SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  286
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S
P A R I T I E S :
C U L T U R A L C O N S O N A N C E , S O C I A L C L A S
S , A N D
H E A L T H
William W. Dressler
University of Alabama
Mauro C. Balieiro
Paulista University
Rosane P. Ribeiro
University of São Paulo-Ribeirão Preto
José Ernesto dos Santos
University of São Paulo-Ribeirão Preto
Health disparities or health inequalities refer to enduring
differences between population
groups in health status, well-being, and mortality. Health
inequalities have been described
by race, ethnic group, gender, and social class. A variety of
theories have been proposed
to account for health inequalities, including access to medical
care and absolute material
deprivation. Several theorists (including Michael Marmot and
Richard Wilkinson) have
argued that relative deprivation is the primary factor. By this
they mean the inability of
individuals to achieve the kind of lifestyle that is valued and
considered normative in their
social context. In this article, we show that the concept and
measurement of cultural consonance
can operationalize what Marmot and Wilkinson mean by relative
deprivation. Cultural
consonance is the degree to which individuals approximate, in
their own beliefs and behaviors,
the prototypes for belief and behavior encoded in shared
cultural models. Widely shared cultural
models in society describe what is regarded both as appropriate
and desirable in many different
domains. These cultural models are both directive and
motivating: people try to achieve the
goals defined in these models; however, as a result of both
social and economic constraints,
some individuals are unable to effectively incorporate these
cultural goals into their own
lives. The result is an enduring loss of coherence in life,
because life is not unfolding in
the way that it, culturally speaking, “should.” The resulting
chronic stress is associated with
psychobiological distress. We illustrate this process with data
collected in urban Brazil. A theory
of cultural consonance provides a uniquely biocultural
contribution to the understanding of
health inequalities. [cultural consonance, cultural consensus,
relative deprivation, Brazil]
The Black Report (named for the chairman, Sir Douglas Black,
of the commission that
released it) was issued in Britain in 1980 (Black and Townsend
1982). It summarized the
findings of a commission whose charge was to examine
available health statistics and
ANNALS OF ANTHROPOLOGICAL PRACTICE 38.2, pp. 214–
231. ISSN: 2153-957X. C© 2015 by the American
Anthropological Association. DOI:10.1111/napa.12053
2 1 4 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
determine if there were detectable and reliable inequalities in
health in Britain. By this
they meant systematic and enduring health differences between
identifiable population
groups. The commission found inequalities in health by age,
gender, race, ethnicity, and,
especially, social class.
These inequalities are now a focus of research in all parts of the
world, although
here in the United States we have seen fit to sanitize the topic
under the rubric health
disparities. As noted on the website for Healthy People 2020,
the official policy blueprint
for improving public health in the United States:
If a health outcome is seen in a greater or lesser extent between
populations, there is disparity.
Race or ethnicity, sex, sexual identity, age, disability,
socioeconomic status, and geographic
location all contribute to an individual’s ability to achieve good
health. (Healthy People
2020, n.d.).
The question driving research is, what accounts for these
disparities? Any number of
structural and material factors could be relevant, as well as
behavioral factors, including
diet, physical activity, and health behaviors (e.g., smoking,
drinking). There are also
potential social selection processes, that is, individuals may
assume a lower socioeconomic
status as a result of their poor health, rather than their lower
socioeconomic status leading
to poor health. While all of these explanations have some merit,
they still, in the final
analysis, do not account for health disparities (Marmot 2004).
The major theorists in the area, notably Michael Marmot (2004),
and Richard Wilkin-
son and Kate Pickett (2011), emphasize instead a psychosocial
stress hypothesis. They
argue that individuals in disadvantaged groups are deprived of
meaningful participation
in the wider society and that the stresses associated with that
deprivation account for
health disparities.
We will argue here that, while there is considerable evidence in
support of this position,
the evidence is not as strong as it could be, primarily because
current models of health
disparities fail to take culture and biocultural interactions into
account. A biocultural
approach in anthropology is uniquely situated to contribute to
the study of health
disparities precisely because it takes the concept of culture
seriously, and is thus able to
link the individual to collective representations of what a
meaningful life is (Dressler
2005). The utility of this approach will be illustrated with data
collected over the past 20
years in urban Brazil.
S O C I O E C O N O M I C D I S P A R I T I E S I N H E A L
T H
Arguably, the most important thinkers on the question of
socioeconomic health dispar-
ities are Michael Marmot (2004) and Richard Wilkinson (1994;
Wilkinson and Pickett
2006, 2007). Marmot is most well known for his direction of the
Whitehall Studies.
These prospective epidemiologic studies were designed to
directly examine factors that
accounted for the inverse association of social class and the risk
of cardiovascular dis-
ease. British civil servants served as the study population. This
controlled for access to
health care, given that all members of the civil service (referred
to as Whitehall in British
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
1 5
vernacular) had access to high-quality care through the National
Health Service and
additional insurance. There was a clear social class hierarchy in
Whitehall, formed by
occupational categories. These included janitors and messengers
at the bottom of the
hierarchy, followed by clerical staff, then professional staff
(statisticians, economists), and
finally, at the top of the hierarchy, the administrative staff.
These are individuals with
elite British educations who help to set and direct policy.
After controlling for heart disease risk factors (blood pressure,
cholesterol, and others),
the lowest social class group was 50 percent more likely to die
from heart disease than
the highest social class group over a 25-year follow-up;
furthermore, there was a gradient
of increasing risk from the administrators down to the janitors,
with no sharp break in
the pattern. This is a particularly important part of the findings,
because this was a study
population in which no one could be considered “poor” in the
sense that they lacked
access to basic material resources for maintaining life. Rather,
what is striking about the
results in this and many other studies is that there is a
continuous gradient, such that even
doctoral level economists are at a slightly higher risk of
mortality than the administrative
staff who outrank them (Marmot 2004:60).
Much effort was invested in testing alternative hypotheses, such
as diet, smoking, or
other medical conditions, that could explain the gradient. Since
nothing could explain
the gradient, Marmot labeled it “The Status Syndrome” (2004).
The main thrust of his
argument is that increasing social status enables individuals to
exercise greater autonomy
in their lives, and this enhances their ability to live the life they
value. Conversely,
lower social status blocks these capabilities, resulting in long-
term, chronic stress, and an
increased risk of disease.
Richard Wilkinson (1994) is known for his studies of income
inequality and health.
Income inequality refers not to the differences in wealth or
income between individuals,
but rather to the entire range of socioeconomic variation within
a system (community,
U.S. state, or entire nation). It assesses the degree of inequality
at the group level. As such,
it is an “integral aggregate variable,” or a variable that refers
only to aggregates as units
of observation. The Gini coefficient is a common way to
measure income inequality. If
everyone in a community had the same amount of money, the
Gini coefficient would
be equal to 0.0; if only one person in the community had all the
money, the Gini
coefficient would be equal to 1.0. Wilkinson (1994) first
explored the association of
income inequality and life expectancy in Western European
societies, finding that as
income inequality increased, life expectancy declined. This was
striking, given that these
are the world’s most affluent nations. It was not the affluence,
however, that was at issue,
but rather how that affluence was distributed.
There have been numerous demonstrations of the association
between income in-
equality and a variety of health, behavioral, and psychological
outcomes (summarized in
Wilkinson and Pickett 2011). For example, here in the United
States, using states as units
of analysis, the correlation between the state-level Gini
coefficient and aggregate health
outcomes are as follows: with overall mortality, r = .403 (p <
.01); with homicide rates,
r = .695 (p < .01); and, with mortality from coronary heart
disease, r = .521 (p < .01;
source: author’s data). These correlations are large and
impressive in part because they
2 1 6 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
are based on aggregate data (hence, noise in the data gets
averaged out); nevertheless, it
is clear that income inequality is a potent correlate of our
collective well-being.
In considering these findings, Wilkinson asks if it is an absolute
(sometimes simply
referred to as material) deprivation or a relative deprivation that
is important. While
these terms will be discussed in greater detail below, the
primary distinction is between
lacking the basic resources of food and shelter necessary for
survival—or absolute/material
deprivation—versus lacking what is considered to be customary
in a given society—
or relative deprivation. Wilkinson (Wilkinson and Pickett 2007)
favors the relative
deprivation argument, since so much of his work has dealt with
social units that are
not only affluent (Western European nations), but are also
functioning welfare states.
Marmot (2004:118) also invokes the concept in his explanation
of the social gradient. The
concept of relative deprivation has a long history in social
thought, and a consideration
of some of that history will be useful for the argument being
constructed here.
D E P R I V A T I O N : A B S O L U T E A N D R E L A T I V
E
Poverty, its nature and effects, has been the focus of social
scientific inquiry since at least
the 19th century and Engels’ investigation of working class
conditions in England (Engels
1958[1845]). At the turn of the century, Rowntree and other
pioneering investigators in
Britain attempted to objectively measure levels of poverty by
estimating the nutritional
needs of working families, and then estimating the amount of
money required to fulfill
both those nutritional needs and additional needs for clothing,
heating, and household
sundries (Townsend 1979:32–33). Current definitions of poverty
in the United States are
based on this approach (Weinberg 1995).
In a real sense, these estimates of poverty levels are based on a
biological reductionist
assumption: that human well-being is to be measured solely in
terms of the minimum
physiological requirements for growth, development, resistance
to disease, and work
capacity. As such, this approach to poverty ignores socially or
culturally defined needs.
Townsend (1979), in his monumental Poverty in the United
Kingdom, argued that this
is a flawed approach to the definition of poverty for a variety of
reasons. He noted the
technical difficulties in estimating the nutritional requirements
of individuals, as well
as in determining the disposable income that is available to a
household (Townsend
1979:32–39). But the primary flaw in this approach is the extent
to which it ignores
consensual social definitions of appropriate lifestyles. As
Townsend put it:
Individuals, families and groups in the population can be said to
be in poverty when they
lack the resources to obtain the types of diet, participate in the
activities and have the
living conditions and amenities which are customary, or are at
least widely encouraged or
approved, in the societies to which they belong. Their resources
are so seriously below those
commanded by the average individual or family that they are, in
effect, excluded from ordinary
living patterns, customs and activities (Townsend 1979:31,
emphasis added).
What Townsend suggested was that poverty should be
understood as the degree
of relative deprivation experienced by individuals and groups.
He distinguished three
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
1 7
forms of deprivation: (1) objective deprivation, or the extent to
which individuals are
deprived of the material conditions of life; (2) conventionally
acknowledged deprivation,
or the extent to which persons are deprived of conditions
socially defined as necessary
or appropriate (see quote above); and (3) subjective deprivation,
meaning the extent to
which individuals feel themselves to be deprived (Townsend
1979:49). He regarded the
second sense of the term relative deprivation to be the most
important and useful, since
it assesses the extent to which individuals are prevented from
acting upon the social and
cultural norms of their own group.
Townsend was not alone in regarding this normative sense of
relative deprivation to
be essential in understanding patterns of poverty (e.g., Bell
1995). There is, however, one
difficulty that always arises in attempts to understand poverty
in this sense. What are the
“ordinary living patterns, customs, and activities” from which
persons are excluded due
to a lack of resources? How are such customs to be determined?
It is this issue that has
led many to adhere to a minimum income definition of poverty.
As Weinberg notes in
his rejection of relative deprivation: “Minimal consumption
standards for all necessary
commodities could in theory be established . . . but doing so
would raise difficult ethical
issues about which commodities to include (e.g., is a telephone
a necessity?)” (Weinberg 1995:6,
emphasis added).
Townsend (1979:38) suggested that, as much as possible, the
definition of relative
deprivation should be based on independent or external criteria.
Townsend argued that
“style of life” is a sociocultural dimension by which deprivation
could be assessed. Style
of life includes the acquisition of basic consumer goods as well
the awareness of culturally
valued knowledge and the participation in culturally valued
social activities. Since at least
the work of Veblen (1918), lifestyle in this sense has been seen
as a major component
of social judgments regarding social worth or prestige, so much
so that Veblen’s term
conspicuous consumption entered the vernacular. In recent
years, however, investigators
have argued that conspicuous consumption is only one aspect,
perhaps relatively small,
of the social meaning of consumption activities (Belk 1988;
Bourdieu 1984; Douglas
and Isherwood 1979; McCracken 1988). While recognizing the
push of consumerism,
this view sees style of life as a broadly patterned activity
expressive of more than what
Townsend (1979:58) referred to as “supercilious and derogatory
distinctions.” Rather,
style of life as a dimension of social life expresses and
reinforces in a concrete way a sense
of belonging to and integration into a social group. And where
an individual’s or family’s
lack of consonance with the community is evident materially, to
be demonstrated day in
and day out, the sense of loss or failure may be profound.
In his empirical work in Britain, Townsend developed a lengthy
inventory that in-
cluded items dealing with material consumption (e.g., owning a
television, refrigerator,
and other similar items) and with social behaviors (e.g., being
able to go out for a meal
periodically). No justification for the inclusion of items was
given other than the rea-
soned judgment of the investigator. A scale of deprivation was
then developed in which
an individual or household received one point for each item
they lacked. The way in
which this scale of relative deprivation covaried with other,
more conventional indicators
of poverty was then examined.
2 1 8 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
Later investigators expanded upon this approach, especially
using survey data to de-
fine what a representative sample of the population defined as
customary and approved
consumption (Hallerod 1996; Mack and Lansley 1985). While
these are reasonable ap-
proaches to the study of relative deprivation, they also tend to
be arbitrary in choosing
the cutoff point for what is thought to be important (e.g., 51
percent of the sample must
say an item is important?). These approaches to the definition of
consensual lifestyles rely
solely on the statistical aggregation of individual responses
(Shore 1991:11). There is no
independent model that enables the investigators: (1) to test for
the existence of a shared
model of lifestyles; (2) to estimate the degree of sharing of the
model; or (3) to estimate
the content of that model.
Furthermore, why limit the investigation of relative deprivation
to the specific domain
of lifestyle? While Veblen’s theory makes clear the importance
of this domain, why would
other domains not be important as well? Indeed, Marmot’s
(2004) argument suggests a
broader range of aspirations that might be considered.
It would be useful to have a theory regarding how normative
judgments might be
structured within a society, and a related set of methodological
procedures to assess the
distribution of normative judgments and normative behaviors
within a society. In this
article, innovations in culture theory (D’Andrade, 1984, 1995)
and ethnographic methods
(Romney et al. 1986) are used that may provide the study of
relative deprivation a more
substantial theoretical and methodological foundation, and in
turn contribute to an
understanding of socioeconomic health disparities (Kawachi and
Kennedy 1999). The
cultural consensus model (Romney et al. 1986) and the related
theory and measurement
of cultural consonance provide a more solid conceptual and
operational foundation for
examining these processes.
C U L T U R A L C O N S E N S U S A N D C U L T U R A L
C O N S O N A N C E
The foundation for the approach to be outlined here rests on a
cognitive theory of
culture. This theory starts with Goodenough’s (1996) definition
of culture: that which
one must know to function adequately in a given social setting.
This knowledge includes
procedural understandings of how to do and make things, social
understandings of how
to interact appropriately with others, and the understanding of
the world that underlies
belief and opinion.
This knowledge is learned both through individual experience,
resulting in idiosyn-
cratic understandings, and through systematic interaction with
others and socialization,
resulting in shared understandings. Shared knowledge, or
culture, is cognitively encoded
in the form of cultural models: skeletal, stripped-down
representations of some cultural
domain (e.g., lifestyle, the family), including the elements that
make up that domain and
processes that link the elements. Each domain contains at least
one prototype, or best
exemplar, of the domain. While all cultural models will be
conditioned by individual
biography, it is the sharing of cultural models that makes all
social life possible.
Cultural models vary in the degree to which they are shared.
The sharing of a cultural
model can be verified and quantified using the cultural
consensus model (Romney
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
1 9
et al. 1986; Weller 2007). The cultural consensus model
examines the degree to which the
similarity in individuals’ responses to a standardized set of
questions about a domain (e.g.,
“How important is it to own a house in order to live a good
life”? This question is repeated
for multiple items populating the domain of lifestyle.) can be
estimated by positing an
underlying shared model of “culturally correct” responses. Note
that the investigator does
not know what is culturally correct; rather, what is culturally
correct is estimated from
the degree to which individuals agree among themselves in their
responses.
Using a factor analysis of the similarities in response among
individuals across a given
knowledge base, cultural consensus analysis generates three
estimates: (1) the overall
degree of sharing, calculated from the ratio of the first-to-
second eigenvalue of a factor
analysis of persons (or Q-factor analysis); (2) the degree to
which each individual shares
in the knowledge base, referred to as cultural competence,
estimated by the individual’s
loading on the first unrotated factor; and (3) the estimated
culturally correct responses,
calculated as the weighted average of the responses of
individuals, giving higher weight
to individuals who are agreed with more strongly by others.
A cognitive theory of culture, operationalized using the cultural
consensus model,
provides a theoretically and methodologically satisfying way of
systematically identifying
for a group of people features of life and living “ . . . which are
customary, or are at
least widely encouraged or approved, in the societies to which
they belong” (Townsend
1979:31).
As Bourdieu (1984) has repeatedly reminded us, however,
people do not merely
think things, they do things as well. This is the importance of
the concept of cultural
consonance, defined as the degree to which individuals
approximate, in their own beliefs
and behaviors, the prototypes for belief and behavior encoded in
shared cultural models.
Using a measure of cultural consonance, the degree to which
individuals are able to put
into practice their shared understanding within a cultural
domain can be evaluated. Why
would people not be culturally consonant? The first and most
obvious answer is that
they choose to be different. This is probably more rare than it
might seem, especially for
a cultural domain in which there is wide agreement.
The second reason that individuals will have low cultural
consonance in a domain is
that “life chances,” to borrow Weber’s term, are stacked against
them. They do not have
the resources—principally economic, but also including racial
and gender inequalities—
to put into motion the understanding of how life is to be lived
that they share with their
neighbors.
Individuals with low cultural consonance suffer a relative
deprivation, when cultural
consonance is measured in terms of broadly shared life goals. A
rough sketch of how
socioeconomic health disparities are mediated by cultural
consonance is as follows: (1)
there are widely shared cultural domains that define “goals in
life,” ways of living to which
individuals aspire across the lifespan; (2) being able to achieve
cultural consonance within
a domain requires a variety of personal resources, especially
socioeconomic resources; (3)
when these resources are lacking, individuals are unable to
achieve cultural consonance;
(4) they see themselves, and are seen by others, not to have
achieved widely shared life
goals; (5) this results in a low sense of coherence, or the feeling
that life has not worked out
2 2 0 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
the way it is supposed to; (6) this also leads to unsatisfying
mundane social interactions
in which low cultural consonance individuals are treated with a
lack of respect because
they embody this status; (7) repeated arousal of the
hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis
and the sympathetic nervous system, as a result of unsatisfying
social interaction and a
low sense of coherence, lead to higher allostatic load; and (8)
higher allostatic load over
a lifetime results in poor health.
We have examined this process in research in urban Brazil over
the past 20 years,
testing key parts of this hypothesized causal chain.
Furthermore, combining data from
two time periods (1991 and 2001), we can examine the
association of cultural consonance
with health outcomes (blood pressure and perceived stress),
relative to the socioeconomic
gradient and relative to income inequality.
C U L T U R A L C O N S O N A N C E A N D H E A L T H I
N U R B A N B R A Z I L
We have carried out research in the city of Ribeirão Preto,
Brazil, over nearly 30 years, with
the past 20 years focusing on cultural consonance and health.
Specifically, we collected
data in 1991 (Dressler et al. 1997, 1998), in 2001 (Dressler,
Balieiro et al. 2005; Dressler
et al. 2007a), and we are currently collecting data in a study
initiated in 2011 (Dressler
et al. 2015). Research on cultural consonance requires a two-
stage method in which
the cultural models for domains in which cultural consonance is
to be measured are
investigated first using ethnographic methods, and then social
survey research is carried
out using measures of cultural consonance derived from the first
stage. Both stages were
carried out in 1991 and 2001; in the 2011 study, we have
completed the cultural modeling
stage and are engaged in collecting social survey data.
The Research Site
Ribeirão Preto is a city of 600,000 people in the north of the
state of São Paulo. Located
in a rich agricultural region, it is a center for light
manufacturing and financial services
related to the cultivation of sugar, coffee, citrus, and soy. It is
also a regional leader in
education and health care. It is known as unusually affluent.
Despite, or because of, its
affluence, Ribeirão Preto exhibits the differences between rich
and poor that characterize
all Brazilian communities. In the 1990s the Gini coefficient for
Brazil exceeded 0.60,
ranking it as one of the most unequal advanced industrial
nations. Since then, the Gini
coefficient has declined to nearly 0.50. This diminishing
inequality is a function of a
variety of factors, including both the stabilization of the
currency under the government
of Fernando Henrique Cardoso, and aggressive programs to deal
with poverty under the
governments of Luiz Inácio Lula da Silva and Dilma Rouseff.
All research in Brazil must take social inequality into account.
In our research we
have done this in two ways. When collecting ethnographic data,
care has been taken
to interview respondents distributed by educational level as a
proxy for social class. For
our survey data, we collected random samples stratified by
neighborhood. Four neigh-
borhoods were selected to represent socioeconomic differences
in Ribeirão Preto. The
first began its life as a classic favela, or squatter settlement. In
late 1992, the municipality
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
2 1
built a small conjunto (housing area or subdivision) of two-
room cinder block houses
and residents of the favela were moved there. Favelados paid
rent to the municipality on
a rent-to-own basis. Many favelados, due to their unstable
employment, were unable to
maintain these payments and left for other favelas in the area.
This enabled persons of
slightly higher means, who could amass the capital to purchase
these abandoned houses
from the city, to move into this neighborhood. Today, it is a
lower-class area of the city,
with fewer than half the residents from the original favela.
Residents tend to work as
unskilled laborers and domestic servants.
The second neighborhood is a classic conjunto habitacional.
These are subdivisions that
are built in partnership between the municipality and a
contractor. The neighborhood
started as uniform four-room cinder-block houses, but quickly
transformed as residents
added rooms, garden walls, and even second stories. Over the
years it has developed its
own commercial district with a supermarket, pharmacies, retail
stores, and bars. Residents
are employed in varied occupations, including school teachers,
nurses aids, store clerks,
and other lower-level professions (e.g., computer technician).
The third neighborhood is an old, traditional, middle-class area
that dates to the
founding of Ribeirão Preto. Built in a European style, houses
present seamless walls
to cobbled streets. Older residents remember the time when
families would spend the
evenings sitting outside on the sidewalk, exchanging news and
gossip with neighbors.
The neighborhood boasts its own central praça or “plaza”
fronting the Catholic Church,
and there is a large and vibrant business section that rivals the
city center. Residents tend
to work as lower-level professionals (teachers, nurses), to own
their own small businesses,
or to work as managers in local businesses.
The fourth neighborhood is a housing area adjacent to a
university. Many residents
are university professors, but many are also in the professions
(physicians, attorneys) or
the owners of large businesses and factories. The homes in the
neighborhood tend to be
quite large and spacious, with large and well-tended gardens.
Cultural Models and Cultural Consensus
Our research has evolved over the years in the study of cultural
models and cultural
consensus. In the 1991 study, we used cultural consensus
principally to confirm the shared
ideas around the cultural domains of lifestyle and social support
that had been selected
for study because of their theoretical importance as predictors
of health status (Dressler
et al. 1996, 1997; Dressler and Santos 2000). In 2001, we used
systematic techniques of
cultural domain analysis (Borgatti 1994) to identify and explore
four cultural domains:
lifestyle, social support, family life, and national identity
(Dressler, Borges et al. 2005).
In our recent study, begun in 2011, we have examined the same
four domains from 2001,
specifically to determine how cultural consensus changes or
remains stable (Dressler
et al. 2015).
The domain of lifestyle refers to material goods (such as
owning a home, a car, having
Internet access) and leisure activities (being with friends,
joining a sports club, shopping)
that are seen as necessary for living a good life. Social support
is the ability to call on specific
sources of support (family, friends, coworkers, church members,
specialists) in times of
2 2 2 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
T A B L E 1 . Cultural Consensus in Various Cultural Domains
in Three Studies
Year of Study
Cultural Domain 1991 2001 2011
Lifestyle
Eigenvalue ratio 5.55 6.59 7.70
Mean competence 0.65 (±.15) .71 (±.12) .72 (± .11)
Social Support
Eigenvalue ratio 3.11 6.53 5.21
Mean competence .61 (±.10) .67 (± .14) .65 (±.16)
Family Life
Eigenvalue ratio – 7.42 9.62
Mean competence .82 (±.09) .84 (±.09)
National Identity
Eigenvalue ratio – 3.97 3.50
Mean competence .57 (±.19) .61 (±.16)
felt need (common problems ranging from needing a ride to
psychological difficulties).
Family life refers to the characteristics, including both structure
and emotional bonds,
that define a good Brazilian family. And, national identity
organizes the characteristics
that make Brazilians, Brazilian (see Dressler et al. 2004;
Dressler, Balieiro, et al. 2005,
2007a, b; Dressler, Borges et al. 2005 for more detailed
descriptions of these domains).
For each domain, we focused on a single dimension of value or
importance to test
for cultural consensus; that is, do people rank elements of each
domain as more or less
important in ways consistent enough to infer that they are
drawing on a shared cultural
model in making their assessments? Table 1 summarizes data on
cultural consensus
for all three of these studies. In each study, in each domain,
