.67
0.64 0.60 0.54
R1
WM
Mt
R2 Lg Em
Database
Target
ID rate
0.5 1.0
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
Why study individual differences?
You always look most like yourself,
regardless of what you’re doing
0.93 0.84 0.63
0.72 0.79 0.67
0.64 0.60 0.54
R1
WM
Mt
R2 Lg Em
Database
Target
ID rate
0.5 1.0
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
Why study individual differences?
You always look most like yourself,
regardless of what you’re doing
0.93 0.84 0.63
0.72 0.79 0.67
0.64 0.60 0.54
R1
WM
Mt
R2 Lg Em
Database
Target
ID rate
0.5 1.0
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
Why study individual differences?
You always look most like yourself,
regardless of what you’re doing
0.93 0.84 0.63
0.72 0.79 0.67
0.64 0.60 0.54
R1
WM
Mt
R2 Lg Em
Database
Target
ID rate
0.5 1.0
Individual differences in FC predict
individual differences in behavior
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
• More nodes —> higher identification rate
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
• More nodes —> higher identification rate
‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
• More nodes —> higher identification rate
‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter
‣ Caution: Higher resolution may amplify effects of anatomical diffs/registration error
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
A. Not really
• More nodes —> higher identification rate
‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter
‣ Caution: Higher resolution may amplify effects of anatomical diffs/registration error
‣ Parcellations in the 200-300 node range seem like a good compromise
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
Q. What about amount of data?
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Longer acquisitions are better:
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Longer acquisitions are better:
• higher reliability within subjects
Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
• higher identifiability across subjects
Longer acquisitions are better:
• higher reliability within subjects
Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
• higher identifiability across subjects
‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot
make up for shorter scan duration
Longer acquisitions are better:
• higher reliability within subjects
Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
• higher identifiability across subjects
‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot
make up for shorter scan duration
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
Longer acquisitions are better:
• higher reliability within subjects
Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
Q. What about amount of data?
A. Scan duration matters!
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
• higher identifiability across subjects
‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot
make up for shorter scan duration
Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)
Shah et al., Brain & Behav (2016)
Longer acquisitions are better:
• higher reliability within subjects
Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
Q. Does scan condition matter?
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
0.93 0.84 0.63
0.72 0.79 0.67
0.64 0.60 0.54
R1
WM
Mt
R2 Lg Em
Database
Target
ID rate
0.5 1.0
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Does scan condition matter?
A. Yes!
Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences,
but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
‣ for a set scan duration, tasks may be more reliable than rest
‣ tasks may converge faster on a subject’s “true” profile
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions
Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions
Target
Database
Day 1 Day 2
IDrate
Day1Day2
Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions
Target
Database
Day 1 Day 2
IDrate
Day1Day2
‣ consider a combination of rest and task
Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions
Target
Database
Day 1 Day 2
IDrate
Day1Day2
‣ consider a combination of rest and task
Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
Inscapes: Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage 2015

