SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  11
Télécharger pour lire hors ligne
DRAFT



                                            Foresight – SR-D1 v1 stage 2
                                         Mental Capital and Mental Wellbeing

                                             Office of Science and Innovation




                                                    Version date: 27th April 2007




                       State-of-Science Review:
                     Specific Language Impairment




                    Professor Dorothy Bishop
              Department of Experimental Psychology,
                       University of Oxford




This review has been commissioned as part of the UK Government’s Foresight
project: Mental Capital and Mental Wellbeing. The views expressed do not represent
the policy of any Government or organisation.




                                       Page 1
Specific Language Impairment

Abstract

   What is Specific Language Impairment (SLI)? Brief account of criteria
    used
   What causes SLI? Evidence for strong genetic component. Popular
    belief that poor language input from parents causes SLI is not
    supported by research evidence.
   How common is SLI? Will depend on cut-offs used. Clinically significant
    problems decline markedly with age
   International comparisons. Difficult because of lack of comparable
    language measures, and differences in organisation of services.
   Educational impact of SLI. High risk of poor literacy and educational
    failure in those whose SLI persists to 5 years or beyond.
   Impact of SLI on the individual and society. High rates of
    unemployment, social isolation and psychiatric disorder.
   Can we predict outcome? Age, severity and level of comprehension as
    important predictors.
   Can we intervene effectively? Woeful lack of good evidence, especially
    for older children. Many have persisting difficulties into adulthood
    despite intensive intervention. Computerised intervention worth
    exploring but has not so far fulfilled early promise.
   Future trends. Prevalence likely to be stable. Emphasis on early years
    intervention only cost-effective if we identify those whose problems are
    likely to persist. Need for controlled trials of intervention. Need for
    proactive approach to identifying niches in society for people who lack
    language skills.



What is Specific Language Impairment (SLI)?
   SLI is diagnosed when a child’s language fails to develop along normal
    lines for no obvious reason: hearing loss, physical disability, emotional
    disturbance, parental neglect and brain injury are all ruled out before
    the diagnosis is made. The child is developing normally in areas other
    than language, and will show adequate intelligence if tested using
    nonverbal measures (e.g. construction puzzles; reasoning tasks with
    shapes).
   Most people have heard of dyslexia and autism, but not SLI. This
    illustrates the power of labels: SLI is far more common than autism,
    and has close commonalities with dyslexia, but it does not have a
    catchy medical-sounding label, so it is neglected.
   The manifestations of SLI are variable both in terms of severity and
    quality: in some children, the main difficulties are a delay in starting to
    talk, with immature language persisting into school age. For other
    children, problems in understanding language predominate: they may
    take in only one or two words of a complex sentence such as “if you
    bring your swimsuit tomorrow, we can go to the pool after lunch.”



                                    Page 2
Vocabulary is usually limited in SLI, and children may have problems
    understanding unusual or abstract terms.
   It is likely that the term SLI is an umbrella term for a range of distinct
    disorders, but as yet there is no agreement about subtypes.
   A brief film featuring two young people with speech and language
    problems, Stephen and Graham, can be found on
    http://www.ican.org.uk/


What causes SLI?
   30 years ago, it was often assumed that SLI was the consequence of
    poor parenting. This view was reinforced as recently as 2003 in a
    speech by Alan Wells, Director of the Basic Skills Agency, who
    maintained that many families do not converse with their children, and
    use instead a "daily grunt”
    (http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/education/2638889.stm). In fact, language
    development in children appears remarkably resilient in the face of
    limited language input: perhaps the clearest evidence comes from
    normally-hearing children who are raised by parents who are
    profoundly deaf and unable to converse intelligibly. In most cases,
    these children learn to talk normally, provided they are exposed to a
    small amount of normal language (Schiff-Myers, 1988).
   Although other factors, such as chronic middle ear disease (which
    causes a mild and fluctuating hearing loss) may play a role in SLI, it is
    now generally accepted that this condition is not usually associated
    with hearing difficulties (Roberts et al., 2004), and is a strongly genetic
    disorder (Bishop, 2006). The best evidence comes from studies of
    twins. Two twins growing up together are exposed to the same home
    environment, yet may differ radically in their language skills. Such
    different outcomes are, however, seen almost exclusively in fraternal
    (non-identical) twins, who are genetically different. Identical twins share
    the same genes and tend to be much more similar in language ability.
    There can be some variation in the severity and persistence of SLI in
    identical twins, indicating that environmental factors affect the course of
    disorder, but it is unusual to find a child with SLI who has an identical
    twin with normal language.
   Although twin studies indicate genes are important, SLI is not usually
    caused by a mutation in a single gene. Current evidence suggests that
    there are many different genes that can influence language learning,
    and SLI results when a child inherits a particularly detrimental
    combination of risk factors (Bishop, 2006).
   Parents of children with SLI have similar genetic makeup and may well
    have limited language skills themselves. We need to beware of
    assuming that a parent with poor language has caused their child’s
    problems because of lack of stimulation.

How common is SLI?
   There is no sharp dividing line between SLI and normality: children vary
    in their language abilities, and the frequency of SLI will therefore




                                    Page 3
depend on how stringently it is defined. An epidemiological study in the
    US identified around 7.4% of 5-year-olds as having SLI (Tomblin et al.,
    1997), based on an initial screening test followed by a more detailed
    language assessment, but they noted that this figure could rise or fall
    depending on the cutoffs used to define impairment.
   An alternative way of estimating the scale of the problem is to consider
    the proportion of children referred for speech and language therapy:
    however, this does not give a satisfactory prevalence estimate,
    because it will depend on resources. Furthermore, problems with
    speech production and expressive language are more readily identified
    than poor language comprehension, but the latter has a worse
    prognosis (Zhang & Tomblin, 2000). In the study by Tomblin et al.
    (1997), in only 29% of cases had parents been informed previously that
    the child had speech and language problems.
   The media occasionally run stories about an “epidemic” of language
    impairment in children starting school, sometimes attributing it to
    working parents, TV, and so on. There is no hard evidence of any
    secular change in prevalence of language difficulties. It is likely that
    perception of problems is inflated by the fact that more children are
    starting school at 4 years of age, and our large cities now include
    unprecedented numbers of children who do not have English as a first
    language. It would be well worth instituting an epidemiological study to
    monitor rates of language impairment (and related disorders), every 10
    years or so, using standard methods of assessment, to estimate the
    true scale of the problem, and identify any secular trends.