there is a broad cultural
consensus that organizes people’s thinking. The level of cultural
consensus varies by
domain, but nevertheless, there is broad agreement within the
community, through
time, and based on different samples, of the importance of these
elements within these
domains. Furthermore, the cultural consensus displayed in Table
1 does not vary by
socioeconomic status. The importance assigned to elements of
each domain is equivalent
across socioeconomic groups (Dressler et al. 2015; Dressler,
Borges et al. 2005).
The cultural domains that we have examined here can be
grouped together under a
larger meta-domain we have labeled “goals in life.” That is,
these overlapping cultural
domains describe ends to which individuals aspire as they pass
through life stages.
Cultural Consonance
How effectively can people act on these shared understandings?
This is the question of
cultural consonance. As we have shown elsewhere, the degree to
which individuals, in
their own lives, actually match the profile of beliefs and
behaviors that are collectively
regarded as important in each domain can be measured in each
domain (Dressler 1996,
2005; Dressler, Borges et al. 2005). And, we have found that
low cultural consonance
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
2 3
is associated with higher blood pressure (Dressler et al. 1997,
1998, 2005), higher body
mass (Dressler et al. 2008, 2012), immune system challenge
(Dressler 2006), and higher
psychological distress (Dressler et al. 2007a, b). Some outcomes
are associated with
cultural consonance in specific domains; most, however, are
associated with generalized
cultural consonance. This refers to the tendency for individuals
to be consonant across
multiple domains (Dressler et al. 2007a).
The dilemma posed by low cultural consonance can be
illustrated in Figure 1. In the
graph in Panel A, cultural competence, averaged across four
cultural domains, is shown
in relation to educational attainment; there is no difference
among the groups in average
cultural competence (i.e., cultural knowledge). In Panel B,
generalized cultural consonance
is shown in relation to education level. These differences are
highly significant. In other
words, many people live in an environment of meaning in which
they know what is
valued and desired in life, but they are unable to achieve it in
their own behaviors.
Cultural Consonance, Economic Inequality, and Health
With the data we have from Brazil, we can examine how
cultural consonance mediates and
moderates the association of economic inequality and health.
Throughout this article, we
have oscillated in the discussion of socioeconomic health
disparities between the social
gradient and conditions of income inequality. Using our 1991
and 2001 data together,
we can examine the association of both these types of inequality
and health, relative to
cultural consonance. At the outset it should be emphasized that
this is best regarded as
an illustrative exercise rather than a definitive test of any
hypotheses. While the data
collection was guided by a single theoretical orientation, from
one study to the next we
were more concerned with refining and extending our theory
and methods than with
precise replication; however, we do have some data in common
between studies that can
serve for at least an exercise. Obviously, age (in years) and
gender (coded as women = 0
and men = 1) are comparable, as is the body mass index (BMI),
calculated from height
(in meters) and weight (in kilograms). For a measure of
socioeconomic status, we can
use family income, collected in both studies as the number of
minimum salaries coming
into the household, and then converted to constant 2001 reais
(the Brazilian currency).
The study itself (coded as 1991 = 0 and 2001 = 1) can serve as a
measure of changing
income inequality, since the Gini coefficient for Brazil declined
from about 0.60 in 1991
to 0.55 in 2001.
There are data available to roughly measure cultural consonance
in one cultural
domain: lifestyle. In 1991, to assess cultural competence, we
asked people to rate the
importance of items as defining one as “a success in life.” In
2001, respondents rated items
in terms of their importance “for having a good life.” There are
14 items in common
between these two inventories. Furthermore, the two sets of
ratings are correlated at
r = .81. This justifies combining them as a measure of cultural
consonance. To do so,
we weighted each item by its 1991 rating of importance, and
summed these for each
individual. The higher the value of this scale, the more an
individual approaches in his
or her own life a lifestyle that is collectively valued.
2 2 4 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
F I G U R E 1 . Association of educational level with cultural
consensus (Panel A) and cultural
consonance (Panel B).
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
2 5
T A B L E 2 . Descriptive Statistics
1991 2001 Total sample
(n = 304) (n = 271) (n = 575)
Age* 38.5 ( ± 12.4) 40.9 ( ± 11.6) 39.6 ( ± 12.1)
Sex (percentage of male) 40.5 39.1 39.8
BMI 24.5 ( ± 4.8) 25.2 ( ± 5.2) 24.8 ( ± 5.0)
Family income 1,309.8 ( ± 650.5) 1,381.1 ( ± 596.3) 1,343.3 ( ±
626.0)
Cultural consonance** 10.4 ( ± 3.4) 12.6 ( ± 2.5) 11.5 ( ± 3.2)
Perceived stress** 11.1 ( ± 6.9) 9.3 ( ± 5.7) 10.2 ( ± 6.5)
Systolic blood pressure 123.1 ( ± 17.7) 122.9 ( ± 16.4) 123.0 ( ±
16.9)
Tests of differences between studies.
*p < .01, **p < .001.
Finally, with respect to health outcomes, we collected Cohen’s
Perceived Stress Scale
(Cohen et al. 1983) in 1991 and 2001. This is a ten-item scale of
globally perceived stress
that is widely used and assesses the degree to which individuals
feel their lives are in
control and predictable. It has acceptable internal consistency
reliability in both studies
(Cronbach’s α = .80 and .79, respectively). Also, we have blood
pressure, measured using
a DINAMAP Vital Signs Monitor 845XT. This is an automated
blood pressure monitor
that essentially removes observer error. In each study, it was
regularly calibrated against a
standard mercury sphygmomanometer. For ease of presentation,
we will only use systolic
blood pressure as an outcome measure.
Descriptive data on these variables, for each study separately
and the studies pooled,
are shown in Table 2. The sample from 2001 is slightly older
than 1991. There is no overall
change in family income, although the group comparison
obscures the fact that income
increased significantly in the two lowest SES neighborhoods,
leveled off in the third, and
increased slightly in the fourth. Overall, cultural consonance in
lifestyle increased from
1991 to 2001, and perceived stress decreased. There was no
change in blood pressure,
BMI, or gender distribution.
Tables 3 and 4 present hierarchical multiple regression models
for each dependent
variable. In each analysis, age, sex, and family income (and
BMI for blood pressure)
are entered into the equation first. For both perceived stress and
blood pressure, family
income has an inverse association with the outcomes,
confirming the social gradient.
Next, study is entered into the equation as a dichotomous
variable. The significant
regression coefficient in Table 4 shows that perceived stress
declined over the ten years
between studies, while blood pressure did not. Next, cultural
consonance in lifestyle is
entered. There is an inverse association between cultural
consonance and each outcome;
furthermore, when cultural consonance is entered and
controlled, the inverse effect of
family income disappears. Finally, a term for the interaction
between cultural consonance
and study is entered. For blood pressure this is nonsignificant.
For perceived stress it is
significant, indicating that in the 2001 study, the size of the
association between cultural
consonance and perceived stress was smaller than in the 1991
study.
2 2 6 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
T A B L E 3 . Regression of Systolic Blood Pressure on
Covariates, Cultural Consonance, Year of Study,
and Interaction of Cultural Consonance × Year of Study
(Standardized Regression Coefficients)
Variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Age .353* .357* .359* .359*
Sex .257* .256* .251* .251*
BMI .238* .241* .248* .253*
Family income −.165* −.162* −.051 −.019
Study −.051 .014 .007
Cultural consonance −.223* −.255*
Cultural consonance × study .058
Multiple R .555* .557* .577* .579*
Multiple R2 .308 .310 .333 .335
*p < .001.
T A B L E 4 . Regression of Perceived Stress on Covariates,
Cultural Consonance, Year of Study, and
Interaction of Cultural Consonance × Year of Study
(Standardized Regression Coefficients)
Variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Age −.123* −.110* −.107* −.106*
Sex −.199** −.202** −.210** −.211**
Family income −.178** −.172** .010 −.022
Study −.124* −.041 −.062
Cultural consonance −.276** −.349**
Cultural consonance × study .145*
Multiple R .310** .333** .381** .398**
Multiple R2 .096 .111 .145 .158
*p < .01, **p < .001.
D I S C U S S I O N
Our aim in this article has been to explore the utility of a
biocultural approach to
the study of health disparities. Specifically, we have examined
cultural consonance as a
measure of relative deprivation in Townsend’s (1979) terms.
Socioeconomic disparities
were conceptualized and measured as a socioeconomic gradient
of individuals and as
time periods varying in level of income inequality. Results from
our research in Brazil
suggest that cultural consonance mediates the social gradient
and is moderated by income
inequality.
The most straightforward results here come from the analysis of
blood pressure. There
is an inverse association between family income and systolic
blood pressure; when cultural
consonance is entered into the analysis, it absorbs all of the
explanatory variance of family
income. This is consistent with a simple linear path model:
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
2 7
F I G U R E 2 . Perceived stress by year of study and
cultural consonance.
Family income → Cultural consonance → Blood pressure.
Borrowing a term from a statistical technique—path analysis—
for testing causal
models, family income is an exogenous variable. It stands at the
beginning of the causal
sequence and is, itself, causally unaccounted for. Through a
variety of means (family
of origin, educational opportunities, employment opportunities,
inheritance, marriage)
an individual is able to attain a particular income level. This in
turn represents the
resources that he or she can draw on and invest in achieving the
widely shared life
goals defined by a cultural consensus within the domains
organizing those life goals
and operationalized by the measure of cultural consonance.
Again, borrowing from path
analysis, cultural consonance is an endogenous variable,
because it is causally accounted
for by the exogenous variable, family income. Then, higher
cultural consonance leads
to lower blood pressure. Cultural consonance mediates the
inverse relationship between
position in the socioeconomic gradient and blood pressure.
This description of the results is true also for perceived stress,
except that the impact
of cultural consonance on perceived stress is moderated by
overall conditions of income
inequality. As the overall level of inequality declines, the effect
of cultural consonance on
perceived stress weakens. The pattern of these results is shown
in Figure 2.
These results may indicate the differing effects that cultural
consonance has for a vari-
able that does not depend on the conscious reporting of some
state by the respondent—
blood pressure—versus an outcome that depends on that
conscious reporting—perceived
stress. As we argued earlier, the chronic stress associated with
low cultural consonance ul-
timately involves pathways via the hypothalamic-pituitary-
adrenal axis and sympathetic
nervous system arousal. The allostatic load associated with
repeated arousals of these sys-
tems can lead directly to a coarsening of the smooth muscle
tissue surrounding arterioles
and, ultimately, sustained higher blood pressure. The source of
that chronic stress must
of course be meaningful to the individual, and we are arguing
that it is the collective
meaning attached to these cultural domains that is important.
In the case of perceived stress, all of the same processes are at
work plus the conscious
awareness and reporting of felt distress. It may be that under
conditions of declining
income inequality the experience of lower cultural consonance
is less distressing because
there is, at least, a sense of the potential for life to improve. In
the case of Brazil specifically,
2 2 8 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
this may have been enhanced in 2001 by the election of Luis
Inácio Lula da Silva, or, as
he was popularly known, “Lula.” (We date the study as 2001,
but survey research actually
extended over the period of 2001–03, which encompasses Lula’s
campaign for and election
to the Brazilian presidency.) Lula represented the Partida dos
Trabalhadores or Worker’s
Party (abbreviated as PT). The PT campaign specifically
focused on the need to help
alleviate the suffering of the poorest segments of the Brazilian
population, ultimately
implementing programs such as Fome Zero (Zero Hunger) and
Bolsa Famı́lia (Family
Allowance). These programs provided various forms of direct
and indirect assistance to
poor families, often consolidating and adding to programs that
had been initiated by
previous administrations.
The actual effectiveness of these social programs is not really
the issue here; rather,
Lula’s election and the promise of these programs, coupled with
the measureable reduc-
tion in income inequality (certainly initiated by the Plano Real,
or currency stabilization
program under the earlier administration of Fernando Henrique
Cardoso), may have
provided a different ethos for the poorer members of Brazilian
society such that the
conscious strain associated with low cultural consonance was
less likely to be reported.
There is, however, an alternative explanation for the moderating
effect of lower income
inequality. The measure of cultural consonance we are using
here may not be sufficiently
sensitive to assess this variable in 2001. With reduced
inequality, achieving what is, in
essence, a 1991 level of cultural consonance may, in 2001, be
easy enough to restrict the
range of variation of cultural consonance in 2001. This could
spuriously produce the
observed moderating effect.
This is, nevertheless, an example of a useful approach to the
study of socioeconomic
health disparities in anthropology. Anthropologists have been
strangely silent in the
empirical study of the health effects of economic inequality,
despite their vocal advocacy
for the poor (Dressler 2010). We argue that a biocultural theory,
explicitly derived
from the integration of perspectives in cultural and biological
anthropology, provides a
productive avenue for better understanding socioeconomic
health disparities. While the
actual results must be interpreted with a certain caution, given
that the data were not truly
designed to test these hypotheses, the theory and method of
cultural consonance appears
to provide a means for refining our grasp of processes that
generate socioeconomic health
inequalities. Future research may profit from adopting the
approach explicitly.
N O T E
Acknowledgments. Research reported here was supported by the
following grants from the National Science
Foundation: BNS-9020786, BCS-0091903, and BCS-1026429.
Jason DeCaro and Kathryn S. Oths offered
helpful comments on earlier drafts of the paper.
R E F E R E N C E S C I T E D
Belk, Russell W.
1988 Possessions and the Extended Self. Journal of Consumer
Research 15(2):139–168.
Bell, Carolyn S.
1995 What Is Poverty? The American Journal of Economics and
Sociology, 54(2):161–162.
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
2 9
Black, Douglas and Peter Townsend
1982 Inequalities in Health: The Black Report. Harmondsworth:
Penguin.
Borgatti, Stephen P.
1994 Cultural Domain Analysis. Journal of Quantitative
Anthropology 4(4):261–278.
Bourdieu, Pierre
1984 Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Cohen, Sheldon, Tom Karmack, and Robin Mermelstein
1983 A Global Measure of Perceived Stress. Journal of Health
and Social Behavior 24:385–396.
Douglas, Mary and Baron Isherwood.
1979 The World of Goods: Towards an Anthropology of
Consumption. New York: Basic Books.
D’Andrade, Roy
1984 Cultural Meaning Systems. In Culture Theory: Essays on
Mind, Self, and Emotion. Richard A.
Shweder, ed. Pp. 88–122. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
1995 The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Dressler, William W.
1996 Culture and Blood Pressure: Using Consensus Analysis to
Create a Measurement. Cultural Anthro-
pology Methods 8(3):6–8.
2006 Cultural consonance and C-reactive protein in urban
Brazil. Abstracts of the 105th Annual Meeting
of the American Anthropological Association, November 15–19,
San Jose, CA.
2005 What’s Cultural about Biocultural Research? Ethos
33(1):20–45.
2010 Social Inequality and Health: A Commentary. Medical
Anthropology Quarterly 24(4):549–554.
Dressler, William W., Mauro C. Balieiro, and José Ernesto dos
Santos
1997 The Cultural Construction of Social Support in Brazil:
Associations with Health Outcomes. Culture,
Medicine, and Psychiatry 21(3):303–335.
1998 Culture, Socioeconomic Status, and Physical and Mental
Health in Brazil. Medical Anthropology
Quarterly 12(4):424–446.
2015 Finding Culture in the Second Factor: Stability and
Change in Cultural Consensus and Residual
Agreement. Field Methods 27:22–38.
Dressler, William W., Mauro C. Balieiro, Rosane P. Ribeiro,
and José Ernesto Dos Santos
2005 Cultural Consonance and Arterial Blood Pressure in Urban
Brazil. Social Science and Medicine
61(3):527–540.
2007a Cultural Consonance and Psychological Distress:
Examining the Associations in Multiple Cultural
Domains. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 31(2):195–224.
2007b A Prospective Study of Cultural Consonance and
Depressive Symptoms in Urban Brazil. Social
Science and Medicine 65(10): 2058–2069.
Dressler, William W., Camila D. Borges, Mauro C. Balieiro,
and José Ernesto dos Santos
2005 Measuring Cultural Consonance: Examples with Special
Reference to Measurement Theory in
Anthropology. Field Methods 17(4):331–355.
Dressler, William W., and José Ernesto dos Santos
2000 Social and Cultural Dimensions of Hypertension in Brazil:
A Review. Cadernos de Saúde Pública
16(2): 303–315.
Dressler, William W., José Ernesto dos Santos, and Mauro C.
Balieiro
1996 Studying Diversity and Sharing in Culture: An Example of
Lifestyle in Brazil. Journal of Anthropo-
logical Research 52(3):331–353.
Dressler, William W., Kathryn S. Oths, Mauro C. Balieiro,
Rosane P. Ribeiro, and José Ernesto Dos Santos
2012 How Culture Shapes the Body: Cultural Consonance and
Body Mass in Urban Brazil. American
Journal of Human Biology 24(3):325–331.
Dressler, William W., Kathryn S. Oths, Rosane P. Ribeiro,
Mauro C. Balieiro, and José Ernesto Dos Santos
2004 Eating, Drinking and Being Depressed: The Social,
Cultural and Psychological Context of Alcohol
Consumption and Nutrition in a Brazilian Community. Social
Science and Medicine 59(4): 709–720.
2008 Cultural Consonance and Adult Body Composition in
Urban Brazil. American Journal of Human
Biology 20(1):15–22.
2 3 0 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 .
2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i
e s
Engels, Friedrich
1958 [1845] The Condition of the Working Class in England.
New York: Macmillan.
Goodenough, Ward
1996 Culture. In Encyclopedia of Cultural Anthropology, vol II.
David Levinson and Melvin Ember, eds.
Pp. 291–299. New York: Henry Holt & Co.
Hallerod, Bjorn
1996 Deprivation and Poverty: A Comparative Analysis of
Sweden and Great Britain. Acta Sociologica
39(2):141–168.
Healthy People 2020
N.d. Disparities.
http://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/about/DisparitiesAbout.asp
x, accessed July 1,
2013.
Kawachi, Ichiro, and Bruce P. Kennedy
1999 Income Inequality and Health: Pathways and Mechanisms.
Health Services Research 34(1 Pt 2):215–
227.
Mack, Joanna, and Stewart Lansley
1985 Poor Britain. London: George Allen and Unwin.
Marmot, Michael G.
2004 The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our
Health and Longevity. New York: Henry
Holt & Co.
McCracken, Grant D.
1988 Culture and Consumption: New Approaches to Symbolic
Character of Consumer Goods and Activ-
ities. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Romney, A. Kimball, Susan C. Weller, and William H.
Batchelder
1986 Culture as Consensus: A Theory of Culture and Informant
Accuracy. American Anthropologist 88(2):
313–338.
Shore, Bradd
1991 Twice-Born, Once Conceived: Meaning Construction and
Cultural Cognition. American Anthro-
pologist 93:9–27.
Townsend, Peter
1979 Poverty in the United Kingdom: A Survey of Household
Resources and Standards of Living. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Veblen, Thorstein
1918 The Theory of the Leisure Class: An Economic Study of
Institutions. New edition. New York: B. W.
Huebsch.
Weinberg, Daniel H.
1995 Measuring Poverty: Issues and Approaches. In Race,
Poverty, and Domestic Policy. C. Michael Henry,
ed. Pp. 99–116. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census.
Weller, Susan C.
2007 Cultural Consensus Theory: Applications and Frequently
Asked Questions. Field Methods 19(4):339–
368.
Wilkinson, Richard D.
1994 The Epidemiologic Transition—From Material Scarcity to
Social Disadvantage. Daedalus 123(4):
61–77.
Wilkinson, Richard G., and Kate E. Pickett
2006 Income Inequality and Population Health: A Review and
Explanation of the Evidence. Social Science
and Medicine 62(7): 1768–1784.
2007 The Problems of Relative Deprivation: Why Some
Societies Do Better than Others. Social Science
and Medicine 65(9):1965–1978.
2011 The Spirit Level: Why Greater Equality Makes Societies
Stronger. New York: Bloomsbury Press.
A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C
u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2
3 1
Copyright of Annals of Anthropological Practice is the property
of Wiley-Blackwell and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted
to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
T h e J o u r n a l o f D e v e l o p i n g A r e a s
Volume 49 No. 4
Fall 2015
RELATIVE DEPRIVATION AND THE
WORKING POOR: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS
Mustafa A. Rahman
North South University, Bangladesh.
ABSTRACT
In developing countries, we observe a new class of workers who
work but live in poverty. Not only
they are lowly paid but also deprived of opportunities available
in the society. A person’s feeling of
deprivation arises from incomes that are higher than his own
income. Sen in his 1976 work on poverty
measurement brought the term deprivation into focus. He
posited that an individual’s level of
deprivation in the income scale is an increasing function of the
number of persons who are better off
than the person in question, or, alternatively, the share of the
given population that is better off. In a
society where distribution of resources is unequal, deprivation
of a particular group of workers is not
unlikely. The idea behind deprivation theory is that merely lack
of some goods and opportunities do
not create a sense of deprivation among the workers. There are
other factors that cause and perpetuate
deprivation of the workers. This paper is an attempt to identify
the factors and examine deprivation
of the workers based on a sample survey conducted in
Bangladesh in 2008-09. Findings of the study
suggest that deprivation is acute among the uneducated aged
workers with large household size
predominated by low paid casual workers owning small
landholding. Deprivation and age of the
workers displayed a U-shaped curve indicating that deprivation
increases with the increase of age.
Deprivation is more pronounced among female workers
compared to male workers. Food and health
expenditure contributes significantly to reducing deprivation of
the workers. Obviously, owners of
better dwelling houses are less deprived than those owning poor
dwelling houses. Interestingly,
manufacturing workers have been found to be more deprived
than other sectors under study.
JEL Classifications: I132, C21, C51, O18
Keywords: Deprivation, working poor, endogenous variable,
regression, labour market.
Corresponding Author’s E-mail Address: [email protected]
INTRODUCTION
A person’s sense of deprivation arises from the comparison of
his situation in the society
with those of better off persons. Since 1970s, concerns of
relativity have become important
in assessing poverty across various groups of people in the
society living in different
conditions. An individual derives a satisfaction level (s) from
his/her income only if the
later is greater than the mean income. If his/her income is
smaller than the mean income,
he/she has a level of deprivation. Sen (1973, 1997) interpreted
the well-known Gini
coefficient from similar point of view. According to Sen, in any
pair wise comparison, the
person with lower income may suffer from depression on
finding that his/her income is
lower. The average of all such depressions in all pair wise
comparisons becomes the Gini
coefficient if the extent of a depression is proportional to the
difference between the
incomes concerned. Kakwani (1980) showed that if an
individual’s depression is
proportional to the square of income difference, we get the
coefficient of variation as the
average depression index.
mailto:[email protected]
380
Sen believes that comparing poverty across distributions may
involve different
standards of minimum necessities (1981) and that absolute
deprivation in terms of a
person’s capabilities relates to relative deprivation in terms of
commodities, incomes and
resources (1983). An absolutely poor person may not be
deprived relatively if he/she shares
the same condition as most people in the neighborhood. It
would not be unjust to say that
deprivation is closely linked to social exclusion which is
multifaceted in nature and arises
out of personal attributes and malfunctioning of socio-political
institutions of the country.
In the words of Sen (1983), people are deprived because their
capability set is simply not
broad enough to permit them catching up. On the contrary, it
may be due to structural lack
of opportunities either at workplaces or in the society.
The theory of relative deprivation is concerned with the feeling
of resentment caused
by inequality in the society. Deprivation is relative because
people feel deprived in relation
to others (Paul 1991). The idea of relative deprivation
originated from Stouffer et.al. (1949)
which later was developed by researchers and social scientists.
Over the past half a century
since Stouffer’s work, relative deprivation has been widely
researched and a large body of
empirical literature had developed based on this issue. The
concept has been used in
development studies and it has been prominent in studies of
worker satisfaction (Butler
1976 and Hill 1974). The term relative deprivation was first
formally defined by Davis
(1959). In his opinion any social group may be divided into
those who possess a desired
good (non-deprived) and those who do not (deprived). When a
deprived person compares
himself with a non-deprived, the resulting state will be called
‘relative deprivation’.
Following Davis, we may say that the preconditions for feeling
deprived are that
individuals (i) want X, (ii) compare themselves to someone
similar to themselves who have
X, and (iii) feel entitled to X.
The concept got further clarification in Runciman’s (1966)
formulation. To Davis’ list
he added a fourth precondition i.e., to feel relatively deprived
of X, one must also “see it
as feasible that he should have X”. Gurr (1970) defines relative
deprivation as the
discrepancy between the goods and opportunities (X) that
individuals want or to which
they feel entitled, and their current or anticipated ability to
obtain X. Later Yitzhaki (1979)
showed that one’s relative deprivation in a society can be
represented by the product of
Gini Coefficient and mean income of the overall population.
Chakravarty and Chakravarty
(1984) introduced new normative indices where each index
implies at least one social
welfare function. Paul (1991) argue that Yitzhaki (1979) and
Chakravarty and Chakravarty
(1984) indices of aggregate relative deprivation are based on the
unrealistic assumption
that the individual’s deprivation is insensitive to income
transfer among those who are
richer than him. He suggested an index where he has shown that
the deprivation of a person
is sensitive to income transfers among those who are richer than
him.
Apart from objective assessment of relative deprivation, there
are some who suggest
that the judgment of one’s own status is not simply a function
of one’s objective status.
Instead, resentment, anger, dissatisfaction and other
deprivation-related emotions vary with
the subjective assessment of one’s own status (Bernstein and
Crosby 19800). Although
sometimes neglected by economists, relative deprivation has
been linked to ‘‘definable and
measurable social and psychological reactions, such as different
types of alienation’’
(Durant and Christian, 1990) by social psychologists and to
social protests, discrimination,
feelings of injustice and subjective ill-being (Olson, Herman,
and Zanna, 1986). A person’s
feeling of deprivation may be absolute or relative. In the former
case income shares are a
381
source of envy while in the latter case people’s dissatisfaction
depends on income
differentials. However, both the measures are extreme cases and
hence the general notion
can be called intermediate invariance (Amiel and Cowell 1992,
Harrison and Sield 1994).
It is a convex mixture of relative and absolute deprivations
(Chakravarty 2009).
Deprivation as such can certainly be a significant handicap that
impoverishes the lives
of the excluded people in the society (Bardhan, 2011). Inclusive
growth therefore calls
forth a gradual reduction of relative deprivation by breaking
poverty traps (Banerjee and
Duflo, 2011). Therefore, relative deprivation is important from
the policy point of view
because if it is severe then the policies should focus on
redistribution rather than economic
growth (Kakwani 1993, Ravallion and Chen 1998).
The socioeconomic condition of the workers under study
indicates that merely lack of
some goods and opportunities does not create a sense of
deprivation among them.
Determining which factors do affect such feelings is the focus
of our investigation. It also
examines deprivation of a worker compared to another worker
in the informal labour
market across various sectors (agriculture, manufacturing,
transport, construction and
service). The study is based on a sample survey conducted in
Bangladesh in 2008-09. To
work out the exercise standard regression and IV regression
have been used as instruments
of analysis.
DATA AND METHODOLOGY
Data Source
Data for estimating the model have been obtained from a sample
survey conducted in
Bangladesh during 2008-09. Data were collected from both rural
and urban areas. A multi-
stage stratified random sampling technique without replacement
was used to select sample
locations and respondents. Households were drawn randomly,
proportionate to the size in
the population: 248 from the rural area and 412 from the urban
area. The rural area refers
to agriculture sector while urban area includes manufacturing,
construction, transport and
services sectors. Thus a total of 660 households have been
selected for intensive interview.
Because of some statistical discrepancy, some observations have
been deleted resulting in
a sample size of 610 instead of 660, the net sample size is 610.
