SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  16
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 1
Components of Infidelity:
Who Is Most Likely to Cheat in Romance?
Kelly Burke
Saint Louis University
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 2
Another field in which the tendency towards infidelity can be analyzed is the “social and
cultural domain,” which states that “personality affects, and is affected by, the social and cultural
context in which it is found” (Larsen and Buss 2014). A study done by Pham et al. examines
“coalitional mate retention,” or a process in which “individuals ask an ally to assist with
thwarting their romantic partner’s infidelity” (Pham et al. 2015). Because coalitional mate
retention involves the influence of the community and a tendency towards gendered norms, it
would fall under the category of social and cultural domain. In this study, Pham et al. aimed to
chart the correlation between the frequency in which participants “requested or received
coalitional mate retention” from both a male friend and a female friend, and the perceived quality
of each friendship (2015). The central question examined whether the amount of coalitional
mate retention requests correlated negatively or positively to the perceived quality of the
friendships, not accounting for the success of the actual retention process (Pham et al. 2015).
Examination of this correlation aims to determine whether coalitional mate retention is a
beneficial practice to the quality of an individual’s friendships. If it builds firmer bonds within a
society, it may be a behavior worth encouraging, as it has “solved sex-specific adaptive
problems” over the course of evolution; if it weakens friendships, however, then it should be
discouraged, as coalitional mate retention has been shown to correlate positively with instances
of allies becoming mate-poachers or otherwise sabotaging the reputation of a former ally (Pham
et al. 2015).
Pham et al. predicted that coalitional mate retention would correlate positively to a high
quality of friendship in relationships involving women: female-female, female-male, and male-
female (2015). The researchers formulated this hypothesis based on past studies showing that
women consider the sharing of personal information to be vital in the establishment and
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 3
maintenance of platonic friendship, as such bonds are formed on trust and mutual confidence.
Coalitional mate retention fundamentally requires that the allies are open to each other about
themselves and their relationships, indicating that the sharing of personal information must take
place (2015). Coalitional mate retention was predicted to correlate negatively to male-male
friendships, as men do not usually share personal information due to a fear of reputational
damage, and are thus much less likely to open up about themselves without the encouragement
of a woman. Men are also less likely to request coalitional mate retention because a majority of
them consider it a sign of weakness or dependence running counter to their masculine ideal; it is
analogous to men’s refusal of health care under the same premises (Pham et al. 2015). Also,
men are more likely to become jealous of their male ally, especially because the “monopolizing”
of the observed partner’s time has the potential to develop into mate-poaching.
To observe the correlation between high quality friendships and coalitional mate
retention, Pham et al. administered the McGill Friendship questionnaire to 387 participants. The
questionnaire has six dimensions: Help, Intimacy, Reliable Alliance, Stimulating
Companionship, Self-Validation, and Emotional Security. The first three dimensions were
focused most closely, as current research supports that they have the greatest overall correlation
to coalitional mate retention (Pham et al. 2015). Of the 387 participants, 176 were women, and
all of the participants were involved in committed, heterosexual relationships which lasted at
least one year. Each participant reported on two of their heterosexual friends, one male and one
female, whom they had described as being close friends whom they had known for at least a
year. On a scale from 0-8, the participants would report “how often their friend fulfilled
friendship roles,” with 0 representative of never and 8 representative of always (Pham et al.
2015). Participants then completed the Coalitional Mate Retention Inventory for each friend; it
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 4
was structured on a 0-3 scale and consisted of 44 items. The questions determined 1) how often
the participant requested the friend to partake in coalitional mate retention practices within the
past year and 2) how often the participant believed these friends actually fulfilled the request
(Pham et al. 2015).
To determine the results, Pham et al. calculated “friendship-quality variables from the
mean of responses to items” which fell into the Help, Intimacy, and Reliable Alliance categories.
The researchers then used the Coalitional Mate Inventory to calculate seven “coalitional mate
retention tactic variables” which were the “mean of responses to items comprising each tactic”
used while cooperating with an ally. These domains were: Manipulation, Praise, Vigilance,
Therapy, Gifts, Monopolizing Time, and Violence (Pham et al. 2015). The data was then placed
through two binomial tests, which examined all four friendship conditions: female-female,
female-male, male-female, and male-male. The first of the two tests “assessed the proportion of
positive correlations out of the 48 total correlations” while the second test “assessed the
proportion of statistically significant positive correlations out of the total number of statistically
significant correlations” (Pham et al. 2015).
Male-female friendships correlated positively to the use of coalitional mate retention,
with 41 of the 48 possible correlations being positive and 19 of the 19 statistically significant
correlations being positive. Female-male friendships also correlated positively to the use of
coalitional mate retention, with 46 of the 48 possible correlations being positive and 31 of the 31
statistically significant correlations being positive. Female-female friendship correlated
positively to the use of coalitional mate retention, with 45 of the 48 possible correlations being
positive and 21 of the 21 statistically significant correlations being positive. These statistics
supported the hypothesis of relationships involving women being more likely to involve the use
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 5
of coalitional mate retention, as women are the impetus behind such behavior (Pham et al. 2015).
Male-male friendships, however, correlated negatively with the use of coalitional mate retention,
with 33 of the 48 correlations being negative, and 13 of the 16 statistically significant
correlations being negative as well. These findings are also consistent with the hypothesis, as
male-male friendships correlate negatively with the sharing of personal information; the lack of
women in the relationship prompts men to guard their dignity, even around male friends with
whom they have a high-quality relationship. Men are also more concerned about the possibility
of mate poaching, which correlates positively with coalitional mate retention (Pham et al. 2015).
Another factor to potentially consider is the difference between the ways which men and women
interpret close opposite-sex relationships. Men, for example, “perceive an intimate opposite-sex
friendship as a mate poaching opportunity” more so than women do, which might be a relevant
factor as to why men are typically less willing to engage in coalitional mate retention (Pham et
al. 2015).
There are a few problems with the study, however. For example, gender differences may
be inadequately accounted for in terms of how the participants actually spend time with their
same-sex friends, a flaw which the study itself admits. Men may spend more time with their
close same-sex friends than females do, which would leave less time for the men to actually
engage in coalitional mate retention. The study also admits that it suffers from the classical
correlational study loophole: does a precede b, or does b precede a (Pham et al. 2015)? Another
potential pitfall of the study is that the sample size only consisted of heterosexual participants.
Homosexual participants might consider their relationships in different ways than which
heterosexual participants might; for example, a gay man might feel more comfortable being more
open with information than a straight man because he might not feel constrained by a
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 6
heteronormative masculine ideal. Homosexuals could be incorporated into the heterosexual
sample size, or the same experiment could be performed again on a homosexual population to
determine how societal gender roles influence concept of friendship and coalitional mate
retention.
This study was valuable for defining the ways in which men and women classify the
closeness of their friendships and how this, in turn, correlates with their sexual tendencies. It
was not surprising to learn that women are more likely to engage in coalitional mate retention
when reflecting upon gender norms. These norms typically expect women to behave in a more
community-oriented manner, which extends to their sexual practices. The male-male results also