International comparisons
   It is not possible to compare SLI in the UK with other countries. Part of
    the problem is that language assessments used to identify SLI may not
    work in translation, and can only be interpreted if we have adequate
    norms (i.e., data on a large representative population of children, that
    allow us to quantify typical performance at different ages). In many
    countries such data do not exist. Furthermore, even in English-
    speaking countries, there are difficulties in making international
    comparisons, because tests may be culturally specific.
   There is a fascinating research literature comparing manifestations of
    SLI in different languages, but this is primarily of interest to those
    working on theoretical conceptualisations of the disorder (Leonard,
    2000).

Educational impact of SLI
   Numerous studies have demonstrated that children with SLI are at high
    risk of educational failure. Low levels of literacy are common, even in
    children who receive specialist help, and educational attainments are
    typically poor (e.g. Catts et al., 2002; Snowling et al., 2001). This bleak
    picture is especially applicable to children who have language
    impairments that persist beyond the age of 5 years, and where
    comprehension as well as expressive language is affected. For
    instance, Simkin and Conti-Ramsden (2006) found that 67% of children




                                    Page 4
with expressive SLI and 88% of those with receptive SLI had serious
    reading difficulties at 11 years of age.

Impact of SLI on the individual and society
   SLI is associated with a high rate of psychiatric disorder (see review by
    Cohen, 2001). For instance, Conti-Ramsden and Botting (2004) found
    that 64% of a sample of 11-year-olds with SLI scored above a clinical
    threshold on a questionnaire for psychiatric difficulties, and 36% were
    regularly bullied, compared with 12% of comparison children. The
    greatest risk appears to be for attentional problems and social
    difficulties, rather than conduct or emotional disorders. Looked at from
    the other direction, a study of children attending psychiatric clinics
    found that 28% had hitherto undetected language difficulties that were
    moderate or severe (Cohen et al., 1998). The reason for this
    association is not fully understood. An obvious mechanism is via stress
    induced by social rejection and isolation, both of which are common in
    children with language difficulties. In addition, a child with poor
    language skills may fail to use internalised language as a method of
    self-regulation, e.g. talking oneself through past events or future plans,
    or instructing oneself to behave in a particular way. It is also possible
    that the same genes that influence language learning lead directly to
    psychiatric vulnerability.
   There are a few studies of the long-term outcome of children with SLI,
    and they mostly make depressing reading. There are elevated rates of
    unemployment, social isolation and psychiatric disorder (Clegg et al.,
    2005). However, most studies focused on outcomes of those with
    severe problems, where comprehension as well as expressive
    language is affected. Better outcomes were seen for children who had
    milder difficulties and did not require special educational provision
    (Snowling, et al., 2006). Where language difficulties had resolved by 5
    years of age, there was no excess psychopathology.

Can we predict outcome?
   Longitudinal studies of young children with SLI generally agree that
    long-term outcome is worse in those who have more severe problems
    (e.g. Stothard et al.,1998). Severity can be measured both in terms of
    the level of performance on a given language measure, and the range
    of language functions that are affected.
   Age is also critical. Understandably, there is much interest in identifying
    children as young as possible, on the grounds that early intervention is
    likely to be more effective than late. However, the situation is
    complicated by the fact that many children make good spontaneous
    progress after a late start in language. There have been several studies
    of ‘late-talkers’, identified at age 18-24 months because they are
    producing very few words. Most such children appear to be late
    bloomers, in that their outcome is similar to that of other children when
    followed up into middle childhood (Paul, 2000). A key factor appears to
    be comprehension level: those who have poor understanding of what
    others say are less likely to spontaneously catch up. However, the




                                    Page 5
evidence base for this claim is slender, as few studies have looked
    specifically at comprehension in young children.
   One can also see spontaneous recovery between 4 and 5 years of age,
    especially in children whose problems are predominantly with
    expressive language. One UK study studied children who had specific
    speech and language problems at 4 years of age; they were seen at
    4.5 yr, 5.5 yr, 8 yr and 15 yr. Around 40% had essentially normalised
    by 5.5 years, and their long-term outcome was generally good,
    although minor literacy problems were found in some (Stothard et al,
    1998). However, the 60% who still had measurable language problems
    at 5.5 years fared much worse, with the gap between them and other
    children widening as time went by. For the most severely affected
    children, it was not uncommon to see a drop in nonverbal intelligence
    in adolescence, as well as stagnation of language skills.

Can we intervene effectively?
   It would be a mistake to imagine that findings on genetics of SLI
    preclude the development of effective interventions. What the genetics
    studies tell us is that the kinds of variations in environment that most
    children experience do not play a salient role in determining who has a
    language problem. Thus, simply talking more to these children is
    unlikely to improve their language skills. But the genetic findings do not
    preclude the development of interventions tailored specifically to the
    child’s problems that could be effective. By analogy, we know that
    diabetes has a genetic basis, but we do not conclude the condition is
    untreatable: rather, we devise treatments that are outside the normal
    range of experience to tackle the underlying cause.
   Children with SLI will normally receive therapy from a Speech and
    Language Therapist (SALT). In more severe cases, the child may
    receive special educational provision, either in a specialist class in a
    mainstream school, or in a special school.
   Unfortunately, there has been little research on efficacy of interventions
    for SLI (Law et al, 2004). Such evidence as exists suggests
    intervention can be effective for improving intelligibility in children with
    speech impairments, but there is a lack of good research on
    interventions for language impairments, particularly with older children.
    In the UK, there are a number of special schools that have developed
    skills in working with older children with SLI over many years, but this
    knowledge is fragmented and not widely available, and there are no
    well-controlled studies of efficacy.
   One problem for intervention studies is that one is often looking for an
    effect of intervention that may be superimposed on spontaneous
    improvement. SALTs regard early intervention as important, and some
    local authorities focus all their resources on preschoolers, and have no
    provision for school-aged children. This strategy is no doubt reinforced
    by the fact that many preschoolers who receive therapy make excellent
    progress. The point that is often missed is that many of these children
    would have done well with no intervention. For example, in one
    controlled study of early intervention there was no difference between