headspacestudios.org
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
Session 1
Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11
Session 2
‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
Session 1
Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11
Session 2
‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest
Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Consider naturalistic tasks:
Session 1
Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11
Session 2
‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest
Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations
Huijbers et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations
ChildrenAdults
Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2015)Huijbers et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Q. Is rest best?
A. Probably not
Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to choose behavior
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
Does it show a good distribution
in your population?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
Does it show a good distribution
in your population?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
Does it show a good distribution
in your population?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
Does it show a good distribution
in your population?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
Does it show a good distribution
in your population?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
How to choose behavior
Is it stable?
Does it show a good distribution
in your population?
Is it something you expect to be
reflected in brain function?
• Trait vs. state
• State variables may be better
suited to within-subject analysis
Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
Siegel et al., Cerebral Cortex (2016)
Negatively: Positively:
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
Siegel et al., Cerebral Cortex (2016)
Negatively: Positively:
Geerligs et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2017)
Age, vascular health:
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
‣ Check for correlation in your sample
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
‣ Check for correlation in your sample
‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
‣ Check for correlation in your sample
‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects
‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
‣ Check for correlation in your sample
‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects
‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques
Ciric et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Behavior: Mitigating confounds
Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
‣ Check for correlation in your sample
‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects
‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques
‣ Use motion as an explicit covariate
Ciric et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Outline
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5How to get good
behavioral data
3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
‣ even better: cross-dataset
Sustained attention model
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016)
Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
‣ even better: cross-dataset
Sustained attention model
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016)
Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)
Observed behav
Predictedbehav
n = 25 adults
Brain behavior: Cross-validate!
Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
‣ leave-one-subject-out
(within dataset)
‣ even better: cross-dataset
Sustained attention model
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling
Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
Predictedbehav
Observed behav
2
7.5
4
6.3
Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016)
Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)
Observed behav
Predictedbehav
n = 25 adults
Observed ADHD score
n = 113 children
PredictedADHDscore
Q. What is the best brain state?
Q. What is the best brain state?
A. Maybe it depends on your behavior
Q. What is the best brain state?
A. Maybe it depends on your behavior
Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:
Q. What is the best brain state?
A. Maybe it depends on your behavior
n = 716, 10-fold cross-validation
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling (CPM; Shen et al., Nat Protocols 2017)
Modelinputdata
Target behavior
Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:
Q. What is the best brain state?
A. Maybe it depends on your behavior
n = 716, 10-fold cross-validation
Connectome-based Predictive Modeling (CPM; Shen et al., Nat Protocols 2017)
Modelinputdata
Target behavior
Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:
Consider tailoring scan condition
to behavior of interest
➡“Stress test”?
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
‣ increase sensitivity
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
‣ increase sensitivity
• Choose an interesting
behavior with a good
distribution
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
‣ increase sensitivity
• Choose an interesting
behavior with a good
distribution
• Consider potential
confounds and take
steps to mitigate them
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
‣ increase sensitivity
• Choose an interesting
behavior with a good
distribution
• Consider potential
confounds and take
steps to mitigate them
• Cross-validate whenever
possible
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
‣ increase sensitivity
• Choose an interesting
behavior with a good
distribution
• Consider potential
confounds and take
steps to mitigate them
• Cross-validate whenever
possible
• Consider tailoring your
scan condition to
behavior of interest
Take-home points
Introduction:
Why study
individual
differences?
1 How to get good
brain data
2 How to get good
behavioral data
3
How to relate
brain to behavior
4
Learn more5
• Data quality ≠ critical
• Longer scans are better!
• Consider using tasks
‣ improve compliance
‣ increase sensitivity
• Choose an interesting
behavior with a good
distribution
• Consider potential
confounds and take
steps to mitigate them
• Cross-validate whenever
possible
• Consider tailoring your
scan condition to
behavior of interest
Open data sets with brain & behavior
➡ Use these on their own or in combination with your own data
to generate or test hypotheses, see if a finding generalizes, etc
Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental
Cohort
Further reading
Building a science of individual differences from fMRI
Dubois & Adolphs, Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2016)
From regions to connections and networks: new bridges
between brain and behavior
Misic & Sporns, Current Opinion in Neurobiology (2016)
Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual
differences in functional connectivity?
Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
Prediction as a humanitarian and pragmatic contribution from
human cognitive neuroscience
Gabrieli, Ghosh & Gabrieli, Neuron (2015)
Selected reviews:
Learn more at OHBM 2017
Symposium:
Collect your thoughts: Individual differences in the networks underlying intelligence
Tues 8:00-9:15am
Symposium:
Relating connectivity to inter- and intra-individual differences in attention and cognition
Weds 8:00-9:15am
Poster #4042: Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual differences in
functional connectivity? Finn et al.
Poster #4040: Large-scale functional connectivity networks predict attention
fluctuations Rosenberg et al.
Poster #2110: Functional connectivity-based predictors of naturalistic reading
comprehension Jangraw et al.
Poster #4029: FMRI connectivity is differentially associated with performance
across tasks in a multi-task study Topolski et al.
@esfinn