aligned with gender norms, as men are expected to be more individualistic and capable. This
ideal masculinity ties into the tendency for males to behave more unfaithfully than females, as
they view friendships as a gateway to intimacy in many cases. Thus, this study can be said to
show that men are more likely than women to commit infidelity, and are less likely to help others
avoid unfaithful behaviors due to the advantage they may gain from a potential mate-poaching.
Infidelity can also be studied within the adjustment domain, which states that
“personality plays a key role in how we cope, adapt, and adjust to the ebb and flow of events in
our day-to-day lives” (Larsen and Buss 2014). It typically relates to one’s physical and mental
health when faced with a variety of stressors, and is thus often invoked in cases of mental illness.
A 2015 study done by Ein-Dor et al. falls under the adjustment domain, as it focuses on how men
and women cope with threats of mate-poaching. The study was generated to test the “rivalry
centered hypothesis,” which posits that women are generally “more sensitive to cues of infidelity
than men are,” tending to “focus their attention on potential rivals in their mate's vicinity.” Men,
on the other hand, are more likely to monitor their own partner for signs of infidelity and focus
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 7
less on potential rivals, a behavior which has recently been postulated to be unique to their sex.
This implies that although men and women generally adopt different strategies for dealing with
mate-poaching, they have the same ultimate goal: mate retention (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). By
monitoring their partner’s behavior, men and women can attempt to prevent a “defection” and
subsequent break in the relationship. These behaviors have evolved because infidelity, which
occurs at fairly high rates within the population (about 22–25% of men and 11–15% of women),
is typically very destructive mentally, physically, emotionally, and financially (Ein-Dor et al.
2015).
In the rivalry-centered hypothesis, women were “expected to show overall higher
infidelity-detection sensitivity than men” because their coping strategy involves monitoring
multiple persons, whereas men’s coping strategy only focuses the attention on their mate. These
coping strategies are believed to be due, in part, to evolutionary mechanisms, as men can
physically coerce their partner into obeying them, whereas women are typically not strong
enough to use this same tactic against their male partners. Thus, they must adopt a preventative
strategy aimed at nixing infidelity before it can occur, or to prevent an existing extra-
monogamous relationship from progressing any further (Ein-Dor et al. 2015).
To test the rivalry-centered hypothesis, the researchers set up four different smaller
studies, using the combined results to test their theory. The first study “examined whether men
and women differ in the extent to which they appraise ambiguous partner-related incidents as
comprising a threat of infidelity.” 194 participants (80 men and 114 women) “completed self-
report measures of partner distrust, history of infidelity, and socio-economic status” and then
were asked whether or not they believed if a “partner-related event” indicated a distinct threat
towards infidelity; the questions were set up in a yes-no format. Women were hypothesized to
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 8
judge more events as a threat towards infidelity than men, as past studies have shown that
women are more likely than men to mistrust their romantic partners (Ein-Dor et al. 2015).
Participants first completed a 5-item self-report measuring “the extent to which they tend to
mistrust romantic partners.” The self-report was set up with a scale from 1-7 with one being “not
at all” and 7 being “very much.” After the questionnaire was completed, participants were
instructed to read 11 vignettes depicting “ambiguous signs of partner unfaithfulness” and were
then asked whether or not the situation constituted infidelity. The researchers totaled the amount
of vignettes that were affirmed to be portraying infidelity for each participant. Once all the
vignettes had been categorized as situations of infidelity or not infidelity, the participants were
asked “Did one of your past or present romantic partners ever commit an act of sexual
infidelity?” and “Were you ever involved in a romantic affair while in a committed
relationship?” (Ein-Dor et al. 2015).
More women than men reported that their partner had cheated on them, which correlates
to the higher rate of women perceiving infidelity; a person who has been cheated on is more
likely to be more aware of the same cues in a different setting. More women also considered the
vignettes to be depicting sexual infidelity than men, affirming the initial hypothesis that men are
more likely than women to interpret ambiguous situations as infidelity. Study 2 was designed to
test whether women were more accurate at depicting infidelity than men, or whether their
suspicions were actually false alarms. The sample size of Study 2 was 118 heterosexual adults
(44 men and 74 women); about twenty-five percent reported that a current or past partner had
been unfaithful to them, while about thirty-eight percent admitted that they were “involved in a
sexual affair” (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). Participants in this study were asked to identify a picture
representing infidelity as quickly as possible from “a matrix of control pictures.” After the
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 9
picture was correctly identified by being clicked on by the participant, the matrix would change
and they would have to locate a different infidelity picture. The timing ranged from 2-22
seconds to identify the picture, while the number of attempts to select the correct picture ranged
from 1-17.
On average, women detected the infidelity pictures more quickly than men and made
fewer errors. These findings affirmed the hypothesis that women detect romantic threat more
quickly and with greater accuracy then men. To affirm that women were indeed more skilled at
detecting romantic threat and not threat in general, Study 3 was implemented. The sample
population of Study 3 consisted of 93 heterosexual participants (47 men and 46 women). The
participants first completed the test described in Study 2 and then completed an additional test,
which asked them to identify the picture on a matrix which represented danger from a poisonous
animal. Once again, women scored lower times and fewer errors than men on the infidelity
identification, but both women and men scored about equally on the poisonous animal detection
test. This confirms the hypothesis that women detect infidelity quicker and more accurately than
men, and that the skill is situation-specific, not applying to all instances of danger (Ein-Dor et al.
2015). Study 4 was designed to test the final part of the hypothesis, namely, the theory that
women focus more closely on potential romantic rivals than men do. Participants were first
asked to complete a self-report measuring “partner distrust, history of infidelity, and socio-
economic status,” and were then asked to complete a computerized classification task, which
measured the participants’ “perception of within-couple threats and threats that reside outside of
the relationship.”
The sample size consisted of 127 heterosexual participants (60 men and 67 women). Ten
percent of the sample reported an unfaithful partner while thirteen percent reported that they
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 10
were involved or had been involved in a romantic affair. The participants first completed the
spousal distrust questionnaire, and were then tasked to complete another visual test. In this
variation, a couple was seated at a bar, and an attractive man or woman was seated close by. The
first phase of the test examined the detection of within-couple infidelity, and the series of
pictures portrayed one member of the couple looking at his or her significant other, looking at an
indeterminate point in space, or looking passionately at the attractive stranger. Participants were
asked to evaluate the possibility that the partner would stray based on his or her level of eye
contact. In the second phase, the participants were asked to evaluate the threat of infidelity from
outside the relationship. Physically attractive women and dominant males were used as the
outside threat in this picture series, as these are traits which each gender finds most threatening
to mate retention. Participants were asked to indicate whether the desirable person was looking
at one member of the couple and whether or not this presented a threat to mate retention. In the
first phase, pictures which depicted ambiguous gaze direction evoked responses that “men were
significantly more likely than women to state that the target's partner was looking at the
attractive stranger,” presenting a within-relationship threat to mate retention. This affirms the
hypothesis that men are more likely to guard their own mate than to focus on external threats. In
the second phase, “women were significantly more likely than men to state that the attractive
stranger was looking at the target's partner,” except in the non-ambiguous situations. Study 4
affirms the initial hypothesis positing that women are more likely to be concerned with same-sex
