SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  6
Télécharger pour lire hors ligne
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 32, No. 3, June 2002 (© 2002)




Brief Reports

Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an
Elementary-Aged Student with Autism Using the Picture
Exchange Communication System

Tamara R. Kravits,1 Debra M. Kamps,1,4 Katie Kemmerer,2 and Jessica Potucek3



                                   The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of the Picture Exchange Communication
                                   System (PECS) on the spontaneous communication skills of a 6-year-old girl with autism
                                   across her home and school environments. The effects of the PECS were also examined for
                                   social interaction. Results indicated increases in spontaneous language (i.e., requests and com-
                                   ments) including use of the icons and verbalizations across those settings in which PECS was
                                   implemented. Intelligible verbalizations increased in two of three settings, and changes in peer
                                   social interaction were noted in one of the two school settings.

                                   KEY WORDS: Picture Exchange Communication System; augmentative communication; peer training.




INTRODUCTION                                                                 sembles a more naturalistic approach to teaching in that
                                                                             communication is child initiated rather than controlled
      Augmentative communication systems (AACs)                              by adult verbal cues. The PECS was developed to
have been shown to be a successful language inter-                           (1) provide an effective AAC for nonverbal children,
vention for many nonverbal children (e.g., Reichle &                         while simultaneously minimizing the prompt depen-
Sigafoos, 1991; Shafer, 1993; Zangari, Lloyd, &                              dency by teaching children to spontaneously initiate
Vicker, 1994). Unfortunately, instructional strategies                       their wants/needs through an exchange of a picture for
using AAC systems may rely on the overuse of verbal                          the corresponding item /activity, and (2) to provide ver-
and/or physical prompts such as “What do you want?”                          bal models of language with use of the picture exchange
or “Point to what you want.” As a result, some children                      to encourage oral language. In addition, PECS combines
become prompt dependent and lack spontaneity in their                        previously researched procedures into the teaching pro-
communication (Mirenda & Dattilo, 1987).                                     tocol to encourage child initiation/motivation including
      A promising instructional intervention with AAC                        (a) child choice and preference (e.g., Dyer, 1989;
is the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS)                          Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987), (b) time delay (e.g.,
developed by Bondy and Frost (1994), which addresses                         Charlop, Schreibman, & Thibodeau, 1985; Halle, 1982),
these concerns. PECS is structured in a way that re-                         (c) environmental arrangement (e.g., Carta, Sainato, &
                                                                             Greenwood, 1988), and (d) differential reinforcement
1
  University of Kansas, Juniper Gardens Children’s Project, Kansas
                                                                             (e.g., Koegel et al., 1987; Reichle & Sigafoos, 1991).
  City, Kansas 66101.                                                              In descriptive reports, Bondy and Frost (1994)
2
  Autism Training Center, University of Louisville, Louisville, Ken-         noted improved communication for children with
  tucky.                                                                     autism using the PECS as well as increases in sponta-
3
  Project S.A.I.L., 10401 Holmes Road, Suite 440, Kansas City, Mis-          neous language acquisition; however, few empirical
  souri 64131.
4
  Correspondence should be addressed to Debra M. Kamps, Juniper
                                                                             demonstrations have been reported. In a second de-
  Gardens Children’s Project, 650 Minnesota Ave., 2nd Floor, Kansas          scriptive study with 31 preschoolers with disabilities,
  City, Kansas 66101; e-mail: dkamps@ukans.edu                               the use of PECS showed increased spontaneous use of
                                                                       225
                                                                                          0162-3257/02/0600-0225/0 © 2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation
226                                                                     Kravits, Kamps, Kemmerer, and Potucek