                                    Page 6
treated and untreated children, not because nobody improved, but
    because everyone improved (Stevenson et al., 1982). Clearly we do
    not want to wait to intervene with those children who are at risk for
    long-term problems, but it is important to use research evidence on
    predictors to focus intervention those children who are unlikely to
    improve spontaneously.
   One relatively new advance has been development of computerised
    interventions for SLI. The best-known of these is FastForword® (FFW),
    which was developed out of research showing that children with SLI
    have problems in processing sounds that are brief or rapid (Tallal et al.,
    1996). The idea behind FFW is that the brain can be trained to
    perceive increasingly small differences between sounds. Children play
    computerised games that adjust the level of difficulty of discrimination
    to the child’s performance level. The games involve both language
    stimuli (e.g. child determines whether a spoken sentence matches a
    pictured scenario) and nonlinguistic sounds (e.g. glides that resemble
    animal sounds, and vary in speed of change). The originators of FFW
    have won scientific accolades for what appeared at first to be a
    researcher’s Holy Grail – an intervention developed out of basic
    neuroscience that would help children. Unfortunately, later evaluations
    have found that FFW has not fulfilled its early promise, and appears to
    do no better than conventional speech and language therapy (e.g.,
    Cohen et al., 2005). This is surprising, given that the intervention is so
    intensive, requiring the child to spend 90-100 minutes per day on the
    program for around 6 weeks. Although school boards in the US have
    bought into the program (in some cases for all pupils, not just those
    with SLI), there is growing scepticism as to whether this is justified,
    given the high financial cost of the program, and the time the child has
    to spend away from regular school activities (Troia & Whitney, 2003).
    An editorial in Nature Neuroscience (January 2004) expressed concern
    that this kind of educational program is being marketed without the
    evidence for efficacy that would be required for a drug treatment.
   A final point to stress about early intervention concerns the link
    between SLI and poor literacy. Children in UK schools are introduced
    to literacy at ever earlier ages. In this regard, the UK is very different
    from our European neighbours, where children are often not introduced
    to literacy until 6 or even 7 years of age; rather, they education focuses
    on language-based activities that build the skills needed for successful
    reading and writing. The available evidence suggests that this late start
    does not lead to literacy problems, though comparisons are
    complicated by the different orthographies of different languages
    (Lundberg & Linnakylä, 1993). Evidence from children with SLI
    suggests that those who fail in literacy are children who are exposed to
    literacy instruction when relevant linguistic skills are still
    underdeveloped (Bird et al., 1995). It is possible that children would do
    better if literacy instruction were delayed until they had been shown to
    have a critical level of linguistic skill.
   Ideally we need studies of efficacy that adopt the methods of
    randomised controlled trials as used in mainstream medicine. These
    are expensive and can run into difficulty because of the reluctance of



                                   Page 7
many SALTs to have an untreated control group. They argue this is
    unethical; however, it could equally be argued that that it is unethical to
    persist in using interventions of unproven efficacy, especially when
    resources are limited. Unfortunately, it is impossible to do a cost-
    effectiveness analysis of interventions for SLI because we lack
    information on efficacy, both for improving SLI and for preventing other
    adverse outcomes.

Future trends
   The prevalence of SLI is likely to remain stable over the years, but its
    impact could become more serious, as our society becomes
    increasingly dependent on literacy skills.
   Intervention currently focuses on the early years – from as early as 18
    months in some cases, and up to around 4 years of age. This focus,
    though understandable, may be non-optimal, because of high levels of
    spontaneous improvement. One way forward would be to combine
    early intervention with use of using predictors from test results to
    identify those whose problems are unlikely to resolve spontaneously.
   It would be worth conducting a controlled trial to consider whether
    children’s literacy skills can be enhanced by deferring literacy training
    until a minimal level of language competence has been attained.
   Computerised intervention has generated great excitement, because it
    has the potential to deliver thousands of events to a child in a game-
    like format, thereby maintaining motivation, and adapting on-line to the
    child’s responses. Although attempts to develop computerised
    programs for treating SLI have, as yet, not been shown to be effective,
    we have as yet barely explored the potential of this approach. More
    research is needed to identify the conditions under which children with
    SLI can learn most effectively.

   We should not underestimate the difficulty of remediating SLI: we may
    need to accept that some children will never be competent
    communicators. If so, then putting all our efforts into working to improve
    their language skills may be as futile as trying to train a blind child to
    see. For many children, education can be a dispiriting experience
    characterised by continual failure. We should continue to do research
    to identify barriers to language learning in these children so we can
    develop more effective interventions, but this should not be at the
    expense of other areas of development. It would be desirable to identify
    for a child with SLI a non-linguistic skill in which they could succeed,
    and to devote time on the curriculum to fostering this. These children
    could also benefit from prophylactic intervention designed to avoid the
    associated social problems: thus intervention with them would go
    beyond the narrow remit of improving language skills, to work also to
    enhance their ability to make friendships and find a role in the wider
    world. The modern world is increasingly hostile to those who lack skills
    in literacy, communication and computer processing: it may be
    necessary to take a more proactive approach to identifying niches for
    people who lack these skills.




                                    Page 8
   Conventional speech and language therapy has evolved in response to
       a clinical need, and is not an exact science. We need to ensure that
       clinical skills that are developed in specialist centres are preserved and
       disseminated. More use of video-links between specialist centres and
       mainstream schools could be helpful. Specific educational methods
       also need formal evaluation, so we can identify the most effective
       approaches.

References
Bird, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Freeman, N. (1995). Phonological awareness and
literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 446-462.

Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). What causes specific language impairment in
children? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 217-221.

Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal
investigation of reading outcomes in children with language
impairments. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45,
1142-1157.

Clegg, J., Hollis, C., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2005). Developmental
language disorders - a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language
and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 46, 128-149.

Cohen, N. J. (2001). Language impairment and psychopathology in infants,
children and adolescents. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Cohen, N. J., Barwick, M. A., Horodezky, N. B., Vallance, D. D., & Im, N.
(1998). Language, achievement, and cognitive processing in psychiatrically
disturbed children with previously identified and unsuspected language
impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 865-877.

Cohen, W., Hodson, A., O'Hare, A., Boyle, J., Durrani, T., McCartney, E.,
Mattey, M., Naftalin, L., & Watson, J. (2005). Effects of computer-based
intervention using acoustically modified speech (Fast ForWord-
Language) in receptive language impairment: Outcomes from a
randomised controlled trial. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing
Research, 48, 715-729.

Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (2004). Social difficulties and victimization in
children with SLI at 11 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing Research, 47, 145-161.

Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2004). The efficacy of treatment for
children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: a
meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47,
924-943.