rivals outside of the relationship, whereas men are more likely to keep watch over their own
partner.
While the studies successfully supported the hypothesis, there were a few problems with
the studies. As the researchers admit, the studies solely examined the participants’ ability to
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 11
detect infidelity within other people’s relationships, not within their own. Detecting infidelity
without the benefit of being an outsider is considerably more difficult, and future studies should
address this disparity when accounting for overall detection ability. Also, in Studies 1 and 2, the
sample size of women significantly outnumbered the sample size of men, which could skew the
results in favor of women’s detection abilities. Lastly, the four components of the larger
hypothesis utilized different persons for the sample population. Thus it lacks within-subject
testing although all the studies were essential to test the same hypothesis.
This article confirms that it is much easier for a man to cheat than for a woman to cheat.
While men only have one partner to observe, women have to watch multiple targets, so the
chance of them missing a cue is greater even though they are overall more able to detect
infidelity. Because men have a greater ability to successfully engage in infidelity, they are more
likely to actually do so. Also, it confirms that although men and women face the threat of
infidelity in different ways and in different frequencies, both are equally affected emotionally
and mentally. Thus both strive to avoid infidelity and retain their partner despite living in a
culture which normalizes extramarital relationships to a certain extent. It will be interesting to
see whether perceptions about infidelity change if the rates continue to grow over time.
Taking all four studies into account, one can conclude that unmarried men with high
levels of social confidence, who have also been unfaithful in the past, are more likely to commit
infidelity than women. The first study examined infidelity intentions, which posited that persons
with a positive outlook on infidelity are most likely to actually carry through with unfaithful
behavior, as beliefs are thought to correlate closely with realized actions (Jackman 2014).
Persons who view themselves as more in control of their behavior and their social situation are
also more likely to engage in infidelity. Men fall into this category more often than women do,
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 12
as gender norms encourage the former to be more assertive and autonomous. It is also easier to
carry out infidelity if one is unmarried, as there is not a spouse that one must constantly
circumvent; past studies have also shown, on average, that married persons have a more negative
opinion on infidelity and are thus less likely to engage in the behavior.
Biological factors also correlate heavily with infidelity. It is more evolutionarily
advantageous for a man to engage in infidelity than a woman, as he is able to spread his genes to
multiple partners at one time. Women, however, can only carry the offspring of one partner at a
time and are tasked with nursing and other forms of caregiving, so it is more advantageous for
them to have a partner who will steadily provide them with resources. Therefore, women are
typically less likely to cheat due to the benefits their partners can bring them. Infidelity in
women was typically an adaptive behavior to keep the DNA pool diverse when humans lived in
small units. Today this is not an issue, as genetic relatives live further apart and potential mates
are geographically much closer, which correlates to decreased rates of women engaging in
infidelity, as the adaptation has mostly become obsolete. Thus, women are more likely to cheat
when there is an obvious advantage to the behavior but otherwise tend to avoid it under a
majority of circumstances. Women with more active Vasopressin receptors, however, were
found to exhibit higher percentages of cheating behavior, but it is currently unknown if the
receptor influences the behavior or vice versa (Zietsch et al. 2014).
Men with close oppose-sex friends are more likely to engage in infidelity as well. As
men typically consider this friendship as a gateway into a relationship, they are more willing to
make romantic advances than women who are in a close friendship with men. This infidelity
often comes in the form of mate-poaching, which could theoretically lead to double counts of
infidelity if both man and poached female mate are already in relationships (Pham et al. 2015).
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 13
Persons who have been the victims of infidelity, a majority of them women, are less likely to
engage in infidelity themselves. They are also better suited to recognize infidelity and will thus
adjust their behavior to avoid repeating the unfavorable scenario. Women are also more suited to
recognize infidelity because the odds of a woman being a victim of infidelity are greater than of
a man being a victim of infidelity, even if the woman has not been victimized before. Lastly,
women are less likely to partake in infidelity because men are more likely to guard their mate.
With focused attention directed towards them, women have less means to engage in infidelity
without being discovered (Ein-Dor et al. 2015)
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 14
References
Ein-Dor, Tsachi., Perry-Paldi, Adi., Hirschberger, Gilad., Birnbaum, Gurit E., and Deutsch,
Danit. (January 2015).
Coping with mate poaching: gender differences in detection of infidelity-related threats.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(1), 17-24. Retrieved from
http://eds.a.ebscohost.com/eds/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=6d064af6-a56c-44e9-9d77-
a270da2805b9%40sessionmgr4004&hid=4111&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d
#db=edselp&AN=S1090513814001019.
AB: People often aspire for true love and committed romantic relationships. These
relationships, however, are recurrently threatened by partner infidelity. The present
research tested a new infidelity-detection model, the rivalry sensitivity hypothesis, that
posits that women are more sensitive to cues of infidelity than men are, and tend to focus
their attention on potential rivals in their mate's vicinity, whereas men show increased
sensitivity of their own partners. In a series of four studies, we found that women
displayed greater alertness to cues of potential partner unfaithfulness than did men, were
quicker and more accurate in detecting cues of infidelity, but were not better than men in
detecting other threats. Women also focused their attention on potential rivals (other
women), whereas men's attention was specifically directed at monitoring their own
partner's intents. These findings suggest that women and men have developed different
strategies aimed at achieving a similar outcome – mate retention.
Jackman, M. (2014). Understanding the Cheating Heart: What Determines Infidelity Intentions?
Sexuality & Culture, 19(1), 72-84. Retrieved from http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com/sp-
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 15
3.15.1b/ovidweb.cgi?&S=PBGAFPMHPFDDPOHONCKKBEDCMDJMAA00&Comple
te+Reference=S.sh.21%7c4%7c1.
Larsen, L.J. and Buss, D.M. (2014). Personality Psychology: Domains of Knowledge About
Human Nature (5th edition). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education.
Pham, M.N., Mogilski, J.K., Shackelford, T.K., and Barbaro, N. (2015) Coalitional mate
retention is correlated positively with friendship quality involving women, but negatively
with male–male friendship quality. Personality and Individual Differences, 79, 87-90.
Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com/eds/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=18d25fef-5d90-
45f5-958c-
c7f421bdb097%40sessionmgr115&hid=119&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d#d
b=edselc&AN=edselc.2-52.0-84922998553.
AB: Friendship solved adaptive problems over human evolutionary history, including
cooperative hunting and alloparenting. Pham, Barbaro, and Shackelford (in press)
investigated another potential function of friendship: the provision of coalitional mate
retention, whereby individuals ask an ally to assist with thwarting their romantic partner’s
infidelity. In the current research, 387 participants (176 women) reported how often they
requested or received coalitional mate retention from a male friend and from a female
friend and reported on the quality of each friendship. The results indicate that the
deployment of coalitional mate retention is positively associated with the quality of
friendships with women (female–female friendships, male–female friendships, female–
male friendships), but negatively associated with the quality of male–male friendships.
COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 16
Zietsch, B.P., Jern, P., Westberg, L., and Santtila, P. (2014).
Genetic analysis of human extrapair mating: Heritability, between-sex correlation,
andreceptor genes for vasopressin and oxytocin. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(2),
130-136. Retrieved from
http://eds.b.ebscohost.com/eds/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=54860916-c006-43b9-9aa4-
90503bb6c5b6%40sessionmgr111&hid=126&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d#
db=edselc&AN=edselc.2-52.0-84922930492.