Specification of the Model
The model of capturing relative deprivation across various
sectors under study has been
specified as follows:
� = �� + �� + � (1)
Where,
� = (��, �� , �� , �� ) ′
�= (��, ��, �� , �� )
382
� = Independent variables {age, education etc.}
Equation (1) can be re-written as
� = �� + ���� + �� �� + �� �� + �� �� +
(2)
In the above equation �stands for relative deprivation
index. The notations �� , �� ,
�� and �� represent dummy variables for agriculture,
manufacturing, transport and
construction sectors respectively. The service sector has been
considered as the reference
dummy. The magnitude of individual sector dummies compared
with that of service sector
dummy would provide us relative deprivation for the individual
sectors with respect to
service sector.
Description of Model Variables
Our dependent variable is relative deprivation index (D) which
is the difference between
actual wage and the average wage of the sample workers. Wages
has been considered
synonymous with income because, for the workers under
analysis wages is their only
source of income. The importance of the average income is due
to the fact that an
individual’s feelings depends not only on his income level but
also on his rank in the
income distribution and an individual is assumed to have
negative feelings if his/her
earning is less than the average income (Berrebi and Silber
1989). The independent
variables have been described below:
TABLE 1: DESCRIPTION OF INDEPENDENT VARIABLES
Independent variables Description
Marital status Married/unmarried
Age Actual years attained
Age2 Age*Age
Education (EDU) Years of schooling
Gender (GEN) Male and Female respondents
Value of assets (VASSET) It refers to assets (livestock,
furniture,
electronics, agricultural equipment, bicycle,
watch, rickshaw, pushcarts, sewing machine
etc.) valued at 2009 prices.
Landholding (LHOLD) Ownership of homestead and cultivable
lands
measured in acres.
Food expenditure (FEXP) Indicates expenses on food items
(rice, wheat,
meat, fish, milk, egg, lentil, vegetables etc.)
valued at 2009 prices
383
Health expenditure ((HLTEXP) Refers to doctor’s consultation
fee, expenses
for medicine, hospitalisation etc.
Education expenditure (EDUEXP) Refers to expenses for tuition
fees, books,
accessories, school dress etc.
Household size (HHSIZE) Average size of the household
Dwelling house 1= Kutcha (mud floor), 2= semi-pucca
(concrete wall and tin-shed), 3= pucca
( concrete wall and roof), 4= thatched
(bamboo and straw made)
Drinking water 1= Tube well, 2= surface water, 3=
tape/supply water
Agriculture sector
Manufacturing sector
Transport sector
Construction sector
Empirical Application
The relative deprivation index (D) has been calculated based on
wages of the workers. It is
the difference between actual wage and the average wage of the
workers. For our analysis
26 independent variables have been considered for the
regression model. To achieve a
sound regression model I examined if the independent variables
were highly correlated or
not. With that end in view I have worked out correlation matrix
for the stated variables.
From the correlation matrix urban-rural area, age-sector (4
variables) and gender-sector
(4variables) interacting variables have been found highly
correlated. Therefore, to avoid
multicollinearity, these variables have been dropped and as a
result 17 variables remained
for regression analysis. Incorporating the independent variables
in equation (2) above I
have got equation (3):
CCTTMMAAi
i
i
ddddxD
17
1
0
(3)
Since the minimum wage in our sample might have been
different from the population
minimum wage,I have compared results from truncated
regression and OLS regression to
examine if there was any significant dissimilarity between the
two models. The estimates
of the models indicate that both the models are similar in terms
of predictability and
significance of the parameters . The estimates of regression
coefficients for both the models
384
have been presented in Table 2. There were some insignificant
variables in the estimated
model which were- value of assets, agriculture and construction
sectors. At this stage, I
have conducted Wald Test to examine if these variables are
jointly insignificant or not. The
Wald test indicates that the variables i.e., value of assets;
agriculture and construction
sectors are jointly insignificant (Table 3).
TABLE 2. ESTIMATES OF TRUNCATED AND OLS
REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS
Independent variables Coefficients
( truncated regression)
Coefficients
( OLS regression)
Constant - 42.893
(4.781)
- 40.440
(4.613)
Age
Age2
1.082***
( 0.296)
- .015***
(0 . 004)
- 0. 949***
(0. 285)
(-. 013)***
( . 003)
Agriculture 0.024
(2.015)
- 0.131
(1. 940)
Construction - 3.552
(2.227)
- 3.413
(2. 131)
Manufacturing -7.093***
(1.731)
- 7.185***
(1. 677)
Transportation 9.487***
( 2.174)
9.466***
(2.140)
Dwelling house 2.895***
(0.715)
2.850***
(0.695)
Drinking water 3.444***
(0.668)
3.239***
(0.653)
Education 1.180**
(0.627)
1.143**
(0.616)
Education expenditure 0.010**
(0.002)
0.010
(0.002)
Food expenditure 0.017***
(0.004)
0.017***
(0.004)
Health expenditure 0.008***
(0.002)
0.008***
(0.002)
Gender 12.174***
(1.192)
11.471***
(1.144)
Household size 1.601***
(0.498)
1.425***
(0.487)
Landholding 13.400***
(2.880)
13.118***
(2. 854)
Value of assets
Marital status
0.004
(0.002)
2.56**
(1.08)
0.004
(0.002)
1.28**
(0.52)
Note: Terms in the parentheses indicate standard error. ** and
*** indicate significance at 5% and 1% levels.
385
TABLE 3.ESTIMATES OF WALD TEST
Test Statistic Value df Probability
F- statistic 1.7162 (3, 643) 0.1624
Chi-square 5.1486 3 0.1612
Eliminating the insignificant independent variables, the
reformulated regression model
stands as follows:
(4)
ANALYSIS OF RESULTS
The estimates of the reformulated regression are given in Table
4. The estimated signs of
the variables have the expected signs and the coefficients are
statistically significant. The
relationship between deprivation faced by workers and their age
follow a U- shaped curve.
The deprivation decreases with an increase in age till the
worker reaches the age of 38 and
then it increases with an increase in workers age. Manufacturing
workers have been found
to be more deprived and transport workers less deprived
compared to service sector
workers. The workers who own high quality of dwelling house
and have access to drinking
water experience less deprivation.
We observe the same trend in case of food and health
expenditure. Male workers have
been found to be less deprived compared to female workers.
Deprivation is more
pronounced for the married workers compared to those who are
unmarried, divorced or
separated. Deprivation is relatively low for larger households
compared to smaller
households which may be because of more earning members of
the larger households.
Households with larger landholdingshave been found less
deprived compared to smaller
landholdings.
It would seem reasonable to assume that if an individual spends
more on his/her food,
then this individual would be less deprived. However, a more
deprived person would also
spend less on food expenditure. This means that the causality
may run both ways. Hence,
to test whether food expenditure is an endogenous variable,I
performed Hausman test in
STATA using IV-2SLS and I found it as an endogenous
variable. Under the null hypothesis
that the food expenditure can actually be treated as exogenous,
the test statistic (see page
482 in Baum et.al., 2007) is distributed as
2
(degree of freedom is the number of regressors being tested for
endogeniety). The null
hypothesis stands rejected since the p-value for the test has
been found to be less than 0.05
or the
2
2
0.95,1
In order to deal with endogenietyI needed to choose instruments
which affected food
expenditure but not directly impacting deprivation. I have
chosen household size as an
instrument. Household size affects food expenditure directly but
affects deprivation via
food expenditure. Ceteris Paribus, households with larger size
would spend more on food
expenditure compared to smaller size. However, household size
may not be directly related
to deprivation. The estimates of the model adjusted for
endogeneity and heteroskcedasticity
CCTTMMAAi
i
i
ddddxD
14
1
0
386
are presented in Table 3. It is interesting to note that while
‘food expenditure’ was found
highly significant in reformulated regression, it appeared
insignificant in 2SLS regression
(Table 3). This could be because it may potentially be an
endogenous variable. I also
observe in this table that after ‘food expenditure’ is
instrumented for, in 2SLS regression
the corresponding coefficient changes its sign.
TABLE 4. ESTIMATES OF REFORMULATED AND 2SLS
REGRESSION
COEFFICIENTS
Independent variables Coefficients
(reformulated OLS regression)
Coefficients
(2SLS regression)
Constant -57.258***
(6.390)
- 36.748***
(7.702)
Age 0 .897***
( 0.285)
0.795***
( 0307)
Age2 - .012***
(0. 004)
- .011***
(0.004)
Manufacturing - 5. 668***
(1.477)
- 3.947**
(1.866)
Transportation 9.974***
(1.773)
11.161***
(2.246)
Dwelling house 2.556***
( 0.652)
2.305***
( 0.679)
Drinking water 3.118***
( 0.617)
3.562***
( 0.677)
Education 1.378**
( 0.610)
1.603***
(0.572)
Education expenditure 0.010***
( 0.002)
0.012***
(0.002)
Food expenditure 0.017***
( 0.003)
-0.006
(0.008)
Health expenditure 0.007***
( 0.002)
0.006***
( 0.002)
Gender 12.35***
( 1.104)
12.903***
( 1.290)
Marital status 4.011***
( 1.300)
3.807***
(1.371)
Household size
1.359***
( 0.483)
na
Landholding 14.31***
( 2.758)
11.621***
( 3.500)
Note: Terms in the parentheses indicate standard error.
Household size is an instrumental variable.
** and *** indicate significance at 5% and 1% levels.
387
CONCLUSIONS
In Bangladesh, deprivation is widespread particularly among the
poor who work but live
in poverty. Our study has provided some interesting insights
whose implications for policy
are enormous. In our study, deprivation has been found to
decrease with the increase of age
of the workers but up to a certain limit. This may be because of
the fact that workers remain
productive at the early stages of their lives.In Bangladesh, the
poor workers if however,
they get job remain employed in low paid occupations. They
end up their career with
meager savings or zero savings. The finding suggests that the
aged workers should be
provided with adequate old age benefit so that they can lead a
plain living.The government
may introduce income supplement schemes for them like India
and Thailand.
Both education and educational expenses have offsetting effect
on deprivation of the
workers. The implication for policy is that if the workers are
provided with education, their
deprivation would cease to exist in near future. Education
lessens deprivation because
educated workers are highly productive, high productivity
generates high income for the
workers. Therefore, education of the workers should be
emphasized at both household and
national levels. At household level, a portion of the family
budget should be reserved for
educating the workers side by side with work. At national level,
adequate resources should
be allocated for educating and training the workers while they
are on-the-job.
The extent of deprivation is severe in manufacturing sector
compared to other sectors
under study. The manufacturing workers as they are mostly
uneducated and unskilled are
low paid workers. The employers are unwilling to pay them
high wages because of their
low productivity, as a result the poor workers fall trapped in
low skill-low paid vicious
cycle of deprivation poverty. The government in this regard
must enforce a reasonable
minimum wages for them so that they can lead a decent life.
Health expenditure has been
found to have reduced deprivation of the workers. The
implication for policy is that if
government subsidizes health related costs such as free
medicine and medicare, it would
help reducing deprivation of the workers because healthy
workers are highly productive.
High productivity yields high income contributing to lessening
severity of deprivation.
Introducing health insurance scheme for the workers may be a
good step forward in this
regard.
Male workers have been found to be less deprived compared to
female workers.
Deprivation is more pronounced for the married workers
compared to those who are
unmarried, divorced or separated. Male-female wage differential
is a stark reality in
developing countries like ours. For equal worth job, female
workers are paid more than
their male counterparts. The implication for policy in this
regard is that government should
enact legislations ensuring equal pay for equal worth job for
both male and female workers.
The female workers should be organized through trade unions
so that they can raise their
voices for fair wages and other benefits. Apart from enacting
legislations the government
should ratify relevant UN conventions for eliminating the wage
gap between male and
female workers. At enterprise level, both male and female
workers should be treated fairly
and judiciously. Land holdings have been found to lessen
acuteness of deprivation.
Therefore, the poor workers may be gifted with adequate fallow
land through appropriate
land reform and distribution.
388
REFERENCES
Akerlof, G.A., “A Theory of Social Custom, of Which
Unemployment May be One
Consequence”, 1980, Quarterly Journal of Economics 94, 749-
775.
Bardhan, P. K.” Awakening Giants, Feet of Clay: Assessing the
Economic Rise of China
and India”, 2010, MIT Press.
Banerjee, A. V. and E. Duflo., “Poor Economics: A Radical
Rethinking of the Way to
Fight Global Poverty”, 2011, Random House, New Delhi.
Baum, C. F., Schaffer, M. E. and Stillman, S., "Enhanced
routines for instrumental
variables/generalized method of moments estimation and
testing,", 2007, Stata
Journal, 7(4), 465-506.
Bernstein. M. and Crosby, F., “An Empirical Examination of
Relative Deprivaiton Theory”
1980, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 442-456.
Berrebi, Z. M. and Silber, J., “Deprivation: the Gini Index of
Inequality and the Flatness
of an Income Distribution”, 1989, Mathematical Social
Sciences, 18, pp. 229-237.
Blau, Peter M.,”Exchange and Power in Social Life”, 1964, New
York: Wiley.
Clark, A.E., Oswald, A.J., “Comparison-Concave Utility and
Following Behaviour in
Social and Economic Settings”, 1998,.Journal of Public
Economics, 63, 232-45.
Chakravarty, S. R. And Chakravarty, A.B., “On Indices of
Relative Deprivation.
Economics Letters”, 1984, 14, 283-287.
Clark, A.E. Oswald, A.J., “Satisfaction and Comparison
Income”, 1996, .Journal of Public
Economics, 61, 359-381.
Durant, T. J. and O, Christian, O., ‘‘Socio-Economic Predictors
of Alienation Among the
Elderly,’’ 1990, International Journal of Aging and Human
Development, 31, 205–
17.
Kakwani, N., “Poverty and economic growth with an application
to Cote d’Ivoire”, 1993,
Review of Income and Wealth, 39(2), pp. 121-139.
Lenoir, Rene., “Les Exclus - UnFrancaissur Dix”, 1974, Paris,
Editions du Seuil.
Olson, J. M., Herman, P. and Zanna, M. P., :” Relatvûe
Depriûation and Social
Comparisons: The Ontario Symposium”, 1986, vo1. 4, Lawrence
Erlbaum
Associates Publishers, London.
Rahman, M. A., “Household Characteristics and Poverty: A
Logistic Regression Analysis”
2013, Journal of Developing Areas, Vol. 47, No. 1.
Ravallion, M. and Chen, S.,“When economic reform is faster
than statistical reform-
measuring and explaining inequality in rural China” 1998, ,
Policy Research
Working Paper Series-1992, World Bank.
Sen, A.,(1981): “Poverty and Famine: An Essay on Entitlement
and Deprivation”, 1981,
Clarendon Press, Oxford University Press.
Sen, A. “Poverty: an Ordinal Approach to Measurement”, 1976,
Econometrica, Vol.44,
pp. 219-231.
Sen, A., “Poor, relatively speaking”, 1983, Oxford Economic
Papers, 35, 153-69.
Silver, H., “Reconceptualising Social Disadvantage - Three
Paradigms of Social
Exclusion”, 1995, In Gerry, R. (ed.) Social Exclusion: Rhetoric,
Reality and
Response.
Paul, S., “An Index of Relative Deprivation”, 1991, Economics
Letters, 36, pp.337-341.
http://ideas.repec.org/a/tsj/stataj/v7y2007i4p465-506.html
http://ideas.repec.org/a/tsj/stataj/v7y2007i4p465-506.html
http://ideas.repec.org/s/tsj/stataj.html
http://ideas.repec.org/s/tsj/stataj.html
389
Stouffer, S. A. Suchman, E.A. DeVinney, L.C., Star, S.A. and
Williams, R.M., “The
American Soldier: Adjustments during Army Life 1”,1949,
Princeton University
Press, Princeton NJ,.Runciman.
W. G., “Relative Deprivation and Social Justice”, 1966,
Routledge and Kegan Paul,
London.
Butler, R. J., “Relative Deprivation and Power: A switched
replication design using time
series data of strike rates in American and British coal mining”,
1976, Human
Relations, 29, 623-641.
Hill, R. C., “Unionisation and racial income inequality in the
metropolice”, 1974, American
Sociological Review, 39, 507-522.
Copyright of Journal of Developing Areas is the property of
Tennessee State University,
College of Business and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted
to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may
print, download, or email articles for individual use.
Development Policy Review, 2015, 33 (1): 5-13
© The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015
Overseas Development Institute.
Development Policy Review 33 (1)
Equity: Not Only for Idealists
Lawrence Haddad∗
Growing disparities in development outcomes are storing up
trouble for
current and future generations. In addition to the moral issues
raised and the
intrinsic welfare effects for those experiencing relative
deprivation, this article
argues that recent research supports a stronger public-policy
focus on equity.
Increasing inequity blunts both growth and the ability of growth
to translate
into human-development outcomes, and puts institutions, social
cohesion and
the productivity of future generations at risk. The article
highlights new micro
evidence suggesting that a focus on the most marginal might
well lead to
higher benefit:cost ratios, and outlines the choices that need to
be made to
generate a more equity-focused policy agenda.
Key words: Equity, policy, politics, idealists, pragmatists
1 Introduction
In the world of international development, a long-standing
divide has separated ‘idealists’,
at one extreme, and the more ‘practical-minded’ at the other.
The former tend to give more
weight to addressing the needs of the most destitute and
excluded and less attention to the
benefit:cost ratios of doing so. The latter are more driven by
aggregate impact and costs,
with a focus on national averages such as GDP per capita. They
worry about whether a
focus on equity will curb growth, about the practicalities of
reaching the poorest and about
the political feasibility of doing so.
This article aims to bring together and outline some disparate
evidence streams that
suggest that the interests of these groups are converging. It
comes to the conclusion that,
increasingly, focusing on the most vulnerable is not only a
moral necessity; it seems like a
practical approach to promoting development.
Current trends on development outcomes are complex and, at
times, even seem
contradictory. There is much good news. At the macro level,
many countries (for example,
India, Ghana, Vietnam, Indonesia) are graduating out of needing
traditional forms of
development assistance because of their impressive rates of
GDP per capita growth. There
also has been progress in reducing poverty rates and child
mortality and in increasing
school enrolment and attainment (UN, 2011). These
achievements are real and are
improving the wellbeing of hundreds of millions of people.
However, while these indicators of development – many based
on averages – are
improving, there is a growing disparity in the rates of progress
in outcomes between rich
and poor groups within countries. For example, the Millennium
Development Goals Report
∗ Senior Research Fellow, International Food Policy Research
Institute, Washington, DC ([email protected]).
This article was motivated by conversations with Tony Lake,
the Executive Director of UNICEF, and his
colleague Robert Jenkins, and he would like to thank both of
them for their support. All errors are his alone.
6 Lawrence Haddad
© The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015
Overseas Development Institute.
Development Policy Review 33 (1)
2011 states that ‘the poorest children are making the slowest
progress in reducing
underweight prevalence’ (UN, 2011: 15). In other examples, a
study by UNICEF (2010a)
found that in 18 out of 26 countries where the national under-
five mortality rate has
declined by 10% or more since 1990, the gap between the
richest and poorest quintiles had
either grown or remained unchanged. In another report
(UNICEF, 2010b) an analysis of
data from India found that 166 million people gained access to
improved sanitation
between 1995 and 2008, but little progress was made in the
poorest households. In the same
report, data from West and Central Africa from 1990 to 2008
show that measles-
immunisation coverage increased by 10% in the wealthiest
quintile of the population, but
by only 3% in the poorest quintile. In other words, progress is
being made, but is skewed
away from the worst-off.
These growing disparities matter. First, there are the moral
issues raised by the fact
that many of the disparities are based largely on chance. The
circumstances someone is
born into, and the genetic make-up they inherit, play a very
large role in the poverty
consequences of their exposure to negative exogenous shocks
(Currie, 2011). For example,
those born in an exposed landscape, with fragile health, are
more likely to suffer from an
extreme weather shock. Second, the disparities carry intrinsic
negative welfare
consequences (Klasen, 2008; Deaton, 2003): status matters for a
whole host of psycho-
social reasons. But our focus here is on the economic and
political consequences. We argue
that these disparities matter for the following reasons: (i) they
exert an ever stronger brake
on the conversion of economic growth into development
outcomes—and on growth itself;
(ii) they are storing up trouble for current and future
generations; and (iii) they demand a
realignment of policy approaches.
Two factors add some urgency to our arguments. First, as we
shall see, there is much
new evidence now emerging on the consequences of inequality.
Second, the disparities do
not promise to narrow anytime soon. For example, the poorest
20% of the world’s
population holds 2% of its income. At the rate of change seen in
the past 20 years, it will
take more than 250 years for the poorest 20% to hold 10%
(Ortiz and Cummins, 2011:
Table 5).
2 Growing disparities are a brake on the poverty-reducing
power
of economic growth and on growth itself
The quality of economic growth is coming under increased
scrutiny. When does it generate
jobs for the poorest? When does it fuel corruption? When does
it generate negative
environmental externalities? When does it bypass the most
vulnerable? These are important
debates, given the strong growth in Africa and Asia and the
need to sustain it to improve
development outcomes.
There is a near consensus on the impact of growth on poverty.
For any type of growth,
we know that its ability to reduce poverty is diminished the
greater the initial inequality.
This is because growth has to work harder to move people
above the (absolute) poverty
line. The empirical evidence—old and new--bears this out at the
cross-country and micro
levels (Ferreira and Ravallion, 2008; Klasen, 2009; Geda et al.,
2009; Fosu, 2009;
Milanovic, 2011).
Moreover, much of the empirical evidence also seems to support
the proposition that
higher initial inequality slows down subsequent growth
(Easterly, 2007; Bluhm and
Equity: Not Only for Idealists 7
© The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015
Overseas Development Institute.
Development Policy Review 33 (1)
Szirmai, 2011; Klasen, 2009; Ortiz and Cummins, 2011) and
reduces the duration of
growth spells (Berg and Ostry, 2011). In the Ortiz and Cummins
study data from a diverse
set of 131 countries show that the countries with higher levels
of inequality experienced
slower annual per capita GDP growth over the past 20 years. An
IMF staff study (Berg and
Ostry, 2011) found that a 10 percentile decrease in inequality
increases the expected length
of an economic growth period by 50%. However, this
inequality-growth literature is fragile.
For example, Barro (2000) finds evidence to support a Kuznets
relationship: higher
inequality tends to retard growth in poor countries and
encourage growth in richer places,
while from her cross-country regression work Forbes (2000)
finds that increased income
inequality has a significant positive relationship with
subsequent economic growth. Duflo
(2011) describes how unstable the cross-country studies are in
this space, with substantial
variation in conclusions depending on countries and time
periods selected, lags and non-
linearities employed and identification approaches tried.
3 Growing disparities today are storing up trouble for the future
Recent studies are adding to the idea that inequalities set up
additional problems for current
and future generations – beyond slower poverty reduction and
economic growth. First, they
seem to lead to weaker economic and political institutions
(Savoia et al., 2010; Bluhm and
Szirmai, 2011). These institutions, such as property rights,
market regulation and civil and
political rights, are vital to ignite and sustain growth and to
hold societies together.
Second, high levels of economic inequality between different
groups and types of
individuals matter greatly. These horizontal inequalities
between ethnic and political groups
are econometrically linked to the onset of civil war (Østby,
2008; Cederman et al., 2011;
Stewart, 2010), and we know that conflict sets back
development by decades (World Bank,
2011). Political instability is a disincentive for foreign
investment and thus significantly
undermines a nation’s growth potential (WEF, 2012). Indeed,
many in the private sector
seem increasingly concerned about inequity as a barrier to
growth. In a recent survey of 469
experts and industry leaders by the World Economic Forum,
inequality was identified as
one of the greatest risks to global growth and stability (ibid.).
Inequalities also occur by gender. Point estimates suggest that
between 0.4% and
0.9% of the differences in growth rates between East Asia and
sub-Saharan Africa, South
Asia and the Middle East can be accounted for by the larger
gender gaps in education
prevailing in the latter regions (Klasen, 2009).
Third, inequalities suffered in early childhood play out in
adulthood and across
generations via nutrition and education deficits (Hoddinott et
al., 2013; Heckman, 2011).
For example, Hoddinott et al. (2011) find that the prevention of
stunting at 36 months of
age, other things held constant, raises per capita consumption in
adulthood by 66%;
inequality in early life has huge consequences in later life.
Moreover, we know that stunted
mothers are more likely to have stunted babies (Andersson and
Bergstrom, 2003). For both
rich and poor countries, an increase in learning achievement (as
measured by test scores
and delivered by improvements in nutrition status and the
quality of schooling) of one
standard deviation is associated on average with an increase in
the long-run growth rate of
around 2% per capita annually (Hanushek and Woessmann, 2008
).
8 Lawrence Haddad
© The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015
Overseas Development Institute.
Development Policy Review 33 (1)
4 The policy agenda
With the evidence mounting that inequality reduces growth and
its poverty-reducing
potential, weakens institutions and undermines political and
social cohesion, what should
the policy response be?
The level of inequality in a society is a reflection, in part, of its
history and culture and
as such is embedded in its politics, electoral or otherwise.
Those denied inclusion,
participation and social mobility, together with those who
support a more inclusive society,
need to build medium-run momentum to change the tolerance of
inequality and the
preference for greater equity. Sometimes a lack of evidence
should not be an excuse to do
nothing when values dictate otherwise. In the short run, the
policy levers are many:
addressing unequal opportunity in early life; correcting
inequality of public provision in
services or infrastructure; using taxes to redistribute income by
progressive taxation; using
transfers to increase the incomes of those vulnerable to poverty,
including old-age pensions;
and the redistribution of productive assets, including land.
This terrain is too big to cover here. Our approach therefore is
to (i) highlight one
particular approach suggested by some new research and (ii) to
outline the contours of
policy options around the what, how, why and who of any
choices made.
4.1 Focusing on the most deprived
Much public policy is focused on achieving the highest
benefit:cost ratios that can be
derived from a set of instruments designed to tackle a given
problem. But the pressure to
reach national targets on health, education and poverty can
narrow the focus of efforts to
the people and areas easiest to reach rather than those who are
most disadvantaged (Besley
and Kanbur, 1993).
We know intrinsically that, given equal costs of action, the
marginal returns to
development tend to be greatest when investments are made in
those with the least. For
example, the same vaccination programme in an area of
widespread disease saves more
lives than in an area less afflicted. The greatest boost to
learning will come from the
opening of a good school in an area where there is none. The
greatest impact of a new
nutrition centre will be in an area where there are no nutrition
services because the area is
excluded on the grounds of, say, ethnicity. The question is
whether these greater impacts
can be realised in practice and whether they outweigh the
additional cost of working in the
areas that are hardest to reach.
The answer seems to be positive. The increase in returns from
focusing on the poorest
communities more than offsets the additional costs. According
to a recent 14-country
analysis (Carrera et al., 2012), an equity-focused approach is
actually more cost-effective,
at least in reducing avoidable deaths of young children. The
study reports:
… an equity-focused approach that prioritises the most deprived
communities,
and a mainstream approach that is representative of current
strategies. We
combined some existing models, notably the Marginal
Budgeting for Bottlenecks
Toolkit and the Lives Saved Tool, to do our analysis. We
showed that, with the
same level of investment, disproportionately higher effects are
possible by
Equity: Not Only for Idealists 9
© The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015
Overseas Development Institute.
Development Policy Review 33 (1)
prioritising the poorest and most marginalised populations, for
averting both
child mortality and stunting. (ibid.: 1341)
The equity-focused approach was more concerned with reaching
the poorest, via
community health workers, with appropriate incentives for
frontline staff, more targeted
cash transfers and infrastructure rehabilitation to connect the
poorest districts. For the more
equity-focused approach, a $1 million investment averted 244
cases of stunting, and the
current approach, with the same budget, averted 84 cases. These
results may well
strengthen in the future as the costs of delivering services to
those most excluded are
reduced through the use of new information and logistic
technologies (HHI, 2011).
4.2 Navigating the policy terrain
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx
C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx

Contenu connexe

Similaire à C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx

DISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docx
DISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docxDISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docx
DISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docxbkbk37
 
REMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docx
REMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docxREMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docx
REMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docxaudeleypearl
 
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docxAssignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docxbraycarissa250
 
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docxAssignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docxhoward4little59962
 
Porque determinantes sociales oct 2010 ok
Porque determinantes sociales oct 2010 okPorque determinantes sociales oct 2010 ok
Porque determinantes sociales oct 2010 okRoger Zapata
 
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United StatesEstimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United StatesJim Bloyd, DrPH, MPH
 
Health Disparities Essay
Health Disparities EssayHealth Disparities Essay
Health Disparities EssayMelissa Luster
 
Health and health care inequalities Name
Health and health care inequalities NameHealth and health care inequalities Name
Health and health care inequalities NameJeanmarieColbert3
 
Commentary _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questions
Commentary  _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questionsCommentary  _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questions
Commentary _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questionsBsie
 
Fielding deterhlth
Fielding deterhlthFielding deterhlth
Fielding deterhlthAnita P
 
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docxASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docxlesleyryder69361
 
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docxASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docxbraycarissa250
 
Justicia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la salud
Justicia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la saludJusticia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la salud
Justicia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la salud02678923
 
The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...
The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...
The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...Chelsea Dade, MS
 
Velasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews (2016) 3731 .docx
Velasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews  (2016) 3731 .docxVelasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews  (2016) 3731 .docx
Velasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews (2016) 3731 .docxjessiehampson
 
httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &
httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &
httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &PazSilviapm
 
Research methods final project
Research methods final projectResearch methods final project
Research methods final projectHayoung Cho
 
LuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_Assignment
LuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_AssignmentLuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_Assignment
LuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_AssignmentLucious Davis
 

Similaire à C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx (20)

DISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docx
DISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docxDISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docx
DISCUSSION BOARD forum 3.docx
 
REMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docx
REMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docxREMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docx
REMOTEAREAMEDICALCan disparities be deadlyControv.docx
 
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docxAssignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
 
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docxAssignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
Assignment OverviewCyber is a relatively new word that did not .docx
 
Porque determinantes sociales oct 2010 ok
Porque determinantes sociales oct 2010 okPorque determinantes sociales oct 2010 ok
Porque determinantes sociales oct 2010 ok
 
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United StatesEstimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
 
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United StatesEstimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
Estimated Deaths Attributable to Social Factors in the United States
 
Health Disparities Essay
Health Disparities EssayHealth Disparities Essay
Health Disparities Essay
 
Health and health care inequalities Name
Health and health care inequalities NameHealth and health care inequalities Name
Health and health care inequalities Name
 
Commentary _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questions
Commentary  _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questionsCommentary  _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questions
Commentary _social_epidemiology__questionable answers and answerable questions
 
Fielding deterhlth
Fielding deterhlthFielding deterhlth
Fielding deterhlth
 
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docxASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
 
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docxASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
ASSIGNMENT COVER SHEET Course NameINTRODUCTION TO HOS.docx
 
Justicia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la salud
Justicia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la saludJusticia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la salud
Justicia social, epidemiologya e inequidad en la salud
 
The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...
The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...
The U.S Healthcare System, African Americans and the Notion of Toughing It Ou...
 
Velasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews (2016) 3731 .docx
Velasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews  (2016) 3731 .docxVelasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews  (2016) 3731 .docx
Velasco-Mondragon et al. Public Health Reviews (2016) 3731 .docx
 
semo2036
semo2036semo2036
semo2036
 
httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &
httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &
httpsdoi.org10.11771090198120922942Health Education &
 
Research methods final project
Research methods final projectResearch methods final project
Research methods final project
 
LuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_Assignment
LuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_AssignmentLuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_Assignment
LuciousDavis1-Research Methods for Health Sciences-01-Unit9_Assignment
 

Plus de humphrieskalyn

Evaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docx
Evaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docxEvaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docx
Evaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docx
Evaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docxEvaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docx
Evaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docx
Evaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docxEvaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docx
Evaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docx
Evaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docxEvaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docx
Evaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docx
Evaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docxEvaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docx
Evaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docx
Evaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docxEvaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docx
Evaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
EV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docx
EV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docxEV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docx
EV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docx
Evaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docxEvaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docx
Evaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docx
Evaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docxEvaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docx
Evaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docx
Evaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docxEvaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docx
Evaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docx
Evaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docxEvaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docx
Evaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docx
Evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docxEvaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docx
Evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docx
Evaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docxEvaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docx
Evaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docx
Evaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docxEvaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docx
Evaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docx
Evaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docxEvaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docx
Evaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docx
Evaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docxEvaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docx
Evaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docx
Evaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docxEvaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docx
Evaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docx
Evaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docxEvaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docx
Evaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docx
Evaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docxEvaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docx
Evaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
Evaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docx
Evaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docxEvaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docx
Evaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docxhumphrieskalyn
 

Plus de humphrieskalyn (20)

Evaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docx
Evaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docxEvaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docx
Evaluate the role of leadership on organizational behaviorProv.docx
 
Evaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docx
Evaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docxEvaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docx
Evaluate the role that PKI plays in cryptography.Ensure that you.docx
 
Evaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docx
Evaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docxEvaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docx
Evaluate the presence and effects of alteration in the homeostatic s.docx
 
Evaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docx
Evaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docxEvaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docx
Evaluate the role of a digital certificate in cryptography.  How doe.docx
 
Evaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docx
Evaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docxEvaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docx
Evaluate the merits of Piaget’s stage theory for explaining cognitiv.docx
 
Evaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docx
Evaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docxEvaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docx
Evaluate the notion that white collar offenders are intrinsically di.docx
 
EV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docx
EV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docxEV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docx
EV 551 Hazardous Materials Assessment – Summer2020Homework 1 – 4.docx
 
Evaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docx
Evaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docxEvaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docx
Evaluate the history of cryptography from its origins.  Analyze how .docx
 
Evaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docx
Evaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docxEvaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docx
Evaluate the evidence provided by Apollo Shoes.Decide how to s.docx
 
Evaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docx
Evaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docxEvaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docx
Evaluate the Health History and Medical Information for Mrs. J.,.docx
 
Evaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docx
Evaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docxEvaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docx
Evaluate the current state of the health care system in Sacramento. .docx
 
Evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docx
Evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docxEvaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docx
Evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of the various decis.docx
 
Evaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docx
Evaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docxEvaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docx
Evaluate some technologies that can help with continuous monitoring..docx
 
Evaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docx
Evaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docxEvaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docx
Evaluate progress on certification plansReport your prog.docx
 
Evaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docx
Evaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docxEvaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docx
Evaluate how you have achieved course competencies and your plans to.docx
 
Evaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docx
Evaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docxEvaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docx
Evaluate how information privacy and security relates to the Interne.docx
 
Evaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docx
Evaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docxEvaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docx
Evaluate assessment of suicide in forensic settings andor cri.docx
 
Evaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docx
Evaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docxEvaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docx
Evaluate different approaches to ethical decision making. Then, choo.docx
 
Evaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docx
Evaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docxEvaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docx
Evaluate and grade websites in terms of their compliance with PL pri.docx
 
Evaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docx
Evaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docxEvaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docx
Evaluate at least (2) factors that make financial statement analys.docx
 

Dernier

How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSCeline George
 
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptxDyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptxcallscotland1987
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.MaryamAhmad92
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...ZurliaSoop
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxnegromaestrong
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesCeline George
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxAreebaZafar22
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxAmanpreet Kaur
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Jisc
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and ModificationsMJDuyan
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Association for Project Management
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsMebane Rash
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...pradhanghanshyam7136
 

Dernier (20)

How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
 
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptxDyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
 
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
Spatium Project Simulation student brief
Spatium Project Simulation student briefSpatium Project Simulation student brief
Spatium Project Simulation student brief
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 