                                      Page 9
Leonard, L. B. (2000). Specific language impairment across languages. In D.
V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and Language Impairments in
Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome. Hove, UK:
Psychology Press.

Lundberg, I., & Linnakylä, P. (1993). Teaching reading around the world. IEA
study of reading literacy. Technical report: International Association for the
Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

Paul, R. (2000). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language delay:
Ethical implications. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech
and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics,
Intervention and Outcome (pp. 195-209). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Roberts, J. E., Rosenfeld, R. M., & Zeisel, S. A. (2004). Otitis media and
speech and language: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Pediatrics, 113,
e238-248.

Rouse, C., & Krueger, A. (2004). Putting computerized instruction to the test:
a randomized evalutaion of a 'scientifically based' reading program.
Economics of Education Review, 23, 323-338.

Schiff-Myers, N. (1988). Hearing children of deaf parents. In D. Bishop & K.
Mogford (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances (pp. 47-
61). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.

Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Evidence of reading difficulty in
subgroups of children with specific language impairment. Child Language
Teaching and Therapy, 22, 315-331.

Snowling, M. J., Adams, J. W., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2001).
Educational attainments of school-leavers with a pre-school history of
speech-language impairments. International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders, 36, 173-183.

Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Stothard, S. E., Chipchase, B., &
Kaplan, C. (2006). Psychosocial outcomes at 15 years of children with a
preschool history of speech-language impairment. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 759-765.

Stevenson, P., Bax, M., & Stevenson, J. (1982). The evaluation of home
based speech therapy for language delayed pre-school children in an inner
city area. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 17, 141-148.

Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., &
Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into
adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41,
407-418.




                                      Page 10
Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Najarajan, S. S.,
Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. M., & Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Language
comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with
acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81-84.

Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., &
O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in
kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 40, 1245-1260.

Troia, G. A., & Whitney, S.D. (2003). A close look at the efficacy of Fast
ForWord Language for children with academic weaknesses. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 28, 465-494.

Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2000). The association of intervention receipt with
speech-language profiles and social-demographic variables. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 345-357.




                                       Page 11

Contenu connexe

Tendances

How is specific language impairment identified?
How is specific language impairment identified?How is specific language impairment identified?
How is specific language impairment identified?
RALLICampaign
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn language
Dorothy Bishop
 
How is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identifiedHow is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identified
Dorothy Bishop
 

Tendances (20)

Genetics and SLI
Genetics and SLIGenetics and SLI
Genetics and SLI
 
How is specific language impairment identified?
How is specific language impairment identified?How is specific language impairment identified?
How is specific language impairment identified?
 
SLI and the brain
SLI and the brainSLI and the brain
SLI and the brain
 
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
 
Identification of SLI: references
Identification of SLI: referencesIdentification of SLI: references
Identification of SLI: references
 
SLI and reading: 1. decoding
SLI and reading: 1. decodingSLI and reading: 1. decoding
SLI and reading: 1. decoding
 
Bilingual 2
Bilingual 2 Bilingual 2
Bilingual 2
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn language
 
How is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identifiedHow is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identified
 
How specific is SLI - slides
How specific is SLI - slidesHow specific is SLI - slides
How specific is SLI - slides
 
How common is SLI?
How common is SLI?How common is SLI?
How common is SLI?
 
Nyborg causes2
Nyborg causes2Nyborg causes2
Nyborg causes2
 
Language, sex chromosomes and autism: unravelling the mystery
Language, sex chromosomes and autism: unravelling the mysteryLanguage, sex chromosomes and autism: unravelling the mystery
Language, sex chromosomes and autism: unravelling the mystery
 
SLI and the brain
SLI and the brainSLI and the brain
SLI and the brain
 
What is Pragmatic Language Impairment?
What is Pragmatic Language Impairment?What is Pragmatic Language Impairment?
What is Pragmatic Language Impairment?
 
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
 
How common is SLI?
How common is SLI?How common is SLI?
How common is SLI?
 
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explainedDevelopmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
 
Language impairment Presentation
Language impairment PresentationLanguage impairment Presentation
Language impairment Presentation
 
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctionsAutism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
 

En vedette

11. de jesus lesch–nyhan syndrome
11. de jesus   lesch–nyhan syndrome11. de jesus   lesch–nyhan syndrome
11. de jesus lesch–nyhan syndrome
John Velo
 
MSUD disease short presentation
MSUD disease short presentationMSUD disease short presentation
MSUD disease short presentation
David Constanza
 

En vedette (14)

11. de jesus lesch–nyhan syndrome
11. de jesus   lesch–nyhan syndrome11. de jesus   lesch–nyhan syndrome
11. de jesus lesch–nyhan syndrome
 
Glutaricaciduria type 2 prerna
Glutaricaciduria type 2 prernaGlutaricaciduria type 2 prerna
Glutaricaciduria type 2 prerna
 
Auditory processing disorder (APD) and specific language impairment
Auditory processing disorder (APD) and specific language impairmentAuditory processing disorder (APD) and specific language impairment
Auditory processing disorder (APD) and specific language impairment
 
Genetic disorders
Genetic disordersGenetic disorders
Genetic disorders
 
04.04.11: Crystal Mediated Arthritidies
04.04.11: Crystal Mediated Arthritidies 04.04.11: Crystal Mediated Arthritidies
04.04.11: Crystal Mediated Arthritidies
 
MSUD disease short presentation
MSUD disease short presentationMSUD disease short presentation
MSUD disease short presentation
 
Maple Syrup Urine Disease
Maple Syrup Urine DiseaseMaple Syrup Urine Disease
Maple Syrup Urine Disease
 
Alcaptonuria
AlcaptonuriaAlcaptonuria
Alcaptonuria
 
Maple syrup urine disease by Jimcale Xamari
Maple syrup urine disease by Jimcale XamariMaple syrup urine disease by Jimcale Xamari
Maple syrup urine disease by Jimcale Xamari
 
Alkaptonuria
AlkaptonuriaAlkaptonuria
Alkaptonuria
 
Amino Acid Metabolism & Purine Disorders
Amino Acid Metabolism & Purine DisordersAmino Acid Metabolism & Purine Disorders
Amino Acid Metabolism & Purine Disorders
 
Alkaptonuria
AlkaptonuriaAlkaptonuria
Alkaptonuria
 
Phenylketonuria
PhenylketonuriaPhenylketonuria
Phenylketonuria
 
Metabolic disorders of proteins
Metabolic disorders of proteinsMetabolic disorders of proteins
Metabolic disorders of proteins
 