Contenu connexe

Tendances

[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)
[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)
[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)Bui Huong
 
MarriageandtheFamResearchPaper
MarriageandtheFamResearchPaperMarriageandtheFamResearchPaper
MarriageandtheFamResearchPaperNykolai Blichar
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaAlejandro Luna
 
SurveyProject_Exclusive Relationships
SurveyProject_Exclusive RelationshipsSurveyProject_Exclusive Relationships
SurveyProject_Exclusive RelationshipsSherri Wielgos
 
Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010
Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010
Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010Jarryd_Willis
 
Effects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of Fathers
Effects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of FathersEffects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of Fathers
Effects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of FathersMatthew Baumann
 
Research Methods: Data Analysis
Research Methods: Data AnalysisResearch Methods: Data Analysis
Research Methods: Data AnalysisBhumi Patel
 
Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?
Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?
Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?CrimsonpublishersPPrs
 
Literature Review
Literature ReviewLiterature Review
Literature ReviewMegan White
 
Topics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic RelationshipsTopics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic RelationshipsBrittany Weber
 
An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)
An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)
An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)Eliseo Vera
 
Kate dodge gender roles powerpoint
Kate dodge gender roles powerpointKate dodge gender roles powerpoint
Kate dodge gender roles powerpointHorses21
 
Sean - Academic Research Project
Sean - Academic Research ProjectSean - Academic Research Project
Sean - Academic Research ProjectPeneyra
 

Tendances (19)

Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
 
[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)
[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)
[Carey m. noland]_sex_talk_the_role_of_communicat(book4_you)
 
JenniferCisco_Final
JenniferCisco_FinalJenniferCisco_Final
JenniferCisco_Final
 
MarriageandtheFamResearchPaper
MarriageandtheFamResearchPaperMarriageandtheFamResearchPaper
MarriageandtheFamResearchPaper
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
 
SurveyProject_Exclusive Relationships
SurveyProject_Exclusive RelationshipsSurveyProject_Exclusive Relationships
SurveyProject_Exclusive Relationships
 
Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010
Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010
Patterns of insecure attachment spsp 2010
 
Cramer et al 2008
Cramer et al 2008Cramer et al 2008
Cramer et al 2008
 
Final Research Report
Final Research ReportFinal Research Report
Final Research Report
 
Effects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of Fathers
Effects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of FathersEffects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of Fathers
Effects of Sexuality and Career Choice on Perceived Femininity of Fathers
 
Research Methods: Data Analysis
Research Methods: Data AnalysisResearch Methods: Data Analysis
Research Methods: Data Analysis
 
Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?
Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?
Crimson Publishers-How Well Do You Know Your Best Friend?
 
Literature Review
Literature ReviewLiterature Review
Literature Review
 
Topics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic RelationshipsTopics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
Topics of Conflict in Romantic Relationships
 
An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)
An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)
An empirical test of low self-control theory among hispanic youth (Published)
 
Kate dodge gender roles powerpoint
Kate dodge gender roles powerpointKate dodge gender roles powerpoint
Kate dodge gender roles powerpoint
 
Santor
SantorSantor
Santor
 
Sean - Academic Research Project
Sean - Academic Research ProjectSean - Academic Research Project
Sean - Academic Research Project
 
Fieldwork plan
Fieldwork planFieldwork plan
Fieldwork plan
 

En vedette

Medios de comunicacion
Medios de comunicacionMedios de comunicacion
Medios de comunicacionaracelisjg20
 
PPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOK
PPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOKPPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOK
PPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOKJared Delana
 
JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)
JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)
JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)CrackDSE
 
XSport Corporate wellness
XSport Corporate wellnessXSport Corporate wellness
XSport Corporate wellnessDenisse Montoya
 
DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008
DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008
DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008Robert J. Abate, CBIP, CDMP
 

En vedette (7)

Medios de comunicacion
Medios de comunicacionMedios de comunicacion
Medios de comunicacion
 
PPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOK
PPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOKPPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOK
PPO USA STRESS LESS eBOOK
 
Resume
ResumeResume
Resume
 
JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)
JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)
JNU MA Economics Entrance Test Paper (2012)
 
XSport Corporate wellness
XSport Corporate wellnessXSport Corporate wellness
XSport Corporate wellness
 
CONOZCA LOS EFECTOS DEL FENÓMENO EL NIÑO EN AMÉRICA LATINA ppt
CONOZCA LOS EFECTOS DEL FENÓMENO EL NIÑO EN AMÉRICA LATINA  pptCONOZCA LOS EFECTOS DEL FENÓMENO EL NIÑO EN AMÉRICA LATINA  ppt
CONOZCA LOS EFECTOS DEL FENÓMENO EL NIÑO EN AMÉRICA LATINA ppt
 
DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008
DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008
DAMA International Symposium San Diego CA 03-17-2008
 

Similaire à Paper 2

Thesis Max Alley
Thesis Max AlleyThesis Max Alley
Thesis Max AlleyMax Alley
 
Article Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docx
Article Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docxArticle Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docx
Article Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docxdavezstarr61655
 
Relationships – Topic 5 Psychology Alevel
Relationships – Topic 5 Psychology AlevelRelationships – Topic 5 Psychology Alevel
Relationships – Topic 5 Psychology AlevelEvie-Anne Davis
 
Running head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docx
Running head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docxRunning head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docx
Running head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docxrtodd599
 
Jarryd willis LGBT & straight attachment bonds
Jarryd willis   LGBT & straight attachment bondsJarryd willis   LGBT & straight attachment bonds
Jarryd willis LGBT & straight attachment bondsJarryd_Willis
 
Attachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human DevelopmentAttachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human DevelopmentLacey Desper
 
Masculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docx
Masculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docxMasculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docx
Masculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docxalfredacavx97
 
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literatureConfiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literatureBASPCAN
 
Smith_Romantic_Relationships_Study
Smith_Romantic_Relationships_StudySmith_Romantic_Relationships_Study
Smith_Romantic_Relationships_StudyRachel Smith
 
Unit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docx
Unit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docxUnit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docx
Unit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docxlillie234567
 
Friends With Benefits Relationships Research
Friends With Benefits Relationships ResearchFriends With Benefits Relationships Research
Friends With Benefits Relationships ResearchNicholas Derry
 
Comparing attachment patterns across relationships
Comparing attachment patterns across relationshipsComparing attachment patterns across relationships
Comparing attachment patterns across relationshipsJarryd_Willis
 
Gender differences
Gender differencesGender differences
Gender differencesSamerYaqoob
 
Falling in love online
Falling in love onlineFalling in love online
Falling in love onlinepaigedenine
 
kissandra project in ICT
kissandra project in ICTkissandra project in ICT
kissandra project in ICTKissandra
 

Similaire à Paper 2 (20)

Thesis Max Alley
Thesis Max AlleyThesis Max Alley
Thesis Max Alley
 
Article Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docx
Article Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docxArticle Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docx
Article Summary Table – Template2Full Refe.docx
 