C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A .docx

  • 1. C U L T U R E A S A M E D I A T O R O F H E A L T H D I S P A R I T I E S : C U L T U R A L C O N S O N A N C E , S O C I A L C L A S S , A N D H E A L T H William W. Dressler University of Alabama Mauro C. Balieiro Paulista University Rosane P. Ribeiro University of São Paulo-Ribeirão Preto José Ernesto dos Santos University of São Paulo-Ribeirão Preto Health disparities or health inequalities refer to enduring differences between population groups in health status, well-being, and mortality. Health inequalities have been described by race, ethnic group, gender, and social class. A variety of theories have been proposed to account for health inequalities, including access to medical care and absolute material deprivation. Several theorists (including Michael Marmot and Richard Wilkinson) have argued that relative deprivation is the primary factor. By this they mean the inability of
  • 2. individuals to achieve the kind of lifestyle that is valued and considered normative in their social context. In this article, we show that the concept and measurement of cultural consonance can operationalize what Marmot and Wilkinson mean by relative deprivation. Cultural consonance is the degree to which individuals approximate, in their own beliefs and behaviors, the prototypes for belief and behavior encoded in shared cultural models. Widely shared cultural models in society describe what is regarded both as appropriate and desirable in many different domains. These cultural models are both directive and motivating: people try to achieve the goals defined in these models; however, as a result of both social and economic constraints, some individuals are unable to effectively incorporate these cultural goals into their own lives. The result is an enduring loss of coherence in life, because life is not unfolding in the way that it, culturally speaking, “should.” The resulting chronic stress is associated with psychobiological distress. We illustrate this process with data collected in urban Brazil. A theory of cultural consonance provides a uniquely biocultural contribution to the understanding of health inequalities. [cultural consonance, cultural consensus, relative deprivation, Brazil] The Black Report (named for the chairman, Sir Douglas Black, of the commission that released it) was issued in Britain in 1980 (Black and Townsend 1982). It summarized the findings of a commission whose charge was to examine available health statistics and
  • 3. ANNALS OF ANTHROPOLOGICAL PRACTICE 38.2, pp. 214– 231. ISSN: 2153-957X. C© 2015 by the American Anthropological Association. DOI:10.1111/napa.12053 2 1 4 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s determine if there were detectable and reliable inequalities in health in Britain. By this they meant systematic and enduring health differences between identifiable population groups. The commission found inequalities in health by age, gender, race, ethnicity, and, especially, social class. These inequalities are now a focus of research in all parts of the world, although here in the United States we have seen fit to sanitize the topic under the rubric health disparities. As noted on the website for Healthy People 2020, the official policy blueprint for improving public health in the United States: If a health outcome is seen in a greater or lesser extent between populations, there is disparity. Race or ethnicity, sex, sexual identity, age, disability, socioeconomic status, and geographic location all contribute to an individual’s ability to achieve good health. (Healthy People 2020, n.d.). The question driving research is, what accounts for these disparities? Any number of
  • 4. structural and material factors could be relevant, as well as behavioral factors, including diet, physical activity, and health behaviors (e.g., smoking, drinking). There are also potential social selection processes, that is, individuals may assume a lower socioeconomic status as a result of their poor health, rather than their lower socioeconomic status leading to poor health. While all of these explanations have some merit, they still, in the final analysis, do not account for health disparities (Marmot 2004). The major theorists in the area, notably Michael Marmot (2004), and Richard Wilkin- son and Kate Pickett (2011), emphasize instead a psychosocial stress hypothesis. They argue that individuals in disadvantaged groups are deprived of meaningful participation in the wider society and that the stresses associated with that deprivation account for health disparities. We will argue here that, while there is considerable evidence in support of this position, the evidence is not as strong as it could be, primarily because current models of health disparities fail to take culture and biocultural interactions into account. A biocultural approach in anthropology is uniquely situated to contribute to the study of health disparities precisely because it takes the concept of culture seriously, and is thus able to link the individual to collective representations of what a meaningful life is (Dressler 2005). The utility of this approach will be illustrated with data collected over the past 20
  • 5. years in urban Brazil. S O C I O E C O N O M I C D I S P A R I T I E S I N H E A L T H Arguably, the most important thinkers on the question of socioeconomic health dispar- ities are Michael Marmot (2004) and Richard Wilkinson (1994; Wilkinson and Pickett 2006, 2007). Marmot is most well known for his direction of the Whitehall Studies. These prospective epidemiologic studies were designed to directly examine factors that accounted for the inverse association of social class and the risk of cardiovascular dis- ease. British civil servants served as the study population. This controlled for access to health care, given that all members of the civil service (referred to as Whitehall in British A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 1 5 vernacular) had access to high-quality care through the National Health Service and additional insurance. There was a clear social class hierarchy in Whitehall, formed by occupational categories. These included janitors and messengers at the bottom of the hierarchy, followed by clerical staff, then professional staff (statisticians, economists), and finally, at the top of the hierarchy, the administrative staff. These are individuals with
  • 6. elite British educations who help to set and direct policy. After controlling for heart disease risk factors (blood pressure, cholesterol, and others), the lowest social class group was 50 percent more likely to die from heart disease than the highest social class group over a 25-year follow-up; furthermore, there was a gradient of increasing risk from the administrators down to the janitors, with no sharp break in the pattern. This is a particularly important part of the findings, because this was a study population in which no one could be considered “poor” in the sense that they lacked access to basic material resources for maintaining life. Rather, what is striking about the results in this and many other studies is that there is a continuous gradient, such that even doctoral level economists are at a slightly higher risk of mortality than the administrative staff who outrank them (Marmot 2004:60). Much effort was invested in testing alternative hypotheses, such as diet, smoking, or other medical conditions, that could explain the gradient. Since nothing could explain the gradient, Marmot labeled it “The Status Syndrome” (2004). The main thrust of his argument is that increasing social status enables individuals to exercise greater autonomy in their lives, and this enhances their ability to live the life they value. Conversely, lower social status blocks these capabilities, resulting in long- term, chronic stress, and an increased risk of disease.
  • 7. Richard Wilkinson (1994) is known for his studies of income inequality and health. Income inequality refers not to the differences in wealth or income between individuals, but rather to the entire range of socioeconomic variation within a system (community, U.S. state, or entire nation). It assesses the degree of inequality at the group level. As such, it is an “integral aggregate variable,” or a variable that refers only to aggregates as units of observation. The Gini coefficient is a common way to measure income inequality. If everyone in a community had the same amount of money, the Gini coefficient would be equal to 0.0; if only one person in the community had all the money, the Gini coefficient would be equal to 1.0. Wilkinson (1994) first explored the association of income inequality and life expectancy in Western European societies, finding that as income inequality increased, life expectancy declined. This was striking, given that these are the world’s most affluent nations. It was not the affluence, however, that was at issue, but rather how that affluence was distributed. There have been numerous demonstrations of the association between income in- equality and a variety of health, behavioral, and psychological outcomes (summarized in Wilkinson and Pickett 2011). For example, here in the United States, using states as units of analysis, the correlation between the state-level Gini coefficient and aggregate health outcomes are as follows: with overall mortality, r = .403 (p < .01); with homicide rates,
  • 8. r = .695 (p < .01); and, with mortality from coronary heart disease, r = .521 (p < .01; source: author’s data). These correlations are large and impressive in part because they 2 1 6 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s are based on aggregate data (hence, noise in the data gets averaged out); nevertheless, it is clear that income inequality is a potent correlate of our collective well-being. In considering these findings, Wilkinson asks if it is an absolute (sometimes simply referred to as material) deprivation or a relative deprivation that is important. While these terms will be discussed in greater detail below, the primary distinction is between lacking the basic resources of food and shelter necessary for survival—or absolute/material deprivation—versus lacking what is considered to be customary in a given society— or relative deprivation. Wilkinson (Wilkinson and Pickett 2007) favors the relative deprivation argument, since so much of his work has dealt with social units that are not only affluent (Western European nations), but are also functioning welfare states. Marmot (2004:118) also invokes the concept in his explanation of the social gradient. The concept of relative deprivation has a long history in social thought, and a consideration
  • 9. of some of that history will be useful for the argument being constructed here. D E P R I V A T I O N : A B S O L U T E A N D R E L A T I V E Poverty, its nature and effects, has been the focus of social scientific inquiry since at least the 19th century and Engels’ investigation of working class conditions in England (Engels 1958[1845]). At the turn of the century, Rowntree and other pioneering investigators in Britain attempted to objectively measure levels of poverty by estimating the nutritional needs of working families, and then estimating the amount of money required to fulfill both those nutritional needs and additional needs for clothing, heating, and household sundries (Townsend 1979:32–33). Current definitions of poverty in the United States are based on this approach (Weinberg 1995). In a real sense, these estimates of poverty levels are based on a biological reductionist assumption: that human well-being is to be measured solely in terms of the minimum physiological requirements for growth, development, resistance to disease, and work capacity. As such, this approach to poverty ignores socially or culturally defined needs. Townsend (1979), in his monumental Poverty in the United Kingdom, argued that this is a flawed approach to the definition of poverty for a variety of reasons. He noted the technical difficulties in estimating the nutritional requirements
  • 10. of individuals, as well as in determining the disposable income that is available to a household (Townsend 1979:32–39). But the primary flaw in this approach is the extent to which it ignores consensual social definitions of appropriate lifestyles. As Townsend put it: Individuals, families and groups in the population can be said to be in poverty when they lack the resources to obtain the types of diet, participate in the activities and have the living conditions and amenities which are customary, or are at least widely encouraged or approved, in the societies to which they belong. Their resources are so seriously below those commanded by the average individual or family that they are, in effect, excluded from ordinary living patterns, customs and activities (Townsend 1979:31, emphasis added). What Townsend suggested was that poverty should be understood as the degree of relative deprivation experienced by individuals and groups. He distinguished three A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 1 7 forms of deprivation: (1) objective deprivation, or the extent to which individuals are deprived of the material conditions of life; (2) conventionally acknowledged deprivation,
  • 11. or the extent to which persons are deprived of conditions socially defined as necessary or appropriate (see quote above); and (3) subjective deprivation, meaning the extent to which individuals feel themselves to be deprived (Townsend 1979:49). He regarded the second sense of the term relative deprivation to be the most important and useful, since it assesses the extent to which individuals are prevented from acting upon the social and cultural norms of their own group. Townsend was not alone in regarding this normative sense of relative deprivation to be essential in understanding patterns of poverty (e.g., Bell 1995). There is, however, one difficulty that always arises in attempts to understand poverty in this sense. What are the “ordinary living patterns, customs, and activities” from which persons are excluded due to a lack of resources? How are such customs to be determined? It is this issue that has led many to adhere to a minimum income definition of poverty. As Weinberg notes in his rejection of relative deprivation: “Minimal consumption standards for all necessary commodities could in theory be established . . . but doing so would raise difficult ethical issues about which commodities to include (e.g., is a telephone a necessity?)” (Weinberg 1995:6, emphasis added). Townsend (1979:38) suggested that, as much as possible, the definition of relative deprivation should be based on independent or external criteria. Townsend argued that
  • 12. “style of life” is a sociocultural dimension by which deprivation could be assessed. Style of life includes the acquisition of basic consumer goods as well the awareness of culturally valued knowledge and the participation in culturally valued social activities. Since at least the work of Veblen (1918), lifestyle in this sense has been seen as a major component of social judgments regarding social worth or prestige, so much so that Veblen’s term conspicuous consumption entered the vernacular. In recent years, however, investigators have argued that conspicuous consumption is only one aspect, perhaps relatively small, of the social meaning of consumption activities (Belk 1988; Bourdieu 1984; Douglas and Isherwood 1979; McCracken 1988). While recognizing the push of consumerism, this view sees style of life as a broadly patterned activity expressive of more than what Townsend (1979:58) referred to as “supercilious and derogatory distinctions.” Rather, style of life as a dimension of social life expresses and reinforces in a concrete way a sense of belonging to and integration into a social group. And where an individual’s or family’s lack of consonance with the community is evident materially, to be demonstrated day in and day out, the sense of loss or failure may be profound. In his empirical work in Britain, Townsend developed a lengthy inventory that in- cluded items dealing with material consumption (e.g., owning a television, refrigerator, and other similar items) and with social behaviors (e.g., being able to go out for a meal
  • 13. periodically). No justification for the inclusion of items was given other than the rea- soned judgment of the investigator. A scale of deprivation was then developed in which an individual or household received one point for each item they lacked. The way in which this scale of relative deprivation covaried with other, more conventional indicators of poverty was then examined. 2 1 8 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s Later investigators expanded upon this approach, especially using survey data to de- fine what a representative sample of the population defined as customary and approved consumption (Hallerod 1996; Mack and Lansley 1985). While these are reasonable ap- proaches to the study of relative deprivation, they also tend to be arbitrary in choosing the cutoff point for what is thought to be important (e.g., 51 percent of the sample must say an item is important?). These approaches to the definition of consensual lifestyles rely solely on the statistical aggregation of individual responses (Shore 1991:11). There is no independent model that enables the investigators: (1) to test for the existence of a shared model of lifestyles; (2) to estimate the degree of sharing of the model; or (3) to estimate the content of that model.
  • 14. Furthermore, why limit the investigation of relative deprivation to the specific domain of lifestyle? While Veblen’s theory makes clear the importance of this domain, why would other domains not be important as well? Indeed, Marmot’s (2004) argument suggests a broader range of aspirations that might be considered. It would be useful to have a theory regarding how normative judgments might be structured within a society, and a related set of methodological procedures to assess the distribution of normative judgments and normative behaviors within a society. In this article, innovations in culture theory (D’Andrade, 1984, 1995) and ethnographic methods (Romney et al. 1986) are used that may provide the study of relative deprivation a more substantial theoretical and methodological foundation, and in turn contribute to an understanding of socioeconomic health disparities (Kawachi and Kennedy 1999). The cultural consensus model (Romney et al. 1986) and the related theory and measurement of cultural consonance provide a more solid conceptual and operational foundation for examining these processes. C U L T U R A L C O N S E N S U S A N D C U L T U R A L C O N S O N A N C E The foundation for the approach to be outlined here rests on a cognitive theory of culture. This theory starts with Goodenough’s (1996) definition of culture: that which one must know to function adequately in a given social setting.
  • 15. This knowledge includes procedural understandings of how to do and make things, social understandings of how to interact appropriately with others, and the understanding of the world that underlies belief and opinion. This knowledge is learned both through individual experience, resulting in idiosyn- cratic understandings, and through systematic interaction with others and socialization, resulting in shared understandings. Shared knowledge, or culture, is cognitively encoded in the form of cultural models: skeletal, stripped-down representations of some cultural domain (e.g., lifestyle, the family), including the elements that make up that domain and processes that link the elements. Each domain contains at least one prototype, or best exemplar, of the domain. While all cultural models will be conditioned by individual biography, it is the sharing of cultural models that makes all social life possible. Cultural models vary in the degree to which they are shared. The sharing of a cultural model can be verified and quantified using the cultural consensus model (Romney A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 1 9 et al. 1986; Weller 2007). The cultural consensus model
  • 16. examines the degree to which the similarity in individuals’ responses to a standardized set of questions about a domain (e.g., “How important is it to own a house in order to live a good life”? This question is repeated for multiple items populating the domain of lifestyle.) can be estimated by positing an underlying shared model of “culturally correct” responses. Note that the investigator does not know what is culturally correct; rather, what is culturally correct is estimated from the degree to which individuals agree among themselves in their responses. Using a factor analysis of the similarities in response among individuals across a given knowledge base, cultural consensus analysis generates three estimates: (1) the overall degree of sharing, calculated from the ratio of the first-to- second eigenvalue of a factor analysis of persons (or Q-factor analysis); (2) the degree to which each individual shares in the knowledge base, referred to as cultural competence, estimated by the individual’s loading on the first unrotated factor; and (3) the estimated culturally correct responses, calculated as the weighted average of the responses of individuals, giving higher weight to individuals who are agreed with more strongly by others. A cognitive theory of culture, operationalized using the cultural consensus model, provides a theoretically and methodologically satisfying way of systematically identifying for a group of people features of life and living “ . . . which are customary, or are at
  • 17. least widely encouraged or approved, in the societies to which they belong” (Townsend 1979:31). As Bourdieu (1984) has repeatedly reminded us, however, people do not merely think things, they do things as well. This is the importance of the concept of cultural consonance, defined as the degree to which individuals approximate, in their own beliefs and behaviors, the prototypes for belief and behavior encoded in shared cultural models. Using a measure of cultural consonance, the degree to which individuals are able to put into practice their shared understanding within a cultural domain can be evaluated. Why would people not be culturally consonant? The first and most obvious answer is that they choose to be different. This is probably more rare than it might seem, especially for a cultural domain in which there is wide agreement. The second reason that individuals will have low cultural consonance in a domain is that “life chances,” to borrow Weber’s term, are stacked against them. They do not have the resources—principally economic, but also including racial and gender inequalities— to put into motion the understanding of how life is to be lived that they share with their neighbors. Individuals with low cultural consonance suffer a relative deprivation, when cultural consonance is measured in terms of broadly shared life goals. A rough sketch of how
  • 18. socioeconomic health disparities are mediated by cultural consonance is as follows: (1) there are widely shared cultural domains that define “goals in life,” ways of living to which individuals aspire across the lifespan; (2) being able to achieve cultural consonance within a domain requires a variety of personal resources, especially socioeconomic resources; (3) when these resources are lacking, individuals are unable to achieve cultural consonance; (4) they see themselves, and are seen by others, not to have achieved widely shared life goals; (5) this results in a low sense of coherence, or the feeling that life has not worked out 2 2 0 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s the way it is supposed to; (6) this also leads to unsatisfying mundane social interactions in which low cultural consonance individuals are treated with a lack of respect because they embody this status; (7) repeated arousal of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis and the sympathetic nervous system, as a result of unsatisfying social interaction and a low sense of coherence, lead to higher allostatic load; and (8) higher allostatic load over a lifetime results in poor health. We have examined this process in research in urban Brazil over the past 20 years, testing key parts of this hypothesized causal chain.
  • 19. Furthermore, combining data from two time periods (1991 and 2001), we can examine the association of cultural consonance with health outcomes (blood pressure and perceived stress), relative to the socioeconomic gradient and relative to income inequality. C U L T U R A L C O N S O N A N C E A N D H E A L T H I N U R B A N B R A Z I L We have carried out research in the city of Ribeirão Preto, Brazil, over nearly 30 years, with the past 20 years focusing on cultural consonance and health. Specifically, we collected data in 1991 (Dressler et al. 1997, 1998), in 2001 (Dressler, Balieiro et al. 2005; Dressler et al. 2007a), and we are currently collecting data in a study initiated in 2011 (Dressler et al. 2015). Research on cultural consonance requires a two- stage method in which the cultural models for domains in which cultural consonance is to be measured are investigated first using ethnographic methods, and then social survey research is carried out using measures of cultural consonance derived from the first stage. Both stages were carried out in 1991 and 2001; in the 2011 study, we have completed the cultural modeling stage and are engaged in collecting social survey data. The Research Site Ribeirão Preto is a city of 600,000 people in the north of the state of São Paulo. Located in a rich agricultural region, it is a center for light manufacturing and financial services
  • 20. related to the cultivation of sugar, coffee, citrus, and soy. It is also a regional leader in education and health care. It is known as unusually affluent. Despite, or because of, its affluence, Ribeirão Preto exhibits the differences between rich and poor that characterize all Brazilian communities. In the 1990s the Gini coefficient for Brazil exceeded 0.60, ranking it as one of the most unequal advanced industrial nations. Since then, the Gini coefficient has declined to nearly 0.50. This diminishing inequality is a function of a variety of factors, including both the stabilization of the currency under the government of Fernando Henrique Cardoso, and aggressive programs to deal with poverty under the governments of Luiz Inácio Lula da Silva and Dilma Rouseff. All research in Brazil must take social inequality into account. In our research we have done this in two ways. When collecting ethnographic data, care has been taken to interview respondents distributed by educational level as a proxy for social class. For our survey data, we collected random samples stratified by neighborhood. Four neigh- borhoods were selected to represent socioeconomic differences in Ribeirão Preto. The first began its life as a classic favela, or squatter settlement. In late 1992, the municipality A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 2 1
  • 21. built a small conjunto (housing area or subdivision) of two- room cinder block houses and residents of the favela were moved there. Favelados paid rent to the municipality on a rent-to-own basis. Many favelados, due to their unstable employment, were unable to maintain these payments and left for other favelas in the area. This enabled persons of slightly higher means, who could amass the capital to purchase these abandoned houses from the city, to move into this neighborhood. Today, it is a lower-class area of the city, with fewer than half the residents from the original favela. Residents tend to work as unskilled laborers and domestic servants. The second neighborhood is a classic conjunto habitacional. These are subdivisions that are built in partnership between the municipality and a contractor. The neighborhood started as uniform four-room cinder-block houses, but quickly transformed as residents added rooms, garden walls, and even second stories. Over the years it has developed its own commercial district with a supermarket, pharmacies, retail stores, and bars. Residents are employed in varied occupations, including school teachers, nurses aids, store clerks, and other lower-level professions (e.g., computer technician). The third neighborhood is an old, traditional, middle-class area that dates to the founding of Ribeirão Preto. Built in a European style, houses present seamless walls to cobbled streets. Older residents remember the time when
  • 22. families would spend the evenings sitting outside on the sidewalk, exchanging news and gossip with neighbors. The neighborhood boasts its own central praça or “plaza” fronting the Catholic Church, and there is a large and vibrant business section that rivals the city center. Residents tend to work as lower-level professionals (teachers, nurses), to own their own small businesses, or to work as managers in local businesses. The fourth neighborhood is a housing area adjacent to a university. Many residents are university professors, but many are also in the professions (physicians, attorneys) or the owners of large businesses and factories. The homes in the neighborhood tend to be quite large and spacious, with large and well-tended gardens. Cultural Models and Cultural Consensus Our research has evolved over the years in the study of cultural models and cultural consensus. In the 1991 study, we used cultural consensus principally to confirm the shared ideas around the cultural domains of lifestyle and social support that had been selected for study because of their theoretical importance as predictors of health status (Dressler et al. 1996, 1997; Dressler and Santos 2000). In 2001, we used systematic techniques of cultural domain analysis (Borgatti 1994) to identify and explore four cultural domains: lifestyle, social support, family life, and national identity (Dressler, Borges et al. 2005). In our recent study, begun in 2011, we have examined the same
  • 23. four domains from 2001, specifically to determine how cultural consensus changes or remains stable (Dressler et al. 2015). The domain of lifestyle refers to material goods (such as owning a home, a car, having Internet access) and leisure activities (being with friends, joining a sports club, shopping) that are seen as necessary for living a good life. Social support is the ability to call on specific sources of support (family, friends, coworkers, church members, specialists) in times of 2 2 2 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s T A B L E 1 . Cultural Consensus in Various Cultural Domains in Three Studies Year of Study Cultural Domain 1991 2001 2011 Lifestyle Eigenvalue ratio 5.55 6.59 7.70 Mean competence 0.65 (±.15) .71 (±.12) .72 (± .11) Social Support Eigenvalue ratio 3.11 6.53 5.21 Mean competence .61 (±.10) .67 (± .14) .65 (±.16) Family Life
  • 24. Eigenvalue ratio – 7.42 9.62 Mean competence .82 (±.09) .84 (±.09) National Identity Eigenvalue ratio – 3.97 3.50 Mean competence .57 (±.19) .61 (±.16) felt need (common problems ranging from needing a ride to psychological difficulties). Family life refers to the characteristics, including both structure and emotional bonds, that define a good Brazilian family. And, national identity organizes the characteristics that make Brazilians, Brazilian (see Dressler et al. 2004; Dressler, Balieiro, et al. 2005, 2007a, b; Dressler, Borges et al. 2005 for more detailed descriptions of these domains). For each domain, we focused on a single dimension of value or importance to test for cultural consensus; that is, do people rank elements of each domain as more or less important in ways consistent enough to infer that they are drawing on a shared cultural model in making their assessments? Table 1 summarizes data on cultural consensus for all three of these studies. In each study, in each domain, there is a broad cultural consensus that organizes people’s thinking. The level of cultural consensus varies by domain, but nevertheless, there is broad agreement within the community, through time, and based on different samples, of the importance of these elements within these domains. Furthermore, the cultural consensus displayed in Table 1 does not vary by