Contenu connexe

Similaire à Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Search the literature: the beginning
Search the literature: the beginningSearch the literature: the beginning
Search the literature: the beginning
Mary Shah
 
Study notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docx
Study notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docxStudy notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docx
Study notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docx
hanneloremccaffery
 
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slidesQualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Alexandra Howson MA, PhD, CHCP
 

Similaire à Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop (20)

Redevelop 2019 - Debugging our biases and intuition in software development
Redevelop 2019 - Debugging our biases and intuition in software developmentRedevelop 2019 - Debugging our biases and intuition in software development
Redevelop 2019 - Debugging our biases and intuition in software development
 
Psyc 321_02 methods of_science
Psyc 321_02 methods of_sciencePsyc 321_02 methods of_science
Psyc 321_02 methods of_science
 
FINDING THE EVIDENCE (1).pdf
FINDING THE EVIDENCE (1).pdfFINDING THE EVIDENCE (1).pdf
FINDING THE EVIDENCE (1).pdf
 
Data collection methods to improve reproducibility
Data collection methods to improve reproducibilityData collection methods to improve reproducibility
Data collection methods to improve reproducibility
 
(2017/06)Practical points of deep learning for medical imaging
(2017/06)Practical points of deep learning for medical imaging(2017/06)Practical points of deep learning for medical imaging
(2017/06)Practical points of deep learning for medical imaging
 
RecSys 2016 Talk: Feature Selection For Human Recommenders
RecSys 2016 Talk: Feature Selection For Human RecommendersRecSys 2016 Talk: Feature Selection For Human Recommenders
RecSys 2016 Talk: Feature Selection For Human Recommenders
 
EBD Course - Lecture 3 Finding the Answer for
EBD Course - Lecture 3 Finding the Answer for EBD Course - Lecture 3 Finding the Answer for
EBD Course - Lecture 3 Finding the Answer for
 
Critical analysis what is it dw
Critical analysis what is it dwCritical analysis what is it dw
Critical analysis what is it dw
 
Search the literature: the beginning
Search the literature: the beginningSearch the literature: the beginning
Search the literature: the beginning
 
EBD Course: Lecture 4 Secondary Sources
EBD Course: Lecture 4 Secondary SourcesEBD Course: Lecture 4 Secondary Sources
EBD Course: Lecture 4 Secondary Sources
 
Ch02 Psychological Research
Ch02 Psychological ResearchCh02 Psychological Research
Ch02 Psychological Research
 
REAP
REAPREAP
REAP
 
Identification1
Identification1Identification1
Identification1
 
Psychometric assessment of older adults oct 21 to 26 2013 winter workshop
Psychometric assessment of older adults oct 21 to 26 2013 winter workshopPsychometric assessment of older adults oct 21 to 26 2013 winter workshop
Psychometric assessment of older adults oct 21 to 26 2013 winter workshop
 
The Reproducibility Crisis in Psychological Science: One Year Later
The Reproducibility Crisis in Psychological Science: One Year LaterThe Reproducibility Crisis in Psychological Science: One Year Later
The Reproducibility Crisis in Psychological Science: One Year Later
 
Study notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docx
Study notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docxStudy notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docx
Study notesSome of the information below may be repetitive of wh.docx
 
17 Sep 25 NIPS Attention & Consciousness
17 Sep 25 NIPS Attention & Consciousness17 Sep 25 NIPS Attention & Consciousness
17 Sep 25 NIPS Attention & Consciousness
 
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slidesQualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
 
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slidesQualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
Qualitative analysis boot camp final presentation slides
 
Does preregistration improve the interpretability and credibility of research...
Does preregistration improve the interpretability and credibility of research...Does preregistration improve the interpretability and credibility of research...
Does preregistration improve the interpretability and credibility of research...
 