Relationships – Topic 5 Psychology Alevel
Relationships – Topic 5 Psychology AlevelRelationships – Topic 5 Psychology Alevel
Relationships – Topic 5 Psychology Alevel
 
Running head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docx
Running head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docxRunning head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docx
Running head What factors affect the quality of our relationships.docx
 
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared ResourceSociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
 
Jarryd willis LGBT & straight attachment bonds
Jarryd willis   LGBT & straight attachment bondsJarryd willis   LGBT & straight attachment bonds
Jarryd willis LGBT & straight attachment bonds
 
Panel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn Literacy
Panel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn LiteracyPanel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn Literacy
Panel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn Literacy
 
Attachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human DevelopmentAttachment Theory in Human Development
Attachment Theory in Human Development
 
Research
ResearchResearch
Research
 
Masculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docx
Masculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docxMasculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docx
Masculinity and Barriers to Seeking Counseling The Buffering.docx
 
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literatureConfiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
 
Smith_Romantic_Relationships_Study
Smith_Romantic_Relationships_StudySmith_Romantic_Relationships_Study
Smith_Romantic_Relationships_Study
 
Unit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docx
Unit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docxUnit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docx
Unit Three Interpersonal Communication in ActionEric L. Mor.docx
 
Friends With Benefits Relationships Research
Friends With Benefits Relationships ResearchFriends With Benefits Relationships Research
Friends With Benefits Relationships Research
 
ISI Paper 1 (1)
ISI Paper 1 (1)ISI Paper 1 (1)
ISI Paper 1 (1)
 
Comparing attachment patterns across relationships
Comparing attachment patterns across relationshipsComparing attachment patterns across relationships
Comparing attachment patterns across relationships
 
Gender differences
Gender differencesGender differences
Gender differences
 
Falling in love online
Falling in love onlineFalling in love online
Falling in love online
 
kissandra project in ICT
kissandra project in ICTkissandra project in ICT
kissandra project in ICT
 
Educational & Social Considerations in Spouse Selection: Preferences of Peopl...
Educational & Social Considerations in Spouse Selection: Preferences of Peopl...Educational & Social Considerations in Spouse Selection: Preferences of Peopl...
Educational & Social Considerations in Spouse Selection: Preferences of Peopl...
 

Plus de Kelly Burke

Plus de Kelly Burke (9)

Paper 1
Paper 1Paper 1
Paper 1
 
Final Portfolio I
Final Portfolio IFinal Portfolio I
Final Portfolio I
 
Final portfolio
Final portfolioFinal portfolio
Final portfolio
 
Schizophrenia and working memory
Schizophrenia and working memorySchizophrenia and working memory
Schizophrenia and working memory
 
Capstone
CapstoneCapstone
Capstone
 
CAPSTONE
CAPSTONECAPSTONE
CAPSTONE
 
Resumé
ResuméResumé
Resumé
 
Resumé
ResuméResumé
Resumé
 
Resumé
ResuméResumé
Resumé
 

Dernier

Hire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls AgencyHire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls AgencyNitya salvi
 
Call girls in Vashi Services : 9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your Doorstep
Call girls in Vashi Services :  9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your DoorstepCall girls in Vashi Services :  9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your Doorstep
Call girls in Vashi Services : 9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your DoorstepPooja Nehwal
 
Top 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon Biotech
Top 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon BiotechTop 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon Biotech
Top 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon BiotechStelon Biotech
 
UDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICE
UDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICEUDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICE
UDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICEApsara Of India
 
💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞
💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞
💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞Apsara Of India
 
💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service
💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service
💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort ServiceApsara Of India
 
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...rajveerescorts2022
 
Pooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home Delivery
Pooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home DeliveryPooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home Delivery
Pooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home DeliveryPooja Nehwal
 
👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...
👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...
👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...Sheetaleventcompany
 
WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)
WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)
WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)Delhi Call girls
 
Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...
Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...
Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...Sheetaleventcompany
 
Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...Nitya salvi
 
Hi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura Bhopalpura
Hi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura BhopalpuraHi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura Bhopalpura
Hi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura BhopalpuraApsara Of India
 
Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...
Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...
Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...Damini Dixit
 
Call Girls in Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call Girls
Call Girls in  Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call GirlsCall Girls in  Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call Girls
Call Girls in Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call GirlsPinki Misra
 
👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...
👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...
👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...Sheetaleventcompany
 
Fun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort Services
Fun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort ServicesFun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort Services
Fun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort ServicesApsara Of India
 
Rudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment Booking
Rudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment BookingRudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment Booking
Rudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment BookingNitya salvi
 
"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx
"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx
"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptxsadiisadiimano
 
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...rajveerescorts2022
 

Dernier (20)

Hire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls AgencyHire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
Hire 💕 8617697112 Pulwama Call Girls Service Call Girls Agency
 
Call girls in Vashi Services : 9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your Doorstep
Call girls in Vashi Services :  9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your DoorstepCall girls in Vashi Services :  9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your Doorstep
Call girls in Vashi Services : 9167673311 Free Delivery 24x7 at Your Doorstep
 
Top 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon Biotech
Top 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon BiotechTop 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon Biotech
Top 10 Moisturising Cream Brands In India - Stelon Biotech
 
UDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICE
UDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICEUDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICE
UDAIPUR CALL GIRLS 96O287O969 CALL GIRL IN UDAIPUR ESCORT SERVICE
 
💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞
💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞
💞ROYAL💞 UDAIPUR ESCORTS Call 09602870969 CaLL GiRLS in UdAiPuR EsCoRt SeRvIcE💞
 
💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service
💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service
💞Sexy Call Girls In Ambala 08168329307 Shahabad Call Girls Escort Service
 
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 📞Book Now📞👉 9878799926 👉Zirakpur Call Girl Service No ...
 
Pooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home Delivery
Pooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home DeliveryPooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home Delivery
Pooja : 9892124323, Dharavi Call Girls. 7000 Cash Payment Free Home Delivery
 
👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...
👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...
👉Amritsar Call Girl 👉📞 8725944379 👉📞 Just📲 Call Mack Call Girls Service In Am...
 
WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)
WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)
WhatsApp 📞 8448380779 ✅Call Girls In Bhangel Sector 102 ( Noida)
 
Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...
Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...
Chandigarh Escort Service 📞9878281761📞 Just📲 Call Navi Chandigarh Call Girls ...
 
Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...
Night 7k to 12k Chennai Call Girls 👉👉 8617697112⭐⭐ 100% Genuine Escort Servic...
 
Hi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura Bhopalpura
Hi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura BhopalpuraHi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura Bhopalpura
Hi Profile Escorts In Udaipur 09602870969 Call Girls in Sobaghpura Bhopalpura
 
Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...
Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...
Russian CalDeed Circle Call Girls Service ☎ ️93326-06886 ❤️‍🔥 Enjoy 24/7 Esco...
 
Call Girls in Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call Girls
Call Girls in  Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call GirlsCall Girls in  Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call Girls
Call Girls in Bangalore Prachi 💋9136956627 Bangalore Call Girls
 
👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...
👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...
👉Amritsar Escorts📞Book Now📞👉 8725944379 👉Amritsar Escort Service No Advance C...
 