  • 25. socioeconomic status. The importance assigned to elements of each domain is equivalent across socioeconomic groups (Dressler et al. 2015; Dressler, Borges et al. 2005). The cultural domains that we have examined here can be grouped together under a larger meta-domain we have labeled “goals in life.” That is, these overlapping cultural domains describe ends to which individuals aspire as they pass through life stages. Cultural Consonance How effectively can people act on these shared understandings? This is the question of cultural consonance. As we have shown elsewhere, the degree to which individuals, in their own lives, actually match the profile of beliefs and behaviors that are collectively regarded as important in each domain can be measured in each domain (Dressler 1996, 2005; Dressler, Borges et al. 2005). And, we have found that low cultural consonance A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 2 3 is associated with higher blood pressure (Dressler et al. 1997, 1998, 2005), higher body mass (Dressler et al. 2008, 2012), immune system challenge (Dressler 2006), and higher psychological distress (Dressler et al. 2007a, b). Some outcomes
  • 26. are associated with cultural consonance in specific domains; most, however, are associated with generalized cultural consonance. This refers to the tendency for individuals to be consonant across multiple domains (Dressler et al. 2007a). The dilemma posed by low cultural consonance can be illustrated in Figure 1. In the graph in Panel A, cultural competence, averaged across four cultural domains, is shown in relation to educational attainment; there is no difference among the groups in average cultural competence (i.e., cultural knowledge). In Panel B, generalized cultural consonance is shown in relation to education level. These differences are highly significant. In other words, many people live in an environment of meaning in which they know what is valued and desired in life, but they are unable to achieve it in their own behaviors. Cultural Consonance, Economic Inequality, and Health With the data we have from Brazil, we can examine how cultural consonance mediates and moderates the association of economic inequality and health. Throughout this article, we have oscillated in the discussion of socioeconomic health disparities between the social gradient and conditions of income inequality. Using our 1991 and 2001 data together, we can examine the association of both these types of inequality and health, relative to cultural consonance. At the outset it should be emphasized that this is best regarded as
  • 27. an illustrative exercise rather than a definitive test of any hypotheses. While the data collection was guided by a single theoretical orientation, from one study to the next we were more concerned with refining and extending our theory and methods than with precise replication; however, we do have some data in common between studies that can serve for at least an exercise. Obviously, age (in years) and gender (coded as women = 0 and men = 1) are comparable, as is the body mass index (BMI), calculated from height (in meters) and weight (in kilograms). For a measure of socioeconomic status, we can use family income, collected in both studies as the number of minimum salaries coming into the household, and then converted to constant 2001 reais (the Brazilian currency). The study itself (coded as 1991 = 0 and 2001 = 1) can serve as a measure of changing income inequality, since the Gini coefficient for Brazil declined from about 0.60 in 1991 to 0.55 in 2001. There are data available to roughly measure cultural consonance in one cultural domain: lifestyle. In 1991, to assess cultural competence, we asked people to rate the importance of items as defining one as “a success in life.” In 2001, respondents rated items in terms of their importance “for having a good life.” There are 14 items in common between these two inventories. Furthermore, the two sets of ratings are correlated at r = .81. This justifies combining them as a measure of cultural consonance. To do so,
  • 28. we weighted each item by its 1991 rating of importance, and summed these for each individual. The higher the value of this scale, the more an individual approaches in his or her own life a lifestyle that is collectively valued. 2 2 4 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s F I G U R E 1 . Association of educational level with cultural consensus (Panel A) and cultural consonance (Panel B). A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 2 5 T A B L E 2 . Descriptive Statistics 1991 2001 Total sample (n = 304) (n = 271) (n = 575) Age* 38.5 ( ± 12.4) 40.9 ( ± 11.6) 39.6 ( ± 12.1) Sex (percentage of male) 40.5 39.1 39.8 BMI 24.5 ( ± 4.8) 25.2 ( ± 5.2) 24.8 ( ± 5.0) Family income 1,309.8 ( ± 650.5) 1,381.1 ( ± 596.3) 1,343.3 ( ± 626.0) Cultural consonance** 10.4 ( ± 3.4) 12.6 ( ± 2.5) 11.5 ( ± 3.2) Perceived stress** 11.1 ( ± 6.9) 9.3 ( ± 5.7) 10.2 ( ± 6.5) Systolic blood pressure 123.1 ( ± 17.7) 122.9 ( ± 16.4) 123.0 ( ± 16.9)
  • 29. Tests of differences between studies. *p < .01, **p < .001. Finally, with respect to health outcomes, we collected Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen et al. 1983) in 1991 and 2001. This is a ten-item scale of globally perceived stress that is widely used and assesses the degree to which individuals feel their lives are in control and predictable. It has acceptable internal consistency reliability in both studies (Cronbach’s α = .80 and .79, respectively). Also, we have blood pressure, measured using a DINAMAP Vital Signs Monitor 845XT. This is an automated blood pressure monitor that essentially removes observer error. In each study, it was regularly calibrated against a standard mercury sphygmomanometer. For ease of presentation, we will only use systolic blood pressure as an outcome measure. Descriptive data on these variables, for each study separately and the studies pooled, are shown in Table 2. The sample from 2001 is slightly older than 1991. There is no overall change in family income, although the group comparison obscures the fact that income increased significantly in the two lowest SES neighborhoods, leveled off in the third, and increased slightly in the fourth. Overall, cultural consonance in lifestyle increased from 1991 to 2001, and perceived stress decreased. There was no change in blood pressure, BMI, or gender distribution.
  • 30. Tables 3 and 4 present hierarchical multiple regression models for each dependent variable. In each analysis, age, sex, and family income (and BMI for blood pressure) are entered into the equation first. For both perceived stress and blood pressure, family income has an inverse association with the outcomes, confirming the social gradient. Next, study is entered into the equation as a dichotomous variable. The significant regression coefficient in Table 4 shows that perceived stress declined over the ten years between studies, while blood pressure did not. Next, cultural consonance in lifestyle is entered. There is an inverse association between cultural consonance and each outcome; furthermore, when cultural consonance is entered and controlled, the inverse effect of family income disappears. Finally, a term for the interaction between cultural consonance and study is entered. For blood pressure this is nonsignificant. For perceived stress it is significant, indicating that in the 2001 study, the size of the association between cultural consonance and perceived stress was smaller than in the 1991 study. 2 2 6 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s T A B L E 3 . Regression of Systolic Blood Pressure on Covariates, Cultural Consonance, Year of Study, and Interaction of Cultural Consonance × Year of Study
  • 31. (Standardized Regression Coefficients) Variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Age .353* .357* .359* .359* Sex .257* .256* .251* .251* BMI .238* .241* .248* .253* Family income −.165* −.162* −.051 −.019 Study −.051 .014 .007 Cultural consonance −.223* −.255* Cultural consonance × study .058 Multiple R .555* .557* .577* .579* Multiple R2 .308 .310 .333 .335 *p < .001. T A B L E 4 . Regression of Perceived Stress on Covariates, Cultural Consonance, Year of Study, and Interaction of Cultural Consonance × Year of Study (Standardized Regression Coefficients) Variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Age −.123* −.110* −.107* −.106* Sex −.199** −.202** −.210** −.211** Family income −.178** −.172** .010 −.022 Study −.124* −.041 −.062 Cultural consonance −.276** −.349** Cultural consonance × study .145* Multiple R .310** .333** .381** .398** Multiple R2 .096 .111 .145 .158
  • 32. *p < .01, **p < .001. D I S C U S S I O N Our aim in this article has been to explore the utility of a biocultural approach to the study of health disparities. Specifically, we have examined cultural consonance as a measure of relative deprivation in Townsend’s (1979) terms. Socioeconomic disparities were conceptualized and measured as a socioeconomic gradient of individuals and as time periods varying in level of income inequality. Results from our research in Brazil suggest that cultural consonance mediates the social gradient and is moderated by income inequality. The most straightforward results here come from the analysis of blood pressure. There is an inverse association between family income and systolic blood pressure; when cultural consonance is entered into the analysis, it absorbs all of the explanatory variance of family income. This is consistent with a simple linear path model: A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 2 7 F I G U R E 2 . Perceived stress by year of study and cultural consonance.
  • 33. Family income → Cultural consonance → Blood pressure. Borrowing a term from a statistical technique—path analysis— for testing causal models, family income is an exogenous variable. It stands at the beginning of the causal sequence and is, itself, causally unaccounted for. Through a variety of means (family of origin, educational opportunities, employment opportunities, inheritance, marriage) an individual is able to attain a particular income level. This in turn represents the resources that he or she can draw on and invest in achieving the widely shared life goals defined by a cultural consensus within the domains organizing those life goals and operationalized by the measure of cultural consonance. Again, borrowing from path analysis, cultural consonance is an endogenous variable, because it is causally accounted for by the exogenous variable, family income. Then, higher cultural consonance leads to lower blood pressure. Cultural consonance mediates the inverse relationship between position in the socioeconomic gradient and blood pressure. This description of the results is true also for perceived stress, except that the impact of cultural consonance on perceived stress is moderated by overall conditions of income inequality. As the overall level of inequality declines, the effect of cultural consonance on perceived stress weakens. The pattern of these results is shown in Figure 2. These results may indicate the differing effects that cultural
  • 34. consonance has for a vari- able that does not depend on the conscious reporting of some state by the respondent— blood pressure—versus an outcome that depends on that conscious reporting—perceived stress. As we argued earlier, the chronic stress associated with low cultural consonance ul- timately involves pathways via the hypothalamic-pituitary- adrenal axis and sympathetic nervous system arousal. The allostatic load associated with repeated arousals of these sys- tems can lead directly to a coarsening of the smooth muscle tissue surrounding arterioles and, ultimately, sustained higher blood pressure. The source of that chronic stress must of course be meaningful to the individual, and we are arguing that it is the collective meaning attached to these cultural domains that is important. In the case of perceived stress, all of the same processes are at work plus the conscious awareness and reporting of felt distress. It may be that under conditions of declining income inequality the experience of lower cultural consonance is less distressing because there is, at least, a sense of the potential for life to improve. In the case of Brazil specifically, 2 2 8 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s this may have been enhanced in 2001 by the election of Luis Inácio Lula da Silva, or, as
  • 35. he was popularly known, “Lula.” (We date the study as 2001, but survey research actually extended over the period of 2001–03, which encompasses Lula’s campaign for and election to the Brazilian presidency.) Lula represented the Partida dos Trabalhadores or Worker’s Party (abbreviated as PT). The PT campaign specifically focused on the need to help alleviate the suffering of the poorest segments of the Brazilian population, ultimately implementing programs such as Fome Zero (Zero Hunger) and Bolsa Famı́lia (Family Allowance). These programs provided various forms of direct and indirect assistance to poor families, often consolidating and adding to programs that had been initiated by previous administrations. The actual effectiveness of these social programs is not really the issue here; rather, Lula’s election and the promise of these programs, coupled with the measureable reduc- tion in income inequality (certainly initiated by the Plano Real, or currency stabilization program under the earlier administration of Fernando Henrique Cardoso), may have provided a different ethos for the poorer members of Brazilian society such that the conscious strain associated with low cultural consonance was less likely to be reported. There is, however, an alternative explanation for the moderating effect of lower income inequality. The measure of cultural consonance we are using here may not be sufficiently sensitive to assess this variable in 2001. With reduced
  • 36. inequality, achieving what is, in essence, a 1991 level of cultural consonance may, in 2001, be easy enough to restrict the range of variation of cultural consonance in 2001. This could spuriously produce the observed moderating effect. This is, nevertheless, an example of a useful approach to the study of socioeconomic health disparities in anthropology. Anthropologists have been strangely silent in the empirical study of the health effects of economic inequality, despite their vocal advocacy for the poor (Dressler 2010). We argue that a biocultural theory, explicitly derived from the integration of perspectives in cultural and biological anthropology, provides a productive avenue for better understanding socioeconomic health disparities. While the actual results must be interpreted with a certain caution, given that the data were not truly designed to test these hypotheses, the theory and method of cultural consonance appears to provide a means for refining our grasp of processes that generate socioeconomic health inequalities. Future research may profit from adopting the approach explicitly. N O T E Acknowledgments. Research reported here was supported by the following grants from the National Science Foundation: BNS-9020786, BCS-0091903, and BCS-1026429. Jason DeCaro and Kathryn S. Oths offered helpful comments on earlier drafts of the paper.
  • 37. R E F E R E N C E S C I T E D Belk, Russell W. 1988 Possessions and the Extended Self. Journal of Consumer Research 15(2):139–168. Bell, Carolyn S. 1995 What Is Poverty? The American Journal of Economics and Sociology, 54(2):161–162. A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 2 9 Black, Douglas and Peter Townsend 1982 Inequalities in Health: The Black Report. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Borgatti, Stephen P. 1994 Cultural Domain Analysis. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology 4(4):261–278. Bourdieu, Pierre 1984 Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Cohen, Sheldon, Tom Karmack, and Robin Mermelstein 1983 A Global Measure of Perceived Stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 24:385–396. Douglas, Mary and Baron Isherwood. 1979 The World of Goods: Towards an Anthropology of Consumption. New York: Basic Books.
  • 38. D’Andrade, Roy 1984 Cultural Meaning Systems. In Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. Richard A. Shweder, ed. Pp. 88–122. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1995 The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Dressler, William W. 1996 Culture and Blood Pressure: Using Consensus Analysis to Create a Measurement. Cultural Anthro- pology Methods 8(3):6–8. 2006 Cultural consonance and C-reactive protein in urban Brazil. Abstracts of the 105th Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, November 15–19, San Jose, CA. 2005 What’s Cultural about Biocultural Research? Ethos 33(1):20–45. 2010 Social Inequality and Health: A Commentary. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 24(4):549–554. Dressler, William W., Mauro C. Balieiro, and José Ernesto dos Santos 1997 The Cultural Construction of Social Support in Brazil: Associations with Health Outcomes. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 21(3):303–335. 1998 Culture, Socioeconomic Status, and Physical and Mental Health in Brazil. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 12(4):424–446. 2015 Finding Culture in the Second Factor: Stability and Change in Cultural Consensus and Residual
  • 39. Agreement. Field Methods 27:22–38. Dressler, William W., Mauro C. Balieiro, Rosane P. Ribeiro, and José Ernesto Dos Santos 2005 Cultural Consonance and Arterial Blood Pressure in Urban Brazil. Social Science and Medicine 61(3):527–540. 2007a Cultural Consonance and Psychological Distress: Examining the Associations in Multiple Cultural Domains. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 31(2):195–224. 2007b A Prospective Study of Cultural Consonance and Depressive Symptoms in Urban Brazil. Social Science and Medicine 65(10): 2058–2069. Dressler, William W., Camila D. Borges, Mauro C. Balieiro, and José Ernesto dos Santos 2005 Measuring Cultural Consonance: Examples with Special Reference to Measurement Theory in Anthropology. Field Methods 17(4):331–355. Dressler, William W., and José Ernesto dos Santos 2000 Social and Cultural Dimensions of Hypertension in Brazil: A Review. Cadernos de Saúde Pública 16(2): 303–315. Dressler, William W., José Ernesto dos Santos, and Mauro C. Balieiro 1996 Studying Diversity and Sharing in Culture: An Example of Lifestyle in Brazil. Journal of Anthropo- logical Research 52(3):331–353. Dressler, William W., Kathryn S. Oths, Mauro C. Balieiro,
  • 40. Rosane P. Ribeiro, and José Ernesto Dos Santos 2012 How Culture Shapes the Body: Cultural Consonance and Body Mass in Urban Brazil. American Journal of Human Biology 24(3):325–331. Dressler, William W., Kathryn S. Oths, Rosane P. Ribeiro, Mauro C. Balieiro, and José Ernesto Dos Santos 2004 Eating, Drinking and Being Depressed: The Social, Cultural and Psychological Context of Alcohol Consumption and Nutrition in a Brazilian Community. Social Science and Medicine 59(4): 709–720. 2008 Cultural Consonance and Adult Body Composition in Urban Brazil. American Journal of Human Biology 20(1):15–22. 2 3 0 A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s Engels, Friedrich 1958 [1845] The Condition of the Working Class in England. New York: Macmillan. Goodenough, Ward 1996 Culture. In Encyclopedia of Cultural Anthropology, vol II. David Levinson and Melvin Ember, eds. Pp. 291–299. New York: Henry Holt & Co. Hallerod, Bjorn 1996 Deprivation and Poverty: A Comparative Analysis of
  • 41. Sweden and Great Britain. Acta Sociologica 39(2):141–168. Healthy People 2020 N.d. Disparities. http://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/about/DisparitiesAbout.asp x, accessed July 1, 2013. Kawachi, Ichiro, and Bruce P. Kennedy 1999 Income Inequality and Health: Pathways and Mechanisms. Health Services Research 34(1 Pt 2):215– 227. Mack, Joanna, and Stewart Lansley 1985 Poor Britain. London: George Allen and Unwin. Marmot, Michael G. 2004 The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our Health and Longevity. New York: Henry Holt & Co. McCracken, Grant D. 1988 Culture and Consumption: New Approaches to Symbolic Character of Consumer Goods and Activ- ities. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. Romney, A. Kimball, Susan C. Weller, and William H. Batchelder 1986 Culture as Consensus: A Theory of Culture and Informant Accuracy. American Anthropologist 88(2): 313–338. Shore, Bradd
  • 42. 1991 Twice-Born, Once Conceived: Meaning Construction and Cultural Cognition. American Anthro- pologist 93:9–27. Townsend, Peter 1979 Poverty in the United Kingdom: A Survey of Household Resources and Standards of Living. Berkeley: University of California Press. Veblen, Thorstein 1918 The Theory of the Leisure Class: An Economic Study of Institutions. New edition. New York: B. W. Huebsch. Weinberg, Daniel H. 1995 Measuring Poverty: Issues and Approaches. In Race, Poverty, and Domestic Policy. C. Michael Henry, ed. Pp. 99–116. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census. Weller, Susan C. 2007 Cultural Consensus Theory: Applications and Frequently Asked Questions. Field Methods 19(4):339– 368. Wilkinson, Richard D. 1994 The Epidemiologic Transition—From Material Scarcity to Social Disadvantage. Daedalus 123(4): 61–77. Wilkinson, Richard G., and Kate E. Pickett 2006 Income Inequality and Population Health: A Review and Explanation of the Evidence. Social Science
  • 43. and Medicine 62(7): 1768–1784. 2007 The Problems of Relative Deprivation: Why Some Societies Do Better than Others. Social Science and Medicine 65(9):1965–1978. 2011 The Spirit Level: Why Greater Equality Makes Societies Stronger. New York: Bloomsbury Press. A n n a l s o f A n t h r o p o l o g i c a l P r a c t i c e 3 8 . 2 / C u l t u r e a s a M e d i a t o r o f H e a l t h D i s p a r i t i e s 2 3 1 Copyright of Annals of Anthropological Practice is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. T h e J o u r n a l o f D e v e l o p i n g A r e a s Volume 49 No. 4 Fall 2015 RELATIVE DEPRIVATION AND THE WORKING POOR: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS
  • 44. Mustafa A. Rahman North South University, Bangladesh. ABSTRACT In developing countries, we observe a new class of workers who work but live in poverty. Not only they are lowly paid but also deprived of opportunities available in the society. A person’s feeling of deprivation arises from incomes that are higher than his own income. Sen in his 1976 work on poverty measurement brought the term deprivation into focus. He posited that an individual’s level of deprivation in the income scale is an increasing function of the number of persons who are better off than the person in question, or, alternatively, the share of the given population that is better off. In a society where distribution of resources is unequal, deprivation of a particular group of workers is not unlikely. The idea behind deprivation theory is that merely lack of some goods and opportunities do not create a sense of deprivation among the workers. There are other factors that cause and perpetuate
  • 45. deprivation of the workers. This paper is an attempt to identify the factors and examine deprivation of the workers based on a sample survey conducted in Bangladesh in 2008-09. Findings of the study suggest that deprivation is acute among the uneducated aged workers with large household size predominated by low paid casual workers owning small landholding. Deprivation and age of the workers displayed a U-shaped curve indicating that deprivation increases with the increase of age. Deprivation is more pronounced among female workers compared to male workers. Food and health expenditure contributes significantly to reducing deprivation of the workers. Obviously, owners of better dwelling houses are less deprived than those owning poor dwelling houses. Interestingly, manufacturing workers have been found to be more deprived than other sectors under study. JEL Classifications: I132, C21, C51, O18 Keywords: Deprivation, working poor, endogenous variable, regression, labour market. Corresponding Author’s E-mail Address: [email protected] INTRODUCTION
  • 46. A person’s sense of deprivation arises from the comparison of his situation in the society with those of better off persons. Since 1970s, concerns of relativity have become important in assessing poverty across various groups of people in the society living in different conditions. An individual derives a satisfaction level (s) from his/her income only if the later is greater than the mean income. If his/her income is smaller than the mean income, he/she has a level of deprivation. Sen (1973, 1997) interpreted the well-known Gini coefficient from similar point of view. According to Sen, in any pair wise comparison, the person with lower income may suffer from depression on finding that his/her income is lower. The average of all such depressions in all pair wise comparisons becomes the Gini coefficient if the extent of a depression is proportional to the difference between the incomes concerned. Kakwani (1980) showed that if an individual’s depression is proportional to the square of income difference, we get the
  • 47. coefficient of variation as the average depression index. mailto:[email protected] 380 Sen believes that comparing poverty across distributions may involve different standards of minimum necessities (1981) and that absolute deprivation in terms of a person’s capabilities relates to relative deprivation in terms of commodities, incomes and resources (1983). An absolutely poor person may not be deprived relatively if he/she shares the same condition as most people in the neighborhood. It would not be unjust to say that deprivation is closely linked to social exclusion which is multifaceted in nature and arises out of personal attributes and malfunctioning of socio-political institutions of the country. In the words of Sen (1983), people are deprived because their capability set is simply not
  • 48. broad enough to permit them catching up. On the contrary, it may be due to structural lack of opportunities either at workplaces or in the society. The theory of relative deprivation is concerned with the feeling of resentment caused by inequality in the society. Deprivation is relative because people feel deprived in relation to others (Paul 1991). The idea of relative deprivation originated from Stouffer et.al. (1949) which later was developed by researchers and social scientists. Over the past half a century since Stouffer’s work, relative deprivation has been widely researched and a large body of empirical literature had developed based on this issue. The concept has been used in development studies and it has been prominent in studies of worker satisfaction (Butler 1976 and Hill 1974). The term relative deprivation was first formally defined by Davis (1959). In his opinion any social group may be divided into those who possess a desired good (non-deprived) and those who do not (deprived). When a deprived person compares himself with a non-deprived, the resulting state will be called
  • 49. ‘relative deprivation’. Following Davis, we may say that the preconditions for feeling deprived are that individuals (i) want X, (ii) compare themselves to someone similar to themselves who have X, and (iii) feel entitled to X. The concept got further clarification in Runciman’s (1966) formulation. To Davis’ list he added a fourth precondition i.e., to feel relatively deprived of X, one must also “see it as feasible that he should have X”. Gurr (1970) defines relative deprivation as the discrepancy between the goods and opportunities (X) that individuals want or to which they feel entitled, and their current or anticipated ability to obtain X. Later Yitzhaki (1979) showed that one’s relative deprivation in a society can be represented by the product of Gini Coefficient and mean income of the overall population. Chakravarty and Chakravarty (1984) introduced new normative indices where each index implies at least one social welfare function. Paul (1991) argue that Yitzhaki (1979) and Chakravarty and Chakravarty
  • 50. (1984) indices of aggregate relative deprivation are based on the unrealistic assumption that the individual’s deprivation is insensitive to income transfer among those who are richer than him. He suggested an index where he has shown that the deprivation of a person is sensitive to income transfers among those who are richer than him. Apart from objective assessment of relative deprivation, there are some who suggest that the judgment of one’s own status is not simply a function of one’s objective status. Instead, resentment, anger, dissatisfaction and other deprivation-related emotions vary with the subjective assessment of one’s own status (Bernstein and Crosby 19800). Although sometimes neglected by economists, relative deprivation has been linked to ‘‘definable and measurable social and psychological reactions, such as different types of alienation’’ (Durant and Christian, 1990) by social psychologists and to social protests, discrimination, feelings of injustice and subjective ill-being (Olson, Herman, and Zanna, 1986). A person’s
  • 51. feeling of deprivation may be absolute or relative. In the former case income shares are a 381 source of envy while in the latter case people’s dissatisfaction depends on income differentials. However, both the measures are extreme cases and hence the general notion can be called intermediate invariance (Amiel and Cowell 1992, Harrison and Sield 1994). It is a convex mixture of relative and absolute deprivations (Chakravarty 2009). Deprivation as such can certainly be a significant handicap that impoverishes the lives of the excluded people in the society (Bardhan, 2011). Inclusive growth therefore calls forth a gradual reduction of relative deprivation by breaking poverty traps (Banerjee and Duflo, 2011). Therefore, relative deprivation is important from the policy point of view because if it is severe then the policies should focus on
  • 52. redistribution rather than economic growth (Kakwani 1993, Ravallion and Chen 1998). The socioeconomic condition of the workers under study indicates that merely lack of some goods and opportunities does not create a sense of deprivation among them. Determining which factors do affect such feelings is the focus of our investigation. It also examines deprivation of a worker compared to another worker in the informal labour market across various sectors (agriculture, manufacturing, transport, construction and service). The study is based on a sample survey conducted in Bangladesh in 2008-09. To work out the exercise standard regression and IV regression have been used as instruments of analysis. DATA AND METHODOLOGY Data Source Data for estimating the model have been obtained from a sample survey conducted in
  • 53. Bangladesh during 2008-09. Data were collected from both rural and urban areas. A multi- stage stratified random sampling technique without replacement was used to select sample locations and respondents. Households were drawn randomly, proportionate to the size in the population: 248 from the rural area and 412 from the urban area. The rural area refers to agriculture sector while urban area includes manufacturing, construction, transport and services sectors. Thus a total of 660 households have been selected for intensive interview. Because of some statistical discrepancy, some observations have been deleted resulting in a sample size of 610 instead of 660, the net sample size is 610. Specification of the Model The model of capturing relative deprivation across various sectors under study has been specified as follows: � = �� + �� + � (1)