Similaire à State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment

ANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docx
ANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docxANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docx
ANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docx
justine1simpson78276
 
#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx
#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx
#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx
mayank272369
 
6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx
6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx
6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx
alinainglis
 
Special needs presentation
Special needs presentationSpecial needs presentation
Special needs presentation
journi
 
Communication Disorders
Communication DisordersCommunication Disorders
Communication Disorders
LindseyGay
 
10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx
10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx
10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx
hyacinthshackley2629
 

Similaire à State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment (20)

ANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docx
ANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docxANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docx
ANNOTATIONManagement of childhood stutteringMark Onslow .docx
 
Apa citation
Apa citationApa citation
Apa citation
 
Apa citation
Apa citationApa citation
Apa citation
 
SLI.pptx
SLI.pptxSLI.pptx
SLI.pptx
 
#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx
#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx
#775632 Topic Don Quixote6.1 What Are Speech and Language Imp.docx
 
What is Developmental Language Disorder
What is Developmental Language DisorderWhat is Developmental Language Disorder
What is Developmental Language Disorder
 
6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx
6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx
6.1 What Are Speech and Language ImpairmentsStudents receive se.docx
 
Journal.pone.0158753
Journal.pone.0158753Journal.pone.0158753
Journal.pone.0158753
 
Auditory Processing Disorder and Specific Language Impairment
Auditory Processing Disorder and Specific Language ImpairmentAuditory Processing Disorder and Specific Language Impairment
Auditory Processing Disorder and Specific Language Impairment
 
Special needs presentation
Special needs presentationSpecial needs presentation
Special needs presentation
 
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum ConditionsA Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
 
Speech development- Delay and other problems
Speech development- Delay and other problemsSpeech development- Delay and other problems
Speech development- Delay and other problems
 
Communication Disorders
Communication DisordersCommunication Disorders
Communication Disorders
 
Communication Disorders
Communication DisordersCommunication Disorders
Communication Disorders
 
Dyslexia basics fact_sheet_2017
Dyslexia basics fact_sheet_2017Dyslexia basics fact_sheet_2017
Dyslexia basics fact_sheet_2017
 
Outreach Sept 2018: Outi Tuomainen
Outreach Sept 2018: Outi Tuomainen Outreach Sept 2018: Outi Tuomainen
Outreach Sept 2018: Outi Tuomainen
 
Gifted Students With Dyslexia
Gifted Students With DyslexiaGifted Students With Dyslexia
Gifted Students With Dyslexia
 
10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx
10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx
10.6 Developmental Disabilities and EducationIn addition to gift.docx
 
dual language
dual language dual language
dual language
 
Late talking/ expressive language delay in pediatrics
Late talking/ expressive language delay in pediatricsLate talking/ expressive language delay in pediatrics
Late talking/ expressive language delay in pediatrics
 

Plus de Dorothy Bishop

Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
Dorothy Bishop
 

Plus de Dorothy Bishop (20)

Exercise/fish oil intervention for dyslexia
Exercise/fish oil intervention for dyslexiaExercise/fish oil intervention for dyslexia
Exercise/fish oil intervention for dyslexia
 
Open Research Practices in the Age of a Papermill Pandemic
Open Research Practices in the Age of a Papermill PandemicOpen Research Practices in the Age of a Papermill Pandemic
Open Research Practices in the Age of a Papermill Pandemic
 
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
 
Journal club summary: Open Science save lives
Journal club summary: Open Science save livesJournal club summary: Open Science save lives
Journal club summary: Open Science save lives
 
Short talk on 2 cognitive biases and reproducibility
Short talk on 2 cognitive biases and reproducibilityShort talk on 2 cognitive biases and reproducibility
Short talk on 2 cognitive biases and reproducibility
 
Otitis media with effusion: an illustration of ascertainment bias
Otitis media with effusion: an illustration of ascertainment biasOtitis media with effusion: an illustration of ascertainment bias
Otitis media with effusion: an illustration of ascertainment bias
 
Insights from psychology on lack of reproducibility
Insights from psychology on lack of reproducibilityInsights from psychology on lack of reproducibility
Insights from psychology on lack of reproducibility
 
What are metrics good for? Reflections on REF and TEF
What are metrics good for? Reflections on REF and TEFWhat are metrics good for? Reflections on REF and TEF
What are metrics good for? Reflections on REF and TEF
 
Biomarkers for psychological phenotypes?
Biomarkers for psychological phenotypes?Biomarkers for psychological phenotypes?
Biomarkers for psychological phenotypes?
 
Data simulation basics
Data simulation basicsData simulation basics
Data simulation basics
 
Simulating data to gain insights into power and p-hacking
Simulating data to gain insights intopower and p-hackingSimulating data to gain insights intopower and p-hacking
Simulating data to gain insights into power and p-hacking
 
Talk on reproducibility in EEG research
Talk on reproducibility in EEG researchTalk on reproducibility in EEG research
Talk on reproducibility in EEG research
 
Developmental language disorder and auditory processing disorder: 
Same or di...
Developmental language disorder and auditory processing disorder: 
Same or di...Developmental language disorder and auditory processing disorder: 
Same or di...
Developmental language disorder and auditory processing disorder: 
Same or di...
 
Fallibility in science: Responsible ways to handle mistakes
Fallibility in science: Responsible ways to handle mistakesFallibility in science: Responsible ways to handle mistakes
Fallibility in science: Responsible ways to handle mistakes
 
Improve your study with pre-registration
Improve your study with pre-registrationImprove your study with pre-registration
Improve your study with pre-registration
 
Introduction to simulating data to improve your research
Introduction to simulating data to improve your researchIntroduction to simulating data to improve your research
Introduction to simulating data to improve your research
 
Southampton: lecture on TEF
Southampton: lecture on TEFSouthampton: lecture on TEF
Southampton: lecture on TEF
 
Reading list: What’s wrong with our universities
Reading list: What’s wrong with our universitiesReading list: What’s wrong with our universities
Reading list: What’s wrong with our universities
 
IJLCD Winter Lecture 2016-7 : References
IJLCD Winter Lecture 2016-7 : ReferencesIJLCD Winter Lecture 2016-7 : References
IJLCD Winter Lecture 2016-7 : References
 
What's wrong with our Universities, and will the Teaching Excellence Framewor...
What's wrong with our Universities, and will the Teaching Excellence Framewor...What's wrong with our Universities, and will the Teaching Excellence Framewor...
What's wrong with our Universities, and will the Teaching Excellence Framewor...
 