Fun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort Services
Fun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort ServicesFun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort Services
Fun Call Girls In Yamunanagar 08168329307 Jagadhri Escort Services
 
Rudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment Booking
Rudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment BookingRudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment Booking
Rudraprayag call girls 📞 8617697112 At Low Cost Cash Payment Booking
 
"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx
"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx
"Maximizing your savings:The power of financial planning".pptx
 
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...
👉Chandigarh Call Girls 👉9878799926👉Just Call👉Chandigarh Call Girl In Chandiga...
 

Paper 2

  • 1. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 1 Components of Infidelity: Who Is Most Likely to Cheat in Romance? Kelly Burke Saint Louis University
  • 2. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 2 Another field in which the tendency towards infidelity can be analyzed is the “social and cultural domain,” which states that “personality affects, and is affected by, the social and cultural context in which it is found” (Larsen and Buss 2014). A study done by Pham et al. examines “coalitional mate retention,” or a process in which “individuals ask an ally to assist with thwarting their romantic partner’s infidelity” (Pham et al. 2015). Because coalitional mate retention involves the influence of the community and a tendency towards gendered norms, it would fall under the category of social and cultural domain. In this study, Pham et al. aimed to chart the correlation between the frequency in which participants “requested or received coalitional mate retention” from both a male friend and a female friend, and the perceived quality of each friendship (2015). The central question examined whether the amount of coalitional mate retention requests correlated negatively or positively to the perceived quality of the friendships, not accounting for the success of the actual retention process (Pham et al. 2015). Examination of this correlation aims to determine whether coalitional mate retention is a beneficial practice to the quality of an individual’s friendships. If it builds firmer bonds within a society, it may be a behavior worth encouraging, as it has “solved sex-specific adaptive problems” over the course of evolution; if it weakens friendships, however, then it should be discouraged, as coalitional mate retention has been shown to correlate positively with instances of allies becoming mate-poachers or otherwise sabotaging the reputation of a former ally (Pham et al. 2015). Pham et al. predicted that coalitional mate retention would correlate positively to a high quality of friendship in relationships involving women: female-female, female-male, and male- female (2015). The researchers formulated this hypothesis based on past studies showing that women consider the sharing of personal information to be vital in the establishment and
  • 3. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 3 maintenance of platonic friendship, as such bonds are formed on trust and mutual confidence. Coalitional mate retention fundamentally requires that the allies are open to each other about themselves and their relationships, indicating that the sharing of personal information must take place (2015). Coalitional mate retention was predicted to correlate negatively to male-male friendships, as men do not usually share personal information due to a fear of reputational damage, and are thus much less likely to open up about themselves without the encouragement of a woman. Men are also less likely to request coalitional mate retention because a majority of them consider it a sign of weakness or dependence running counter to their masculine ideal; it is analogous to men’s refusal of health care under the same premises (Pham et al. 2015). Also, men are more likely to become jealous of their male ally, especially because the “monopolizing” of the observed partner’s time has the potential to develop into mate-poaching. To observe the correlation between high quality friendships and coalitional mate retention, Pham et al. administered the McGill Friendship questionnaire to 387 participants. The questionnaire has six dimensions: Help, Intimacy, Reliable Alliance, Stimulating Companionship, Self-Validation, and Emotional Security. The first three dimensions were focused most closely, as current research supports that they have the greatest overall correlation to coalitional mate retention (Pham et al. 2015). Of the 387 participants, 176 were women, and all of the participants were involved in committed, heterosexual relationships which lasted at least one year. Each participant reported on two of their heterosexual friends, one male and one female, whom they had described as being close friends whom they had known for at least a year. On a scale from 0-8, the participants would report “how often their friend fulfilled friendship roles,” with 0 representative of never and 8 representative of always (Pham et al. 2015). Participants then completed the Coalitional Mate Retention Inventory for each friend; it
  • 4. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 4 was structured on a 0-3 scale and consisted of 44 items. The questions determined 1) how often the participant requested the friend to partake in coalitional mate retention practices within the past year and 2) how often the participant believed these friends actually fulfilled the request (Pham et al. 2015). To determine the results, Pham et al. calculated “friendship-quality variables from the mean of responses to items” which fell into the Help, Intimacy, and Reliable Alliance categories. The researchers then used the Coalitional Mate Inventory to calculate seven “coalitional mate retention tactic variables” which were the “mean of responses to items comprising each tactic” used while cooperating with an ally. These domains were: Manipulation, Praise, Vigilance, Therapy, Gifts, Monopolizing Time, and Violence (Pham et al. 2015). The data was then placed through two binomial tests, which examined all four friendship conditions: female-female, female-male, male-female, and male-male. The first of the two tests “assessed the proportion of positive correlations out of the 48 total correlations” while the second test “assessed the proportion of statistically significant positive correlations out of the total number of statistically significant correlations” (Pham et al. 2015). Male-female friendships correlated positively to the use of coalitional mate retention, with 41 of the 48 possible correlations being positive and 19 of the 19 statistically significant correlations being positive. Female-male friendships also correlated positively to the use of coalitional mate retention, with 46 of the 48 possible correlations being positive and 31 of the 31 statistically significant correlations being positive. Female-female friendship correlated positively to the use of coalitional mate retention, with 45 of the 48 possible correlations being positive and 21 of the 21 statistically significant correlations being positive. These statistics supported the hypothesis of relationships involving women being more likely to involve the use
  • 5. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 5 of coalitional mate retention, as women are the impetus behind such behavior (Pham et al. 2015). Male-male friendships, however, correlated negatively with the use of coalitional mate retention, with 33 of the 48 correlations being negative, and 13 of the 16 statistically significant correlations being negative as well. These findings are also consistent with the hypothesis, as male-male friendships correlate negatively with the sharing of personal information; the lack of women in the relationship prompts men to guard their dignity, even around male friends with whom they have a high-quality relationship. Men are also more concerned about the possibility of mate poaching, which correlates positively with coalitional mate retention (Pham et al. 2015). Another factor to potentially consider is the difference between the ways which men and women interpret close opposite-sex relationships. Men, for example, “perceive an intimate opposite-sex friendship as a mate poaching opportunity” more so than women do, which might be a relevant factor as to why men are typically less willing to engage in coalitional mate retention (Pham et al. 2015). There are a few problems with the study, however. For example, gender differences may be inadequately accounted for in terms of how the participants actually spend time with their same-sex friends, a flaw which the study itself admits. Men may spend more time with their close same-sex friends than females do, which would leave less time for the men to actually engage in coalitional mate retention. The study also admits that it suffers from the classical correlational study loophole: does a precede b, or does b precede a (Pham et al. 2015)? Another potential pitfall of the study is that the sample size only consisted of heterosexual participants. Homosexual participants might consider their relationships in different ways than which heterosexual participants might; for example, a gay man might feel more comfortable being more open with information than a straight man because he might not feel constrained by a