Dernier

Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.
Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.
Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.
Silpa
 
The Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptx
The Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptxThe Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptx
The Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptx
seri bangash
 
Module for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learning
Module for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learningModule for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learning
Module for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learning
levieagacer
 
Biogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune Waterworlds
Biogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune WaterworldsBiogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune Waterworlds
Biogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune Waterworlds
Sérgio Sacani
 
Human genetics..........................pptx
Human genetics..........................pptxHuman genetics..........................pptx
Human genetics..........................pptx
Silpa
 
Asymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 b
Asymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 bAsymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 b
Asymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 b
Sérgio Sacani
 
development of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virus
development of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virusdevelopment of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virus
development of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virus
NazaninKarimi6
 

Dernier (20)

Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.
Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.
Reboulia: features, anatomy, morphology etc.
 
Zoology 5th semester notes( Sumit_yadav).pdf
Zoology 5th semester notes( Sumit_yadav).pdfZoology 5th semester notes( Sumit_yadav).pdf
Zoology 5th semester notes( Sumit_yadav).pdf
 
The Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptx
The Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptxThe Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptx
The Mariana Trench remarkable geological features on Earth.pptx
 
Module for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learning
Module for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learningModule for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learning
Module for Grade 9 for Asynchronous/Distance learning
 
Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx .
Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx       .Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx       .
Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx .
 
Role of AI in seed science Predictive modelling and Beyond.pptx
Role of AI in seed science  Predictive modelling and  Beyond.pptxRole of AI in seed science  Predictive modelling and  Beyond.pptx
Role of AI in seed science Predictive modelling and Beyond.pptx
 
Biogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune Waterworlds
Biogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune WaterworldsBiogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune Waterworlds
Biogenic Sulfur Gases as Biosignatures on Temperate Sub-Neptune Waterworlds
 
Selaginella: features, morphology ,anatomy and reproduction.
Selaginella: features, morphology ,anatomy and reproduction.Selaginella: features, morphology ,anatomy and reproduction.
Selaginella: features, morphology ,anatomy and reproduction.
 
Site Acceptance Test .
Site Acceptance Test                    .Site Acceptance Test                    .
Site Acceptance Test .
 
Bhiwandi Bhiwandi ❤CALL GIRL 7870993772 ❤CALL GIRLS ESCORT SERVICE In Bhiwan...
Bhiwandi Bhiwandi ❤CALL GIRL 7870993772 ❤CALL GIRLS  ESCORT SERVICE In Bhiwan...Bhiwandi Bhiwandi ❤CALL GIRL 7870993772 ❤CALL GIRLS  ESCORT SERVICE In Bhiwan...
Bhiwandi Bhiwandi ❤CALL GIRL 7870993772 ❤CALL GIRLS ESCORT SERVICE In Bhiwan...
 
PATNA CALL GIRLS 8617370543 LOW PRICE ESCORT SERVICE
PATNA CALL GIRLS 8617370543 LOW PRICE ESCORT SERVICEPATNA CALL GIRLS 8617370543 LOW PRICE ESCORT SERVICE
PATNA CALL GIRLS 8617370543 LOW PRICE ESCORT SERVICE
 
Call Girls Ahmedabad +917728919243 call me Independent Escort Service
Call Girls Ahmedabad +917728919243 call me Independent Escort ServiceCall Girls Ahmedabad +917728919243 call me Independent Escort Service
Call Girls Ahmedabad +917728919243 call me Independent Escort Service
 
Clean In Place(CIP).pptx .
Clean In Place(CIP).pptx                 .Clean In Place(CIP).pptx                 .
Clean In Place(CIP).pptx .
 
Human genetics..........................pptx
Human genetics..........................pptxHuman genetics..........................pptx
Human genetics..........................pptx
 
Asymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 b
Asymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 bAsymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 b
Asymmetry in the atmosphere of the ultra-hot Jupiter WASP-76 b
 
development of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virus
development of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virusdevelopment of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virus
development of diagnostic enzyme assay to detect leuser virus
 
Chemistry 5th semester paper 1st Notes.pdf
Chemistry 5th semester paper 1st Notes.pdfChemistry 5th semester paper 1st Notes.pdf
Chemistry 5th semester paper 1st Notes.pdf
 
Human & Veterinary Respiratory Physilogy_DR.E.Muralinath_Associate Professor....
Human & Veterinary Respiratory Physilogy_DR.E.Muralinath_Associate Professor....Human & Veterinary Respiratory Physilogy_DR.E.Muralinath_Associate Professor....
Human & Veterinary Respiratory Physilogy_DR.E.Muralinath_Associate Professor....
 