Dernier

Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
KarakKing
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
heathfieldcps1
 

Dernier (20)

Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 

State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment

  • 1. DRAFT Foresight – SR-D1 v1 stage 2 Mental Capital and Mental Wellbeing Office of Science and Innovation Version date: 27th April 2007 State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment Professor Dorothy Bishop Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford This review has been commissioned as part of the UK Government’s Foresight project: Mental Capital and Mental Wellbeing. The views expressed do not represent the policy of any Government or organisation. Page 1
  • 2. Specific Language Impairment Abstract  What is Specific Language Impairment (SLI)? Brief account of criteria used  What causes SLI? Evidence for strong genetic component. Popular belief that poor language input from parents causes SLI is not supported by research evidence.  How common is SLI? Will depend on cut-offs used. Clinically significant problems decline markedly with age  International comparisons. Difficult because of lack of comparable language measures, and differences in organisation of services.  Educational impact of SLI. High risk of poor literacy and educational failure in those whose SLI persists to 5 years or beyond.  Impact of SLI on the individual and society. High rates of unemployment, social isolation and psychiatric disorder.  Can we predict outcome? Age, severity and level of comprehension as important predictors.  Can we intervene effectively? Woeful lack of good evidence, especially for older children. Many have persisting difficulties into adulthood despite intensive intervention. Computerised intervention worth exploring but has not so far fulfilled early promise.  Future trends. Prevalence likely to be stable. Emphasis on early years intervention only cost-effective if we identify those whose problems are likely to persist. Need for controlled trials of intervention. Need for proactive approach to identifying niches in society for people who lack language skills. What is Specific Language Impairment (SLI)?  SLI is diagnosed when a child’s language fails to develop along normal lines for no obvious reason: hearing loss, physical disability, emotional disturbance, parental neglect and brain injury are all ruled out before the diagnosis is made. The child is developing normally in areas other than language, and will show adequate intelligence if tested using nonverbal measures (e.g. construction puzzles; reasoning tasks with shapes).  Most people have heard of dyslexia and autism, but not SLI. This illustrates the power of labels: SLI is far more common than autism, and has close commonalities with dyslexia, but it does not have a catchy medical-sounding label, so it is neglected.  The manifestations of SLI are variable both in terms of severity and quality: in some children, the main difficulties are a delay in starting to talk, with immature language persisting into school age. For other children, problems in understanding language predominate: they may take in only one or two words of a complex sentence such as “if you bring your swimsuit tomorrow, we can go to the pool after lunch.” Page 2
  • 3. Vocabulary is usually limited in SLI, and children may have problems understanding unusual or abstract terms.  It is likely that the term SLI is an umbrella term for a range of distinct disorders, but as yet there is no agreement about subtypes.  A brief film featuring two young people with speech and language problems, Stephen and Graham, can be found on http://www.ican.org.uk/ What causes SLI?  30 years ago, it was often assumed that SLI was the consequence of poor parenting. This view was reinforced as recently as 2003 in a speech by Alan Wells, Director of the Basic Skills Agency, who maintained that many families do not converse with their children, and use instead a "daily grunt” (http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/education/2638889.stm). In fact, language development in children appears remarkably resilient in the face of limited language input: perhaps the clearest evidence comes from normally-hearing children who are raised by parents who are profoundly deaf and unable to converse intelligibly. In most cases, these children learn to talk normally, provided they are exposed to a small amount of normal language (Schiff-Myers, 1988).  Although other factors, such as chronic middle ear disease (which causes a mild and fluctuating hearing loss) may play a role in SLI, it is now generally accepted that this condition is not usually associated with hearing difficulties (Roberts et al., 2004), and is a strongly genetic disorder (Bishop, 2006). The best evidence comes from studies of twins. Two twins growing up together are exposed to the same home environment, yet may differ radically in their language skills. Such different outcomes are, however, seen almost exclusively in fraternal (non-identical) twins, who are genetically different. Identical twins share the same genes and tend to be much more similar in language ability. There can be some variation in the severity and persistence of SLI in identical twins, indicating that environmental factors affect the course of disorder, but it is unusual to find a child with SLI who has an identical twin with normal language.  Although twin studies indicate genes are important, SLI is not usually caused by a mutation in a single gene. Current evidence suggests that there are many different genes that can influence language learning, and SLI results when a child inherits a particularly detrimental combination of risk factors (Bishop, 2006).  Parents of children with SLI have similar genetic makeup and may well have limited language skills themselves. We need to beware of assuming that a parent with poor language has caused their child’s problems because of lack of stimulation. How common is SLI?  There is no sharp dividing line between SLI and normality: children vary in their language abilities, and the frequency of SLI will therefore Page 3
  • 4. depend on how stringently it is defined. An epidemiological study in the US identified around 7.4% of 5-year-olds as having SLI (Tomblin et al., 1997), based on an initial screening test followed by a more detailed language assessment, but they noted that this figure could rise or fall depending on the cutoffs used to define impairment.  An alternative way of estimating the scale of the problem is to consider the proportion of children referred for speech and language therapy: however, this does not give a satisfactory prevalence estimate, because it will depend on resources. Furthermore, problems with speech production and expressive language are more readily identified than poor language comprehension, but the latter has a worse prognosis (Zhang & Tomblin, 2000). In the study by Tomblin et al. (1997), in only 29% of cases had parents been informed previously that the child had speech and language problems.  The media occasionally run stories about an “epidemic” of language impairment in children starting school, sometimes attributing it to working parents, TV, and so on. There is no hard evidence of any secular change in prevalence of language difficulties. It is likely that perception of problems is inflated by the fact that more children are starting school at 4 years of age, and our large cities now include unprecedented numbers of children who do not have English as a first language. It would be well worth instituting an epidemiological study to monitor rates of language impairment (and related disorders), every 10 years or so, using standard methods of assessment, to estimate the true scale of the problem, and identify any secular trends. International comparisons  It is not possible to compare SLI in the UK with other countries. Part of the problem is that language assessments used to identify SLI may not work in translation, and can only be interpreted if we have adequate norms (i.e., data on a large representative population of children, that allow us to quantify typical performance at different ages). In many countries such data do not exist. Furthermore, even in English- speaking countries, there are difficulties in making international comparisons, because tests may be culturally specific.  There is a fascinating research literature comparing manifestations of SLI in different languages, but this is primarily of interest to those working on theoretical conceptualisations of the disorder (Leonard, 2000). Educational impact of SLI  Numerous studies have demonstrated that children with SLI are at high risk of educational failure. Low levels of literacy are common, even in children who receive specialist help, and educational attainments are typically poor (e.g. Catts et al., 2002; Snowling et al., 2001). This bleak picture is especially applicable to children who have language impairments that persist beyond the age of 5 years, and where comprehension as well as expressive language is affected. For instance, Simkin and Conti-Ramsden (2006) found that 67% of children Page 4