  • 6. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 6 heteronormative masculine ideal. Homosexuals could be incorporated into the heterosexual sample size, or the same experiment could be performed again on a homosexual population to determine how societal gender roles influence concept of friendship and coalitional mate retention. This study was valuable for defining the ways in which men and women classify the closeness of their friendships and how this, in turn, correlates with their sexual tendencies. It was not surprising to learn that women are more likely to engage in coalitional mate retention when reflecting upon gender norms. These norms typically expect women to behave in a more community-oriented manner, which extends to their sexual practices. The male-male results also aligned with gender norms, as men are expected to be more individualistic and capable. This ideal masculinity ties into the tendency for males to behave more unfaithfully than females, as they view friendships as a gateway to intimacy in many cases. Thus, this study can be said to show that men are more likely than women to commit infidelity, and are less likely to help others avoid unfaithful behaviors due to the advantage they may gain from a potential mate-poaching. Infidelity can also be studied within the adjustment domain, which states that “personality plays a key role in how we cope, adapt, and adjust to the ebb and flow of events in our day-to-day lives” (Larsen and Buss 2014). It typically relates to one’s physical and mental health when faced with a variety of stressors, and is thus often invoked in cases of mental illness. A 2015 study done by Ein-Dor et al. falls under the adjustment domain, as it focuses on how men and women cope with threats of mate-poaching. The study was generated to test the “rivalry centered hypothesis,” which posits that women are generally “more sensitive to cues of infidelity than men are,” tending to “focus their attention on potential rivals in their mate's vicinity.” Men, on the other hand, are more likely to monitor their own partner for signs of infidelity and focus
  • 7. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 7 less on potential rivals, a behavior which has recently been postulated to be unique to their sex. This implies that although men and women generally adopt different strategies for dealing with mate-poaching, they have the same ultimate goal: mate retention (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). By monitoring their partner’s behavior, men and women can attempt to prevent a “defection” and subsequent break in the relationship. These behaviors have evolved because infidelity, which occurs at fairly high rates within the population (about 22–25% of men and 11–15% of women), is typically very destructive mentally, physically, emotionally, and financially (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). In the rivalry-centered hypothesis, women were “expected to show overall higher infidelity-detection sensitivity than men” because their coping strategy involves monitoring multiple persons, whereas men’s coping strategy only focuses the attention on their mate. These coping strategies are believed to be due, in part, to evolutionary mechanisms, as men can physically coerce their partner into obeying them, whereas women are typically not strong enough to use this same tactic against their male partners. Thus, they must adopt a preventative strategy aimed at nixing infidelity before it can occur, or to prevent an existing extra- monogamous relationship from progressing any further (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). To test the rivalry-centered hypothesis, the researchers set up four different smaller studies, using the combined results to test their theory. The first study “examined whether men and women differ in the extent to which they appraise ambiguous partner-related incidents as comprising a threat of infidelity.” 194 participants (80 men and 114 women) “completed self- report measures of partner distrust, history of infidelity, and socio-economic status” and then were asked whether or not they believed if a “partner-related event” indicated a distinct threat towards infidelity; the questions were set up in a yes-no format. Women were hypothesized to
  • 8. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 8 judge more events as a threat towards infidelity than men, as past studies have shown that women are more likely than men to mistrust their romantic partners (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). Participants first completed a 5-item self-report measuring “the extent to which they tend to mistrust romantic partners.” The self-report was set up with a scale from 1-7 with one being “not at all” and 7 being “very much.” After the questionnaire was completed, participants were instructed to read 11 vignettes depicting “ambiguous signs of partner unfaithfulness” and were then asked whether or not the situation constituted infidelity. The researchers totaled the amount of vignettes that were affirmed to be portraying infidelity for each participant. Once all the vignettes had been categorized as situations of infidelity or not infidelity, the participants were asked “Did one of your past or present romantic partners ever commit an act of sexual infidelity?” and “Were you ever involved in a romantic affair while in a committed relationship?” (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). More women than men reported that their partner had cheated on them, which correlates to the higher rate of women perceiving infidelity; a person who has been cheated on is more likely to be more aware of the same cues in a different setting. More women also considered the vignettes to be depicting sexual infidelity than men, affirming the initial hypothesis that men are more likely than women to interpret ambiguous situations as infidelity. Study 2 was designed to test whether women were more accurate at depicting infidelity than men, or whether their suspicions were actually false alarms. The sample size of Study 2 was 118 heterosexual adults (44 men and 74 women); about twenty-five percent reported that a current or past partner had been unfaithful to them, while about thirty-eight percent admitted that they were “involved in a sexual affair” (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). Participants in this study were asked to identify a picture representing infidelity as quickly as possible from “a matrix of control pictures.” After the
  • 9. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 9 picture was correctly identified by being clicked on by the participant, the matrix would change and they would have to locate a different infidelity picture. The timing ranged from 2-22 seconds to identify the picture, while the number of attempts to select the correct picture ranged from 1-17. On average, women detected the infidelity pictures more quickly than men and made fewer errors. These findings affirmed the hypothesis that women detect romantic threat more quickly and with greater accuracy then men. To affirm that women were indeed more skilled at detecting romantic threat and not threat in general, Study 3 was implemented. The sample population of Study 3 consisted of 93 heterosexual participants (47 men and 46 women). The participants first completed the test described in Study 2 and then completed an additional test, which asked them to identify the picture on a matrix which represented danger from a poisonous animal. Once again, women scored lower times and fewer errors than men on the infidelity identification, but both women and men scored about equally on the poisonous animal detection test. This confirms the hypothesis that women detect infidelity quicker and more accurately than men, and that the skill is situation-specific, not applying to all instances of danger (Ein-Dor et al. 2015). Study 4 was designed to test the final part of the hypothesis, namely, the theory that women focus more closely on potential romantic rivals than men do. Participants were first asked to complete a self-report measuring “partner distrust, history of infidelity, and socio- economic status,” and were then asked to complete a computerized classification task, which measured the participants’ “perception of within-couple threats and threats that reside outside of the relationship.” The sample size consisted of 127 heterosexual participants (60 men and 67 women). Ten percent of the sample reported an unfaithful partner while thirteen percent reported that they
  • 10. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 10 were involved or had been involved in a romantic affair. The participants first completed the spousal distrust questionnaire, and were then tasked to complete another visual test. In this variation, a couple was seated at a bar, and an attractive man or woman was seated close by. The first phase of the test examined the detection of within-couple infidelity, and the series of pictures portrayed one member of the couple looking at his or her significant other, looking at an indeterminate point in space, or looking passionately at the attractive stranger. Participants were asked to evaluate the possibility that the partner would stray based on his or her level of eye contact. In the second phase, the participants were asked to evaluate the threat of infidelity from outside the relationship. Physically attractive women and dominant males were used as the outside threat in this picture series, as these are traits which each gender finds most threatening to mate retention. Participants were asked to indicate whether the desirable person was looking at one member of the couple and whether or not this presented a threat to mate retention. In the first phase, pictures which depicted ambiguous gaze direction evoked responses that “men were significantly more likely than women to state that the target's partner was looking at the attractive stranger,” presenting a within-relationship threat to mate retention. This affirms the hypothesis that men are more likely to guard their own mate than to focus on external threats. In the second phase, “women were significantly more likely than men to state that the attractive stranger was looking at the target's partner,” except in the non-ambiguous situations. Study 4 affirms the initial hypothesis positing that women are more likely to be concerned with same-sex rivals outside of the relationship, whereas men are more likely to keep watch over their own partner. While the studies successfully supported the hypothesis, there were a few problems with the studies. As the researchers admit, the studies solely examined the participants’ ability to