  • 54. Where, � = (��, �� , �� , �� ) ′ �= (��, ��, �� , �� ) 382 � = Independent variables {age, education etc.} Equation (1) can be re-written as � = �� + ���� + �� �� + �� �� + �� �� + (2) In the above equation �stands for relative deprivation index. The notations �� , �� , �� and �� represent dummy variables for agriculture, manufacturing, transport and construction sectors respectively. The service sector has been considered as the reference dummy. The magnitude of individual sector dummies compared with that of service sector dummy would provide us relative deprivation for the individual sectors with respect to service sector. Description of Model Variables
  • 55. Our dependent variable is relative deprivation index (D) which is the difference between actual wage and the average wage of the sample workers. Wages has been considered synonymous with income because, for the workers under analysis wages is their only source of income. The importance of the average income is due to the fact that an individual’s feelings depends not only on his income level but also on his rank in the income distribution and an individual is assumed to have negative feelings if his/her earning is less than the average income (Berrebi and Silber 1989). The independent variables have been described below: TABLE 1: DESCRIPTION OF INDEPENDENT VARIABLES Independent variables Description Marital status Married/unmarried Age Actual years attained Age2 Age*Age Education (EDU) Years of schooling
  • 56. Gender (GEN) Male and Female respondents Value of assets (VASSET) It refers to assets (livestock, furniture, electronics, agricultural equipment, bicycle, watch, rickshaw, pushcarts, sewing machine etc.) valued at 2009 prices. Landholding (LHOLD) Ownership of homestead and cultivable lands measured in acres. Food expenditure (FEXP) Indicates expenses on food items (rice, wheat, meat, fish, milk, egg, lentil, vegetables etc.) valued at 2009 prices 383 Health expenditure ((HLTEXP) Refers to doctor’s consultation fee, expenses for medicine, hospitalisation etc.
  • 57. Education expenditure (EDUEXP) Refers to expenses for tuition fees, books, accessories, school dress etc. Household size (HHSIZE) Average size of the household Dwelling house 1= Kutcha (mud floor), 2= semi-pucca (concrete wall and tin-shed), 3= pucca ( concrete wall and roof), 4= thatched (bamboo and straw made) Drinking water 1= Tube well, 2= surface water, 3= tape/supply water Agriculture sector Manufacturing sector Transport sector Construction sector Empirical Application
  • 58. The relative deprivation index (D) has been calculated based on wages of the workers. It is the difference between actual wage and the average wage of the workers. For our analysis 26 independent variables have been considered for the regression model. To achieve a sound regression model I examined if the independent variables were highly correlated or not. With that end in view I have worked out correlation matrix for the stated variables. From the correlation matrix urban-rural area, age-sector (4 variables) and gender-sector (4variables) interacting variables have been found highly correlated. Therefore, to avoid multicollinearity, these variables have been dropped and as a result 17 variables remained for regression analysis. Incorporating the independent variables in equation (2) above I have got equation (3): CCTTMMAAi i
  • 59. i ddddxD 17 1 0 (3) Since the minimum wage in our sample might have been different from the population minimum wage,I have compared results from truncated regression and OLS regression to examine if there was any significant dissimilarity between the two models. The estimates of the models indicate that both the models are similar in terms of predictability and significance of the parameters . The estimates of regression coefficients for both the models 384 have been presented in Table 2. There were some insignificant variables in the estimated
  • 60. model which were- value of assets, agriculture and construction sectors. At this stage, I have conducted Wald Test to examine if these variables are jointly insignificant or not. The Wald test indicates that the variables i.e., value of assets; agriculture and construction sectors are jointly insignificant (Table 3). TABLE 2. ESTIMATES OF TRUNCATED AND OLS REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS Independent variables Coefficients ( truncated regression) Coefficients ( OLS regression) Constant - 42.893 (4.781) - 40.440 (4.613) Age Age2
  • 61. 1.082*** ( 0.296) - .015*** (0 . 004) - 0. 949*** (0. 285) (-. 013)*** ( . 003) Agriculture 0.024 (2.015) - 0.131 (1. 940) Construction - 3.552 (2.227) - 3.413 (2. 131) Manufacturing -7.093*** (1.731)
  • 62. - 7.185*** (1. 677) Transportation 9.487*** ( 2.174) 9.466*** (2.140) Dwelling house 2.895*** (0.715) 2.850*** (0.695) Drinking water 3.444*** (0.668) 3.239*** (0.653) Education 1.180** (0.627) 1.143** (0.616)
  • 63. Education expenditure 0.010** (0.002) 0.010 (0.002) Food expenditure 0.017*** (0.004) 0.017*** (0.004) Health expenditure 0.008*** (0.002) 0.008*** (0.002) Gender 12.174*** (1.192) 11.471*** (1.144) Household size 1.601*** (0.498)
  • 64. 1.425*** (0.487) Landholding 13.400*** (2.880) 13.118*** (2. 854) Value of assets Marital status 0.004 (0.002) 2.56** (1.08) 0.004 (0.002) 1.28** (0.52)
  • 65. Note: Terms in the parentheses indicate standard error. ** and *** indicate significance at 5% and 1% levels. 385 TABLE 3.ESTIMATES OF WALD TEST Test Statistic Value df Probability F- statistic 1.7162 (3, 643) 0.1624 Chi-square 5.1486 3 0.1612 Eliminating the insignificant independent variables, the reformulated regression model stands as follows: (4) ANALYSIS OF RESULTS The estimates of the reformulated regression are given in Table 4. The estimated signs of the variables have the expected signs and the coefficients are statistically significant. The relationship between deprivation faced by workers and their age
  • 66. follow a U- shaped curve. The deprivation decreases with an increase in age till the worker reaches the age of 38 and then it increases with an increase in workers age. Manufacturing workers have been found to be more deprived and transport workers less deprived compared to service sector workers. The workers who own high quality of dwelling house and have access to drinking water experience less deprivation. We observe the same trend in case of food and health expenditure. Male workers have been found to be less deprived compared to female workers. Deprivation is more pronounced for the married workers compared to those who are unmarried, divorced or separated. Deprivation is relatively low for larger households compared to smaller households which may be because of more earning members of the larger households. Households with larger landholdingshave been found less deprived compared to smaller landholdings.
  • 67. It would seem reasonable to assume that if an individual spends more on his/her food, then this individual would be less deprived. However, a more deprived person would also spend less on food expenditure. This means that the causality may run both ways. Hence, to test whether food expenditure is an endogenous variable,I performed Hausman test in STATA using IV-2SLS and I found it as an endogenous variable. Under the null hypothesis that the food expenditure can actually be treated as exogenous, the test statistic (see page 482 in Baum et.al., 2007) is distributed as 2 (degree of freedom is the number of regressors being tested for endogeniety). The null hypothesis stands rejected since the p-value for the test has been found to be less than 0.05 or the 2 2 0.95,1
  • 68. In order to deal with endogenietyI needed to choose instruments which affected food expenditure but not directly impacting deprivation. I have chosen household size as an instrument. Household size affects food expenditure directly but affects deprivation via food expenditure. Ceteris Paribus, households with larger size would spend more on food expenditure compared to smaller size. However, household size may not be directly related to deprivation. The estimates of the model adjusted for endogeneity and heteroskcedasticity CCTTMMAAi i i ddddxD 14 1 0
  • 69. 386 are presented in Table 3. It is interesting to note that while ‘food expenditure’ was found highly significant in reformulated regression, it appeared insignificant in 2SLS regression (Table 3). This could be because it may potentially be an endogenous variable. I also observe in this table that after ‘food expenditure’ is instrumented for, in 2SLS regression the corresponding coefficient changes its sign. TABLE 4. ESTIMATES OF REFORMULATED AND 2SLS REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS Independent variables Coefficients (reformulated OLS regression) Coefficients (2SLS regression) Constant -57.258***
  • 70. (6.390) - 36.748*** (7.702) Age 0 .897*** ( 0.285) 0.795*** ( 0307) Age2 - .012*** (0. 004) - .011*** (0.004) Manufacturing - 5. 668*** (1.477) - 3.947** (1.866) Transportation 9.974*** (1.773) 11.161***
  • 71. (2.246) Dwelling house 2.556*** ( 0.652) 2.305*** ( 0.679) Drinking water 3.118*** ( 0.617) 3.562*** ( 0.677) Education 1.378** ( 0.610) 1.603*** (0.572) Education expenditure 0.010*** ( 0.002) 0.012*** (0.002) Food expenditure 0.017***
  • 72. ( 0.003) -0.006 (0.008) Health expenditure 0.007*** ( 0.002) 0.006*** ( 0.002) Gender 12.35*** ( 1.104) 12.903*** ( 1.290) Marital status 4.011*** ( 1.300) 3.807*** (1.371) Household size 1.359*** ( 0.483)
  • 73. na Landholding 14.31*** ( 2.758) 11.621*** ( 3.500) Note: Terms in the parentheses indicate standard error. Household size is an instrumental variable. ** and *** indicate significance at 5% and 1% levels. 387 CONCLUSIONS In Bangladesh, deprivation is widespread particularly among the poor who work but live
  • 74. in poverty. Our study has provided some interesting insights whose implications for policy are enormous. In our study, deprivation has been found to decrease with the increase of age of the workers but up to a certain limit. This may be because of the fact that workers remain productive at the early stages of their lives.In Bangladesh, the poor workers if however, they get job remain employed in low paid occupations. They end up their career with meager savings or zero savings. The finding suggests that the aged workers should be provided with adequate old age benefit so that they can lead a plain living.The government may introduce income supplement schemes for them like India and Thailand. Both education and educational expenses have offsetting effect on deprivation of the workers. The implication for policy is that if the workers are provided with education, their deprivation would cease to exist in near future. Education lessens deprivation because educated workers are highly productive, high productivity generates high income for the
  • 75. workers. Therefore, education of the workers should be emphasized at both household and national levels. At household level, a portion of the family budget should be reserved for educating the workers side by side with work. At national level, adequate resources should be allocated for educating and training the workers while they are on-the-job. The extent of deprivation is severe in manufacturing sector compared to other sectors under study. The manufacturing workers as they are mostly uneducated and unskilled are low paid workers. The employers are unwilling to pay them high wages because of their low productivity, as a result the poor workers fall trapped in low skill-low paid vicious cycle of deprivation poverty. The government in this regard must enforce a reasonable minimum wages for them so that they can lead a decent life. Health expenditure has been found to have reduced deprivation of the workers. The implication for policy is that if government subsidizes health related costs such as free medicine and medicare, it would
  • 76. help reducing deprivation of the workers because healthy workers are highly productive. High productivity yields high income contributing to lessening severity of deprivation. Introducing health insurance scheme for the workers may be a good step forward in this regard. Male workers have been found to be less deprived compared to female workers. Deprivation is more pronounced for the married workers compared to those who are unmarried, divorced or separated. Male-female wage differential is a stark reality in developing countries like ours. For equal worth job, female workers are paid more than their male counterparts. The implication for policy in this regard is that government should enact legislations ensuring equal pay for equal worth job for both male and female workers. The female workers should be organized through trade unions so that they can raise their voices for fair wages and other benefits. Apart from enacting legislations the government should ratify relevant UN conventions for eliminating the wage
  • 77. gap between male and female workers. At enterprise level, both male and female workers should be treated fairly and judiciously. Land holdings have been found to lessen acuteness of deprivation. Therefore, the poor workers may be gifted with adequate fallow land through appropriate land reform and distribution. 388 REFERENCES Akerlof, G.A., “A Theory of Social Custom, of Which Unemployment May be One Consequence”, 1980, Quarterly Journal of Economics 94, 749- 775. Bardhan, P. K.” Awakening Giants, Feet of Clay: Assessing the Economic Rise of China and India”, 2010, MIT Press.
  • 78. Banerjee, A. V. and E. Duflo., “Poor Economics: A Radical Rethinking of the Way to Fight Global Poverty”, 2011, Random House, New Delhi. Baum, C. F., Schaffer, M. E. and Stillman, S., "Enhanced routines for instrumental variables/generalized method of moments estimation and testing,", 2007, Stata Journal, 7(4), 465-506. Bernstein. M. and Crosby, F., “An Empirical Examination of Relative Deprivaiton Theory” 1980, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 442-456. Berrebi, Z. M. and Silber, J., “Deprivation: the Gini Index of Inequality and the Flatness of an Income Distribution”, 1989, Mathematical Social Sciences, 18, pp. 229-237. Blau, Peter M.,”Exchange and Power in Social Life”, 1964, New York: Wiley. Clark, A.E., Oswald, A.J., “Comparison-Concave Utility and Following Behaviour in Social and Economic Settings”, 1998,.Journal of Public Economics, 63, 232-45. Chakravarty, S. R. And Chakravarty, A.B., “On Indices of Relative Deprivation.
  • 79. Economics Letters”, 1984, 14, 283-287. Clark, A.E. Oswald, A.J., “Satisfaction and Comparison Income”, 1996, .Journal of Public Economics, 61, 359-381. Durant, T. J. and O, Christian, O., ‘‘Socio-Economic Predictors of Alienation Among the Elderly,’’ 1990, International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 31, 205– 17. Kakwani, N., “Poverty and economic growth with an application to Cote d’Ivoire”, 1993, Review of Income and Wealth, 39(2), pp. 121-139. Lenoir, Rene., “Les Exclus - UnFrancaissur Dix”, 1974, Paris, Editions du Seuil. Olson, J. M., Herman, P. and Zanna, M. P., :” Relatvûe Depriûation and Social Comparisons: The Ontario Symposium”, 1986, vo1. 4, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, London. Rahman, M. A., “Household Characteristics and Poverty: A Logistic Regression Analysis” 2013, Journal of Developing Areas, Vol. 47, No. 1.
  • 80. Ravallion, M. and Chen, S.,“When economic reform is faster than statistical reform- measuring and explaining inequality in rural China” 1998, , Policy Research Working Paper Series-1992, World Bank. Sen, A.,(1981): “Poverty and Famine: An Essay on Entitlement and Deprivation”, 1981, Clarendon Press, Oxford University Press. Sen, A. “Poverty: an Ordinal Approach to Measurement”, 1976, Econometrica, Vol.44, pp. 219-231. Sen, A., “Poor, relatively speaking”, 1983, Oxford Economic Papers, 35, 153-69. Silver, H., “Reconceptualising Social Disadvantage - Three Paradigms of Social Exclusion”, 1995, In Gerry, R. (ed.) Social Exclusion: Rhetoric, Reality and Response. Paul, S., “An Index of Relative Deprivation”, 1991, Economics Letters, 36, pp.337-341. http://ideas.repec.org/a/tsj/stataj/v7y2007i4p465-506.html http://ideas.repec.org/a/tsj/stataj/v7y2007i4p465-506.html http://ideas.repec.org/s/tsj/stataj.html
  • 81. http://ideas.repec.org/s/tsj/stataj.html 389 Stouffer, S. A. Suchman, E.A. DeVinney, L.C., Star, S.A. and Williams, R.M., “The American Soldier: Adjustments during Army Life 1”,1949, Princeton University Press, Princeton NJ,.Runciman. W. G., “Relative Deprivation and Social Justice”, 1966, Routledge and Kegan Paul, London. Butler, R. J., “Relative Deprivation and Power: A switched replication design using time series data of strike rates in American and British coal mining”, 1976, Human Relations, 29, 623-641. Hill, R. C., “Unionisation and racial income inequality in the metropolice”, 1974, American Sociological Review, 39, 507-522.
  • 82. Copyright of Journal of Developing Areas is the property of Tennessee State University, College of Business and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Development Policy Review, 2015, 33 (1): 5-13 © The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015 Overseas Development Institute. Development Policy Review 33 (1) Equity: Not Only for Idealists Lawrence Haddad∗ Growing disparities in development outcomes are storing up trouble for current and future generations. In addition to the moral issues raised and the intrinsic welfare effects for those experiencing relative deprivation, this article argues that recent research supports a stronger public-policy focus on equity. Increasing inequity blunts both growth and the ability of growth
  • 83. to translate into human-development outcomes, and puts institutions, social cohesion and the productivity of future generations at risk. The article highlights new micro evidence suggesting that a focus on the most marginal might well lead to higher benefit:cost ratios, and outlines the choices that need to be made to generate a more equity-focused policy agenda. Key words: Equity, policy, politics, idealists, pragmatists 1 Introduction In the world of international development, a long-standing divide has separated ‘idealists’, at one extreme, and the more ‘practical-minded’ at the other. The former tend to give more weight to addressing the needs of the most destitute and excluded and less attention to the benefit:cost ratios of doing so. The latter are more driven by aggregate impact and costs, with a focus on national averages such as GDP per capita. They worry about whether a focus on equity will curb growth, about the practicalities of reaching the poorest and about the political feasibility of doing so. This article aims to bring together and outline some disparate evidence streams that suggest that the interests of these groups are converging. It comes to the conclusion that, increasingly, focusing on the most vulnerable is not only a moral necessity; it seems like a
  • 84. practical approach to promoting development. Current trends on development outcomes are complex and, at times, even seem contradictory. There is much good news. At the macro level, many countries (for example, India, Ghana, Vietnam, Indonesia) are graduating out of needing traditional forms of development assistance because of their impressive rates of GDP per capita growth. There also has been progress in reducing poverty rates and child mortality and in increasing school enrolment and attainment (UN, 2011). These achievements are real and are improving the wellbeing of hundreds of millions of people. However, while these indicators of development – many based on averages – are improving, there is a growing disparity in the rates of progress in outcomes between rich and poor groups within countries. For example, the Millennium Development Goals Report ∗ Senior Research Fellow, International Food Policy Research Institute, Washington, DC ([email protected]). This article was motivated by conversations with Tony Lake, the Executive Director of UNICEF, and his colleague Robert Jenkins, and he would like to thank both of them for their support. All errors are his alone. 6 Lawrence Haddad
  • 85. © The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015 Overseas Development Institute. Development Policy Review 33 (1) 2011 states that ‘the poorest children are making the slowest progress in reducing underweight prevalence’ (UN, 2011: 15). In other examples, a study by UNICEF (2010a) found that in 18 out of 26 countries where the national under- five mortality rate has declined by 10% or more since 1990, the gap between the richest and poorest quintiles had either grown or remained unchanged. In another report (UNICEF, 2010b) an analysis of data from India found that 166 million people gained access to improved sanitation between 1995 and 2008, but little progress was made in the poorest households. In the same report, data from West and Central Africa from 1990 to 2008 show that measles- immunisation coverage increased by 10% in the wealthiest quintile of the population, but by only 3% in the poorest quintile. In other words, progress is being made, but is skewed away from the worst-off. These growing disparities matter. First, there are the moral issues raised by the fact that many of the disparities are based largely on chance. The circumstances someone is born into, and the genetic make-up they inherit, play a very large role in the poverty consequences of their exposure to negative exogenous shocks
  • 86. (Currie, 2011). For example, those born in an exposed landscape, with fragile health, are more likely to suffer from an extreme weather shock. Second, the disparities carry intrinsic negative welfare consequences (Klasen, 2008; Deaton, 2003): status matters for a whole host of psycho- social reasons. But our focus here is on the economic and political consequences. We argue that these disparities matter for the following reasons: (i) they exert an ever stronger brake on the conversion of economic growth into development outcomes—and on growth itself; (ii) they are storing up trouble for current and future generations; and (iii) they demand a realignment of policy approaches. Two factors add some urgency to our arguments. First, as we shall see, there is much new evidence now emerging on the consequences of inequality. Second, the disparities do not promise to narrow anytime soon. For example, the poorest 20% of the world’s population holds 2% of its income. At the rate of change seen in the past 20 years, it will take more than 250 years for the poorest 20% to hold 10% (Ortiz and Cummins, 2011: Table 5). 2 Growing disparities are a brake on the poverty-reducing power of economic growth and on growth itself The quality of economic growth is coming under increased
  • 87. scrutiny. When does it generate jobs for the poorest? When does it fuel corruption? When does it generate negative environmental externalities? When does it bypass the most vulnerable? These are important debates, given the strong growth in Africa and Asia and the need to sustain it to improve development outcomes. There is a near consensus on the impact of growth on poverty. For any type of growth, we know that its ability to reduce poverty is diminished the greater the initial inequality. This is because growth has to work harder to move people above the (absolute) poverty line. The empirical evidence—old and new--bears this out at the cross-country and micro levels (Ferreira and Ravallion, 2008; Klasen, 2009; Geda et al., 2009; Fosu, 2009; Milanovic, 2011). Moreover, much of the empirical evidence also seems to support the proposition that higher initial inequality slows down subsequent growth (Easterly, 2007; Bluhm and Equity: Not Only for Idealists 7 © The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015 Overseas Development Institute. Development Policy Review 33 (1)
  • 88. Szirmai, 2011; Klasen, 2009; Ortiz and Cummins, 2011) and reduces the duration of growth spells (Berg and Ostry, 2011). In the Ortiz and Cummins study data from a diverse set of 131 countries show that the countries with higher levels of inequality experienced slower annual per capita GDP growth over the past 20 years. An IMF staff study (Berg and Ostry, 2011) found that a 10 percentile decrease in inequality increases the expected length of an economic growth period by 50%. However, this inequality-growth literature is fragile. For example, Barro (2000) finds evidence to support a Kuznets relationship: higher inequality tends to retard growth in poor countries and encourage growth in richer places, while from her cross-country regression work Forbes (2000) finds that increased income inequality has a significant positive relationship with subsequent economic growth. Duflo (2011) describes how unstable the cross-country studies are in this space, with substantial variation in conclusions depending on countries and time periods selected, lags and non- linearities employed and identification approaches tried. 3 Growing disparities today are storing up trouble for the future Recent studies are adding to the idea that inequalities set up additional problems for current and future generations – beyond slower poverty reduction and economic growth. First, they seem to lead to weaker economic and political institutions (Savoia et al., 2010; Bluhm and Szirmai, 2011). These institutions, such as property rights,
  • 89. market regulation and civil and political rights, are vital to ignite and sustain growth and to hold societies together. Second, high levels of economic inequality between different groups and types of individuals matter greatly. These horizontal inequalities between ethnic and political groups are econometrically linked to the onset of civil war (Østby, 2008; Cederman et al., 2011; Stewart, 2010), and we know that conflict sets back development by decades (World Bank, 2011). Political instability is a disincentive for foreign investment and thus significantly undermines a nation’s growth potential (WEF, 2012). Indeed, many in the private sector seem increasingly concerned about inequity as a barrier to growth. In a recent survey of 469 experts and industry leaders by the World Economic Forum, inequality was identified as one of the greatest risks to global growth and stability (ibid.). Inequalities also occur by gender. Point estimates suggest that between 0.4% and 0.9% of the differences in growth rates between East Asia and sub-Saharan Africa, South Asia and the Middle East can be accounted for by the larger gender gaps in education prevailing in the latter regions (Klasen, 2009). Third, inequalities suffered in early childhood play out in adulthood and across generations via nutrition and education deficits (Hoddinott et al., 2013; Heckman, 2011). For example, Hoddinott et al. (2011) find that the prevention of stunting at 36 months of
  • 90. age, other things held constant, raises per capita consumption in adulthood by 66%; inequality in early life has huge consequences in later life. Moreover, we know that stunted mothers are more likely to have stunted babies (Andersson and Bergstrom, 2003). For both rich and poor countries, an increase in learning achievement (as measured by test scores and delivered by improvements in nutrition status and the quality of schooling) of one standard deviation is associated on average with an increase in the long-run growth rate of around 2% per capita annually (Hanushek and Woessmann, 2008 ). 8 Lawrence Haddad © The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015 Overseas Development Institute. Development Policy Review 33 (1) 4 The policy agenda With the evidence mounting that inequality reduces growth and its poverty-reducing potential, weakens institutions and undermines political and social cohesion, what should the policy response be? The level of inequality in a society is a reflection, in part, of its
  • 91. history and culture and as such is embedded in its politics, electoral or otherwise. Those denied inclusion, participation and social mobility, together with those who support a more inclusive society, need to build medium-run momentum to change the tolerance of inequality and the preference for greater equity. Sometimes a lack of evidence should not be an excuse to do nothing when values dictate otherwise. In the short run, the policy levers are many: addressing unequal opportunity in early life; correcting inequality of public provision in services or infrastructure; using taxes to redistribute income by progressive taxation; using transfers to increase the incomes of those vulnerable to poverty, including old-age pensions; and the redistribution of productive assets, including land. This terrain is too big to cover here. Our approach therefore is to (i) highlight one particular approach suggested by some new research and (ii) to outline the contours of policy options around the what, how, why and who of any choices made. 4.1 Focusing on the most deprived Much public policy is focused on achieving the highest benefit:cost ratios that can be derived from a set of instruments designed to tackle a given problem. But the pressure to reach national targets on health, education and poverty can narrow the focus of efforts to the people and areas easiest to reach rather than those who are most disadvantaged (Besley
  • 92. and Kanbur, 1993). We know intrinsically that, given equal costs of action, the marginal returns to development tend to be greatest when investments are made in those with the least. For example, the same vaccination programme in an area of widespread disease saves more lives than in an area less afflicted. The greatest boost to learning will come from the opening of a good school in an area where there is none. The greatest impact of a new nutrition centre will be in an area where there are no nutrition services because the area is excluded on the grounds of, say, ethnicity. The question is whether these greater impacts can be realised in practice and whether they outweigh the additional cost of working in the areas that are hardest to reach. The answer seems to be positive. The increase in returns from focusing on the poorest communities more than offsets the additional costs. According to a recent 14-country analysis (Carrera et al., 2012), an equity-focused approach is actually more cost-effective, at least in reducing avoidable deaths of young children. The study reports: … an equity-focused approach that prioritises the most deprived communities, and a mainstream approach that is representative of current strategies. We combined some existing models, notably the Marginal Budgeting for Bottlenecks
  • 93. Toolkit and the Lives Saved Tool, to do our analysis. We showed that, with the same level of investment, disproportionately higher effects are possible by Equity: Not Only for Idealists 9 © The Author 2015. Development Policy Review © 2015 Overseas Development Institute. Development Policy Review 33 (1) prioritising the poorest and most marginalised populations, for averting both child mortality and stunting. (ibid.: 1341) The equity-focused approach was more concerned with reaching the poorest, via community health workers, with appropriate incentives for frontline staff, more targeted cash transfers and infrastructure rehabilitation to connect the poorest districts. For the more equity-focused approach, a $1 million investment averted 244 cases of stunting, and the current approach, with the same budget, averted 84 cases. These results may well strengthen in the future as the costs of delivering services to those most excluded are reduced through the use of new information and logistic technologies (HHI, 2011). 4.2 Navigating the policy terrain