  • 5. with expressive SLI and 88% of those with receptive SLI had serious reading difficulties at 11 years of age. Impact of SLI on the individual and society  SLI is associated with a high rate of psychiatric disorder (see review by Cohen, 2001). For instance, Conti-Ramsden and Botting (2004) found that 64% of a sample of 11-year-olds with SLI scored above a clinical threshold on a questionnaire for psychiatric difficulties, and 36% were regularly bullied, compared with 12% of comparison children. The greatest risk appears to be for attentional problems and social difficulties, rather than conduct or emotional disorders. Looked at from the other direction, a study of children attending psychiatric clinics found that 28% had hitherto undetected language difficulties that were moderate or severe (Cohen et al., 1998). The reason for this association is not fully understood. An obvious mechanism is via stress induced by social rejection and isolation, both of which are common in children with language difficulties. In addition, a child with poor language skills may fail to use internalised language as a method of self-regulation, e.g. talking oneself through past events or future plans, or instructing oneself to behave in a particular way. It is also possible that the same genes that influence language learning lead directly to psychiatric vulnerability.  There are a few studies of the long-term outcome of children with SLI, and they mostly make depressing reading. There are elevated rates of unemployment, social isolation and psychiatric disorder (Clegg et al., 2005). However, most studies focused on outcomes of those with severe problems, where comprehension as well as expressive language is affected. Better outcomes were seen for children who had milder difficulties and did not require special educational provision (Snowling, et al., 2006). Where language difficulties had resolved by 5 years of age, there was no excess psychopathology. Can we predict outcome?  Longitudinal studies of young children with SLI generally agree that long-term outcome is worse in those who have more severe problems (e.g. Stothard et al.,1998). Severity can be measured both in terms of the level of performance on a given language measure, and the range of language functions that are affected.  Age is also critical. Understandably, there is much interest in identifying children as young as possible, on the grounds that early intervention is likely to be more effective than late. However, the situation is complicated by the fact that many children make good spontaneous progress after a late start in language. There have been several studies of ‘late-talkers’, identified at age 18-24 months because they are producing very few words. Most such children appear to be late bloomers, in that their outcome is similar to that of other children when followed up into middle childhood (Paul, 2000). A key factor appears to be comprehension level: those who have poor understanding of what others say are less likely to spontaneously catch up. However, the Page 5
  • 6. evidence base for this claim is slender, as few studies have looked specifically at comprehension in young children.  One can also see spontaneous recovery between 4 and 5 years of age, especially in children whose problems are predominantly with expressive language. One UK study studied children who had specific speech and language problems at 4 years of age; they were seen at 4.5 yr, 5.5 yr, 8 yr and 15 yr. Around 40% had essentially normalised by 5.5 years, and their long-term outcome was generally good, although minor literacy problems were found in some (Stothard et al, 1998). However, the 60% who still had measurable language problems at 5.5 years fared much worse, with the gap between them and other children widening as time went by. For the most severely affected children, it was not uncommon to see a drop in nonverbal intelligence in adolescence, as well as stagnation of language skills. Can we intervene effectively?  It would be a mistake to imagine that findings on genetics of SLI preclude the development of effective interventions. What the genetics studies tell us is that the kinds of variations in environment that most children experience do not play a salient role in determining who has a language problem. Thus, simply talking more to these children is unlikely to improve their language skills. But the genetic findings do not preclude the development of interventions tailored specifically to the child’s problems that could be effective. By analogy, we know that diabetes has a genetic basis, but we do not conclude the condition is untreatable: rather, we devise treatments that are outside the normal range of experience to tackle the underlying cause.  Children with SLI will normally receive therapy from a Speech and Language Therapist (SALT). In more severe cases, the child may receive special educational provision, either in a specialist class in a mainstream school, or in a special school.  Unfortunately, there has been little research on efficacy of interventions for SLI (Law et al, 2004). Such evidence as exists suggests intervention can be effective for improving intelligibility in children with speech impairments, but there is a lack of good research on interventions for language impairments, particularly with older children. In the UK, there are a number of special schools that have developed skills in working with older children with SLI over many years, but this knowledge is fragmented and not widely available, and there are no well-controlled studies of efficacy.  One problem for intervention studies is that one is often looking for an effect of intervention that may be superimposed on spontaneous improvement. SALTs regard early intervention as important, and some local authorities focus all their resources on preschoolers, and have no provision for school-aged children. This strategy is no doubt reinforced by the fact that many preschoolers who receive therapy make excellent progress. The point that is often missed is that many of these children would have done well with no intervention. For example, in one controlled study of early intervention there was no difference between Page 6
  • 7. treated and untreated children, not because nobody improved, but because everyone improved (Stevenson et al., 1982). Clearly we do not want to wait to intervene with those children who are at risk for long-term problems, but it is important to use research evidence on predictors to focus intervention those children who are unlikely to improve spontaneously.  One relatively new advance has been development of computerised interventions for SLI. The best-known of these is FastForword® (FFW), which was developed out of research showing that children with SLI have problems in processing sounds that are brief or rapid (Tallal et al., 1996). The idea behind FFW is that the brain can be trained to perceive increasingly small differences between sounds. Children play computerised games that adjust the level of difficulty of discrimination to the child’s performance level. The games involve both language stimuli (e.g. child determines whether a spoken sentence matches a pictured scenario) and nonlinguistic sounds (e.g. glides that resemble animal sounds, and vary in speed of change). The originators of FFW have won scientific accolades for what appeared at first to be a researcher’s Holy Grail – an intervention developed out of basic neuroscience that would help children. Unfortunately, later evaluations have found that FFW has not fulfilled its early promise, and appears to do no better than conventional speech and language therapy (e.g., Cohen et al., 2005). This is surprising, given that the intervention is so intensive, requiring the child to spend 90-100 minutes per day on the program for around 6 weeks. Although school boards in the US have bought into the program (in some cases for all pupils, not just those with SLI), there is growing scepticism as to whether this is justified, given the high financial cost of the program, and the time the child has to spend away from regular school activities (Troia & Whitney, 2003). An editorial in Nature Neuroscience (January 2004) expressed concern that this kind of educational program is being marketed without the evidence for efficacy that would be required for a drug treatment.  A final point to stress about early intervention concerns the link between SLI and poor literacy. Children in UK schools are introduced to literacy at ever earlier ages. In this regard, the UK is very different from our European neighbours, where children are often not introduced to literacy until 6 or even 7 years of age; rather, they education focuses on language-based activities that build the skills needed for successful reading and writing. The available evidence suggests that this late start does not lead to literacy problems, though comparisons are complicated by the different orthographies of different languages (Lundberg & Linnakylä, 1993). Evidence from children with SLI suggests that those who fail in literacy are children who are exposed to literacy instruction when relevant linguistic skills are still underdeveloped (Bird et al., 1995). It is possible that children would do better if literacy instruction were delayed until they had been shown to have a critical level of linguistic skill.  Ideally we need studies of efficacy that adopt the methods of randomised controlled trials as used in mainstream medicine. These are expensive and can run into difficulty because of the reluctance of Page 7