  • 11. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 11 detect infidelity within other people’s relationships, not within their own. Detecting infidelity without the benefit of being an outsider is considerably more difficult, and future studies should address this disparity when accounting for overall detection ability. Also, in Studies 1 and 2, the sample size of women significantly outnumbered the sample size of men, which could skew the results in favor of women’s detection abilities. Lastly, the four components of the larger hypothesis utilized different persons for the sample population. Thus it lacks within-subject testing although all the studies were essential to test the same hypothesis. This article confirms that it is much easier for a man to cheat than for a woman to cheat. While men only have one partner to observe, women have to watch multiple targets, so the chance of them missing a cue is greater even though they are overall more able to detect infidelity. Because men have a greater ability to successfully engage in infidelity, they are more likely to actually do so. Also, it confirms that although men and women face the threat of infidelity in different ways and in different frequencies, both are equally affected emotionally and mentally. Thus both strive to avoid infidelity and retain their partner despite living in a culture which normalizes extramarital relationships to a certain extent. It will be interesting to see whether perceptions about infidelity change if the rates continue to grow over time. Taking all four studies into account, one can conclude that unmarried men with high levels of social confidence, who have also been unfaithful in the past, are more likely to commit infidelity than women. The first study examined infidelity intentions, which posited that persons with a positive outlook on infidelity are most likely to actually carry through with unfaithful behavior, as beliefs are thought to correlate closely with realized actions (Jackman 2014). Persons who view themselves as more in control of their behavior and their social situation are also more likely to engage in infidelity. Men fall into this category more often than women do,
  • 12. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 12 as gender norms encourage the former to be more assertive and autonomous. It is also easier to carry out infidelity if one is unmarried, as there is not a spouse that one must constantly circumvent; past studies have also shown, on average, that married persons have a more negative opinion on infidelity and are thus less likely to engage in the behavior. Biological factors also correlate heavily with infidelity. It is more evolutionarily advantageous for a man to engage in infidelity than a woman, as he is able to spread his genes to multiple partners at one time. Women, however, can only carry the offspring of one partner at a time and are tasked with nursing and other forms of caregiving, so it is more advantageous for them to have a partner who will steadily provide them with resources. Therefore, women are typically less likely to cheat due to the benefits their partners can bring them. Infidelity in women was typically an adaptive behavior to keep the DNA pool diverse when humans lived in small units. Today this is not an issue, as genetic relatives live further apart and potential mates are geographically much closer, which correlates to decreased rates of women engaging in infidelity, as the adaptation has mostly become obsolete. Thus, women are more likely to cheat when there is an obvious advantage to the behavior but otherwise tend to avoid it under a majority of circumstances. Women with more active Vasopressin receptors, however, were found to exhibit higher percentages of cheating behavior, but it is currently unknown if the receptor influences the behavior or vice versa (Zietsch et al. 2014). Men with close oppose-sex friends are more likely to engage in infidelity as well. As men typically consider this friendship as a gateway into a relationship, they are more willing to make romantic advances than women who are in a close friendship with men. This infidelity often comes in the form of mate-poaching, which could theoretically lead to double counts of infidelity if both man and poached female mate are already in relationships (Pham et al. 2015).
  • 13. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 13 Persons who have been the victims of infidelity, a majority of them women, are less likely to engage in infidelity themselves. They are also better suited to recognize infidelity and will thus adjust their behavior to avoid repeating the unfavorable scenario. Women are also more suited to recognize infidelity because the odds of a woman being a victim of infidelity are greater than of a man being a victim of infidelity, even if the woman has not been victimized before. Lastly, women are less likely to partake in infidelity because men are more likely to guard their mate. With focused attention directed towards them, women have less means to engage in infidelity without being discovered (Ein-Dor et al. 2015)
  • 14. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 14 References Ein-Dor, Tsachi., Perry-Paldi, Adi., Hirschberger, Gilad., Birnbaum, Gurit E., and Deutsch, Danit. (January 2015). Coping with mate poaching: gender differences in detection of infidelity-related threats. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(1), 17-24. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com/eds/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=6d064af6-a56c-44e9-9d77- a270da2805b9%40sessionmgr4004&hid=4111&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d #db=edselp&AN=S1090513814001019. AB: People often aspire for true love and committed romantic relationships. These relationships, however, are recurrently threatened by partner infidelity. The present research tested a new infidelity-detection model, the rivalry sensitivity hypothesis, that posits that women are more sensitive to cues of infidelity than men are, and tend to focus their attention on potential rivals in their mate's vicinity, whereas men show increased sensitivity of their own partners. In a series of four studies, we found that women displayed greater alertness to cues of potential partner unfaithfulness than did men, were quicker and more accurate in detecting cues of infidelity, but were not better than men in detecting other threats. Women also focused their attention on potential rivals (other women), whereas men's attention was specifically directed at monitoring their own partner's intents. These findings suggest that women and men have developed different strategies aimed at achieving a similar outcome – mate retention. Jackman, M. (2014). Understanding the Cheating Heart: What Determines Infidelity Intentions? Sexuality & Culture, 19(1), 72-84. Retrieved from http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com/sp-
  • 15. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 15 3.15.1b/ovidweb.cgi?&S=PBGAFPMHPFDDPOHONCKKBEDCMDJMAA00&Comple te+Reference=S.sh.21%7c4%7c1. Larsen, L.J. and Buss, D.M. (2014). Personality Psychology: Domains of Knowledge About Human Nature (5th edition). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education. Pham, M.N., Mogilski, J.K., Shackelford, T.K., and Barbaro, N. (2015) Coalitional mate retention is correlated positively with friendship quality involving women, but negatively with male–male friendship quality. Personality and Individual Differences, 79, 87-90. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com/eds/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=18d25fef-5d90- 45f5-958c- c7f421bdb097%40sessionmgr115&hid=119&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d#d b=edselc&AN=edselc.2-52.0-84922998553. AB: Friendship solved adaptive problems over human evolutionary history, including cooperative hunting and alloparenting. Pham, Barbaro, and Shackelford (in press) investigated another potential function of friendship: the provision of coalitional mate retention, whereby individuals ask an ally to assist with thwarting their romantic partner’s infidelity. In the current research, 387 participants (176 women) reported how often they requested or received coalitional mate retention from a male friend and from a female friend and reported on the quality of each friendship. The results indicate that the deployment of coalitional mate retention is positively associated with the quality of friendships with women (female–female friendships, male–female friendships, female– male friendships), but negatively associated with the quality of male–male friendships.
  • 16. COMPONENTS OF INFIDELITY 16 Zietsch, B.P., Jern, P., Westberg, L., and Santtila, P. (2014). Genetic analysis of human extrapair mating: Heritability, between-sex correlation, andreceptor genes for vasopressin and oxytocin. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(2), 130-136. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com/eds/detail/detail?vid=1&sid=54860916-c006-43b9-9aa4- 90503bb6c5b6%40sessionmgr111&hid=126&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d# db=edselc&AN=edselc.2-52.0-84922930492.