TransientOffsetin14CAftertheCarringtonEventRecordedbyPolarTreeRings
TransientOffsetin14CAftertheCarringtonEventRecordedbyPolarTreeRingsTransientOffsetin14CAftertheCarringtonEventRecordedbyPolarTreeRings
TransientOffsetin14CAftertheCarringtonEventRecordedbyPolarTreeRings
 
Climate Change Impacts on Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecosystems.pptx
Climate Change Impacts on Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecosystems.pptxClimate Change Impacts on Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecosystems.pptx
Climate Change Impacts on Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecosystems.pptx
 

Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

  • 1. Reliable individual functional networks and their relationship to behavior Emily S. Finn, PhD Section on Functional Imaging Methods Laboratory of Brain & Cognition, NIMH emily.finn@nih.gov Taking Connectivity to a Skeptical Future Educational Workshop | OHBM Annual Meeting June 25, 2017 @esfinn
  • 2. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 3. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 4. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 5. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 6. Why study individual differences? Controls Patients
  • 7. Why study individual differences? Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors
  • 8. Why study individual differences? Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications:
  • 9. Why study individual differences? Categorical approach Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications:
  • 10. Why study individual differences? Categorical approach Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications:
  • 11. Why study individual differences? Categorical approach Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications:
  • 12. Why study individual differences? Categorical approach Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications: p < 0.05*
  • 13. Why study individual differences? Categorical approach Dimensional approach Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications: p < 0.05*
  • 14. Why study individual differences? Categorical approach Dimensional approach Controls Patients • Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors • Eventual clinical applications: p < 0.05*
  • 15. Why study individual differences? Motor Mt Emotion Em Language Lg Working memory WM Resting R1 + Resting R2 +
  • 16. Why study individual differences? Motor Mt Emotion Em Language Lg Working memory WM Resting R1 Resting R2
  • 17. Why study individual differences? Motor Mt Emotion Em Language Lg Working memory WM Resting R1 Resting R2
  • 18. Why study individual differences? Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
  • 19. Why study individual differences? 0.93 0.84 0.63 0.72 0.79 0.67 0.64 0.60 0.54 R1 WM Mt R2 Lg Em Database Target ID rate 0.5 1.0 Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
  • 20. Why study individual differences? You always look most like yourself, regardless of what you’re doing 0.93 0.84 0.63 0.72 0.79 0.67 0.64 0.60 0.54 R1 WM Mt R2 Lg Em Database Target ID rate 0.5 1.0 Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
  • 21. Why study individual differences? You always look most like yourself, regardless of what you’re doing 0.93 0.84 0.63 0.72 0.79 0.67 0.64 0.60 0.54 R1 WM Mt R2 Lg Em Database Target ID rate 0.5 1.0 Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
  • 22. Why study individual differences? You always look most like yourself, regardless of what you’re doing 0.93 0.84 0.63 0.72 0.79 0.67 0.64 0.60 0.54 R1 WM Mt R2 Lg Em Database Target ID rate 0.5 1.0 Individual differences in FC predict individual differences in behavior Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
  • 23. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 24. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 25.
  • 26. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?
  • 27. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really
  • 28. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
  • 29. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:
  • 30. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions: Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
  • 31. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really • More nodes —> higher identification rate Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions: Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
  • 32. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really • More nodes —> higher identification rate ‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions: Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
  • 33. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really • More nodes —> higher identification rate ‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter ‣ Caution: Higher resolution may amplify effects of anatomical diffs/registration error Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions: Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
  • 34. Q. Do you need HCP-quality data? A. Not really • More nodes —> higher identification rate ‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter ‣ Caution: Higher resolution may amplify effects of anatomical diffs/registration error ‣ Parcellations in the 200-300 node range seem like a good compromise Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions: Courtesy of Jason Druzgal
  • 35.
  • 36. Q. What about amount of data?
  • 37. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters!
  • 38. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters! Longer acquisitions are better:
  • 39. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters! Longer acquisitions are better: • higher reliability within subjects Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
  • 40. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters! Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015) • higher identifiability across subjects Longer acquisitions are better: • higher reliability within subjects Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
  • 41. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters! Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015) • higher identifiability across subjects ‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot make up for shorter scan duration Longer acquisitions are better: • higher reliability within subjects Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
  • 42. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters! Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015) • higher identifiability across subjects ‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot make up for shorter scan duration Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) Longer acquisitions are better: • higher reliability within subjects Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