  • 8. many SALTs to have an untreated control group. They argue this is unethical; however, it could equally be argued that that it is unethical to persist in using interventions of unproven efficacy, especially when resources are limited. Unfortunately, it is impossible to do a cost- effectiveness analysis of interventions for SLI because we lack information on efficacy, both for improving SLI and for preventing other adverse outcomes. Future trends  The prevalence of SLI is likely to remain stable over the years, but its impact could become more serious, as our society becomes increasingly dependent on literacy skills.  Intervention currently focuses on the early years – from as early as 18 months in some cases, and up to around 4 years of age. This focus, though understandable, may be non-optimal, because of high levels of spontaneous improvement. One way forward would be to combine early intervention with use of using predictors from test results to identify those whose problems are unlikely to resolve spontaneously.  It would be worth conducting a controlled trial to consider whether children’s literacy skills can be enhanced by deferring literacy training until a minimal level of language competence has been attained.  Computerised intervention has generated great excitement, because it has the potential to deliver thousands of events to a child in a game- like format, thereby maintaining motivation, and adapting on-line to the child’s responses. Although attempts to develop computerised programs for treating SLI have, as yet, not been shown to be effective, we have as yet barely explored the potential of this approach. More research is needed to identify the conditions under which children with SLI can learn most effectively.  We should not underestimate the difficulty of remediating SLI: we may need to accept that some children will never be competent communicators. If so, then putting all our efforts into working to improve their language skills may be as futile as trying to train a blind child to see. For many children, education can be a dispiriting experience characterised by continual failure. We should continue to do research to identify barriers to language learning in these children so we can develop more effective interventions, but this should not be at the expense of other areas of development. It would be desirable to identify for a child with SLI a non-linguistic skill in which they could succeed, and to devote time on the curriculum to fostering this. These children could also benefit from prophylactic intervention designed to avoid the associated social problems: thus intervention with them would go beyond the narrow remit of improving language skills, to work also to enhance their ability to make friendships and find a role in the wider world. The modern world is increasingly hostile to those who lack skills in literacy, communication and computer processing: it may be necessary to take a more proactive approach to identifying niches for people who lack these skills. Page 8
  • 9. Conventional speech and language therapy has evolved in response to a clinical need, and is not an exact science. We need to ensure that clinical skills that are developed in specialist centres are preserved and disseminated. More use of video-links between specialist centres and mainstream schools could be helpful. Specific educational methods also need formal evaluation, so we can identify the most effective approaches. References Bird, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Freeman, N. (1995). Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 446-462. Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). What causes specific language impairment in children? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 217-221. Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 1142-1157. Clegg, J., Hollis, C., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2005). Developmental language disorders - a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 128-149. Cohen, N. J. (2001). Language impairment and psychopathology in infants, children and adolescents. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cohen, N. J., Barwick, M. A., Horodezky, N. B., Vallance, D. D., & Im, N. (1998). Language, achievement, and cognitive processing in psychiatrically disturbed children with previously identified and unsuspected language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 865-877. Cohen, W., Hodson, A., O'Hare, A., Boyle, J., Durrani, T., McCartney, E., Mattey, M., Naftalin, L., & Watson, J. (2005). Effects of computer-based intervention using acoustically modified speech (Fast ForWord- Language) in receptive language impairment: Outcomes from a randomised controlled trial. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 715-729. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (2004). Social difficulties and victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 145-161. Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2004). The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 924-943. Page 9
  • 10. Leonard, L. B. (2000). Specific language impairment across languages. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Lundberg, I., & Linnakylä, P. (1993). Teaching reading around the world. IEA study of reading literacy. Technical report: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement. Paul, R. (2000). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language delay: Ethical implications. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome (pp. 195-209). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Roberts, J. E., Rosenfeld, R. M., & Zeisel, S. A. (2004). Otitis media and speech and language: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Pediatrics, 113, e238-248. Rouse, C., & Krueger, A. (2004). Putting computerized instruction to the test: a randomized evalutaion of a 'scientifically based' reading program. Economics of Education Review, 23, 323-338. Schiff-Myers, N. (1988). Hearing children of deaf parents. In D. Bishop & K. Mogford (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances (pp. 47- 61). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Evidence of reading difficulty in subgroups of children with specific language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22, 315-331. Snowling, M. J., Adams, J. W., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2001). Educational attainments of school-leavers with a pre-school history of speech-language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 173-183. Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Stothard, S. E., Chipchase, B., & Kaplan, C. (2006). Psychosocial outcomes at 15 years of children with a preschool history of speech-language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 759-765. Stevenson, P., Bax, M., & Stevenson, J. (1982). The evaluation of home based speech therapy for language delayed pre-school children in an inner city area. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 17, 141-148. Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 407-418. Page 10
  • 11. Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Najarajan, S. S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. M., & Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81-84. Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245-1260. Troia, G. A., & Whitney, S.D. (2003). A close look at the efficacy of Fast ForWord Language for children with academic weaknesses. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 465-494. Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2000). The association of intervention receipt with speech-language profiles and social-demographic variables. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 345-357. Page 11