  • 43. Q. What about amount of data? A. Scan duration matters! Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015) • higher identifiability across subjects ‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot make up for shorter scan duration Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016) Shah et al., Brain & Behav (2016) Longer acquisitions are better: • higher reliability within subjects Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)
  • 44.
  • 45. Q. Does scan condition matter?
  • 46. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes!
  • 47. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
  • 48. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise 0.93 0.84 0.63 0.72 0.79 0.67 0.64 0.60 0.54 R1 WM Mt R2 Lg Em Database Target ID rate 0.5 1.0
  • 49. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise
  • 50. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 51. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 52. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 53. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 54. Q. Does scan condition matter? A. Yes! Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise ‣ for a set scan duration, tasks may be more reliable than rest ‣ tasks may converge faster on a subject’s “true” profile Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 55.
  • 56. Q. Is rest best?
  • 57. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not
  • 58. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
  • 59. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not ‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:
  • 60. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not ‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions Target Database Day 1 Day 2 IDrate Day1Day2 Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR: Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 61. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not ‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions Target Database Day 1 Day 2 IDrate Day1Day2 ‣ consider a combination of rest and task Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR: Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 62. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not ‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions Target Database Day 1 Day 2 IDrate Day1Day2 ‣ consider a combination of rest and task Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR: Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017) Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)
  • 63. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not
  • 64. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks:
  • 65. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks: Inscapes: Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage 2015 headspacestudios.org
  • 66. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks:
  • 67. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks: ‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest
  • 68. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks: Session 1 Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11 Session 2 ‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest
  • 69. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks: Session 1 Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11 Session 2 ‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 70. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Consider naturalistic tasks: Session 1 Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11 Session 2 ‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 71. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not
  • 72. Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
  • 73. ‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
  • 74. ‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations Huijbers et al., NeuroImage (2017) Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
  • 75. ‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations ChildrenAdults Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2015)Huijbers et al., NeuroImage (2017) Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not Tasks also have purely practical advantages:
  • 76. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 77. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 78. How to choose behavior
  • 79. How to choose behavior Is it stable?
  • 80. How to choose behavior Is it stable? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis
  • 81. How to choose behavior Is it stable? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 82. How to choose behavior Is it stable? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 83. How to choose behavior Is it stable? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 84. How to choose behavior Is it stable? Does it show a good distribution in your population? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 85. How to choose behavior Is it stable? Does it show a good distribution in your population? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 86. How to choose behavior Is it stable? Does it show a good distribution in your population? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 87. How to choose behavior Is it stable? Does it show a good distribution in your population? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 88. How to choose behavior Is it stable? Does it show a good distribution in your population? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 89. How to choose behavior Is it stable? Does it show a good distribution in your population? Is it something you expect to be reflected in brain function? • Trait vs. state • State variables may be better suited to within-subject analysis Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)
  • 91. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
  • 92. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! Siegel et al., Cerebral Cortex (2016) Negatively: Positively:
  • 93. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! Siegel et al., Cerebral Cortex (2016) Negatively: Positively: Geerligs et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2017) Age, vascular health:
  • 94. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!
  • 95. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! ‣ Check for correlation in your sample
  • 96. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! ‣ Check for correlation in your sample ‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects
  • 97. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! ‣ Check for correlation in your sample ‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects ‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques
  • 98. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! ‣ Check for correlation in your sample ‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects ‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques Ciric et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 99. Behavior: Mitigating confounds Many behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion! ‣ Check for correlation in your sample ‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects ‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques ‣ Use motion as an explicit covariate Ciric et al., NeuroImage (2017)
  • 100. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 101. Outline Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5How to get good behavioral data 3
  • 103. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample
  • 104. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)
  • 105. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 106. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 107. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 108. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 109. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 110. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 111. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 112. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 113. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 114. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 115. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3
  • 116. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) ‣ even better: cross-dataset Sustained attention model Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3 Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016) Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)
  • 117. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) ‣ even better: cross-dataset Sustained attention model Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3 Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016) Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017) Observed behav Predictedbehav n = 25 adults
  • 118. Brain behavior: Cross-validate! Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample ‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset) ‣ even better: cross-dataset Sustained attention model Connectome-based Predictive Modeling Shen et al., Nature Protocols (2017) Predictedbehav Observed behav 2 7.5 4 6.3 Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016) Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017) Observed behav Predictedbehav n = 25 adults Observed ADHD score n = 113 children PredictedADHDscore
  • 119.
  • 120. Q. What is the best brain state?
  • 121. Q. What is the best brain state? A. Maybe it depends on your behavior
  • 122. Q. What is the best brain state? A. Maybe it depends on your behavior Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:
  • 123. Q. What is the best brain state? A. Maybe it depends on your behavior n = 716, 10-fold cross-validation Connectome-based Predictive Modeling (CPM; Shen et al., Nat Protocols 2017) Modelinputdata Target behavior Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:
  • 124. Q. What is the best brain state? A. Maybe it depends on your behavior n = 716, 10-fold cross-validation Connectome-based Predictive Modeling (CPM; Shen et al., Nat Protocols 2017) Modelinputdata Target behavior Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior: Consider tailoring scan condition to behavior of interest ➡“Stress test”?
  • 125. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5
  • 126. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical
  • 127. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better!
  • 128. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks
  • 129. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance
  • 130. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance ‣ increase sensitivity
  • 131. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance ‣ increase sensitivity • Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution
  • 132. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance ‣ increase sensitivity • Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution • Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them
  • 133. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance ‣ increase sensitivity • Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution • Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them • Cross-validate whenever possible
  • 134. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance ‣ increase sensitivity • Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution • Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them • Cross-validate whenever possible • Consider tailoring your scan condition to behavior of interest
  • 135. Take-home points Introduction: Why study individual differences? 1 How to get good brain data 2 How to get good behavioral data 3 How to relate brain to behavior 4 Learn more5 • Data quality ≠ critical • Longer scans are better! • Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance ‣ increase sensitivity • Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution • Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them • Cross-validate whenever possible • Consider tailoring your scan condition to behavior of interest
  • 136. Open data sets with brain & behavior ➡ Use these on their own or in combination with your own data to generate or test hypotheses, see if a finding generalizes, etc Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort
  • 137. Further reading Building a science of individual differences from fMRI Dubois & Adolphs, Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2016) From regions to connections and networks: new bridges between brain and behavior Misic & Sporns, Current Opinion in Neurobiology (2016) Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual differences in functional connectivity? Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017) Prediction as a humanitarian and pragmatic contribution from human cognitive neuroscience Gabrieli, Ghosh & Gabrieli, Neuron (2015) Selected reviews:
  • 138. Learn more at OHBM 2017 Symposium: Collect your thoughts: Individual differences in the networks underlying intelligence Tues 8:00-9:15am Symposium: Relating connectivity to inter- and intra-individual differences in attention and cognition Weds 8:00-9:15am Poster #4042: Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual differences in functional connectivity? Finn et al. Poster #4040: Large-scale functional connectivity networks predict attention fluctuations Rosenberg et al. Poster #2110: Functional connectivity-based predictors of naturalistic reading comprehension Jangraw et al. Poster #4029: FMRI connectivity is differentially associated with performance across tasks in a multi-task study Topolski et al. @esfinn