icons and generalization to novel school settings             Measurement
(Schwartz, Garfinkle, & Bauer, 1998). Verbalizations
increased for talkers but not for nonverbal children                The frequency of spontaneous language including
in the study. The purpose of the current study was to         requests (i.e., words or approximations asking for items
(a) evaluate the effectiveness of teaching PECS on the        or help), comments (i.e., labeling or describing items,
spontaneous communication of an elementary-aged               situations), or expansions (adding new elements to pre-
child with autism, (b) determine feasibility of use by        vious utterances) was selected as the target behavior
the mother, classroom teachers, and peers across home         (dependent variable) to be treated with the PECS.
and school environments, and (c) note effects in con-         Requests and comments were counted only if they
junction with social skills training for social interaction   occurred without prompting (i.e., no instruction, ques-
behaviors.                                                    tion within 5 seconds prior).
                                                                    Data were collected by the experimenters for lan-
                                                              guage behaviors that occurred within 10-minute sam-
METHOD
                                                              ple periods. When the student communicated, the
                                                              observer recorded what the student communicated, the
Participant, Settings, Materials
                                                              mode (e.g., verbal, symbol, sign), the function (e.g.,
     Molly was a 6-year-old girl with a diagnosis of          request, comment), and to whom (i.e., adult or peer) the
autism. She was integrated, with the assistance of a para-    student was communicating. Repeated verbalizations
professional, in a half-day kindergarten program in the       were recorded as one communicative episode, ending
public school. In addition, she received 30–60 minutes        after a 5-second pause.
of special education services per day from the learning             Social interaction data were collected in 5-minute
center teacher and language therapist. Molly’s score on       intervals using a laptop computer programmed with the
the Vineland indicated a 2 years 8 months performance         Multi Option Observation System for Experimental
level. Scores on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary           Studies (MOOSES) developed by Tapp, Wehby, and
Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-R) were within the 27th          Ellis (1992). This system was used to code the dura-
percentile for verbal behavior and the 1st percentile for     tion of interaction between Molly and her peers or the
adaptive behavior. Her developmental age was 2–2.5            adults in her environment. Once an interaction (i.e., ini-
years on the Psychoeducational Profile-Revised (PEP-          tiation followed by a response within 5 seconds) was
R). When prompted, Molly communicated using 1- to             observed, it was keyed in and continuously recorded
2-word utterances (e.g., “Want cookie”), but her speech       until the observer keyed the end of the interaction; see
was difficult to understand, and her frequency of initi-      prior reports for program description (Kamps, Potucek,
ations was very low. She also used gestures and eye con-      Gonzalez-Lopez, Kravits, & Kemmerer, 1997).
tact to communicate, but she initiated and used more                Language samples and social interaction data were
verbalizations at home with her mother than with teach-       collected at least once every session across all settings
ers or peers at school. Thus, the PECS was chosen to          (i.e., two times a week in journal time and centers, two
provide Molly with an effective communication system          or three times a week at home).
across settings and to increase her spontaneous initia-             Interobserver agreement was collected for each
tions and interactions with others in her environment.        language variable for 11% of the observation sessions.
     Settings for the study included leisure and snack        The mean percentage of agreement for content of the
time in Molly’s home and play periods with peers dur-         verbalizations was 93% in baseline and 91% in inter-
ing journal (writing/coloring in notebooks followed by        vention; 97% in baseline for mode (icon versus sign ver-
play) and center activities (free play) at school. All set-   sus verbal) and 96% in intervention; and 89% in
tings were similar to free time with play with others as      baseline for function (i.e., spontaneous request versus
a primary activity and the inclusion of peers at school.      comment) and 86% in intervention. Reliability was
Materials consisted of food (e.g., popcorn, candy, soda,      computed on an item-by-item basis, with the number
cookies), toys (e.g., Casio piano, Koosh Balls, Slinkys,      of agreements divided by the number of agreements
markers, Silly Putty), and games (e.g., Don’t Break the       plus disagreements. Reliability for social interaction
Ice). Graphic symbols/icons (line drawings) of items          data for 14% of the MOOSES files was computed me-
were taken from the Mayer-Johnson Picture Commu-              chanically using software that matched the files of the
nication Symbols (1990) or were hand drawn (2            2-   primary and second observers with the mean agreement
inch black and white).                                        at 86%.
PECS                                                                                                              227

Design and Procedures                                       Phase I, Physically-Assisted Exchange, consisted of
                                                            teaching Molly to initiate a communicative exchange
     A multiple baseline design across settings (Baer,
                                                            by giving her a picture of a desired item/activity to the
Wolf, & Risley, 1968) was used to document treatment
                                                            trainer. Training consisted of placing the item slightly
effectiveness. The experimental conditions included
                                                            out of her reach. When she reached for the item, a
two baseline conditions and two treatment phases that
                                                            prompter (seated behind Molly) assisted her using
occurred during play activities at home and school.
                                                            “hand over hand” to pick up the picture and hand it to
                                                            the trainer. If she did not reach within 3–4 seconds, the
Baseline (A1)
                                                            trainer prompted from behind. The receiving trainer
      Molly was observed while in play situations across    held out her hand serving as a cue. Once the picture
all settings before teaching using the PECS. Reinforcer     touched the hand of the receiving trainer, the trainer
assessment was also conducted once before beginning         stated “Oh, you want _____,” and the requested item
baseline. Then in baseline, sessions included a variety     was given to the student. Delays were increased to pro-
of available materials, including those noted as pre-       mote spontaneity and discrimination. The criterion for
ferred items from the reinforcer assessment. These were     Phase I consisted of Molly exchanging the pictures in-
available without contingent requests during the play       dependently without the prompter’s assistance and
time. Data were collected on the frequency of Molly’s       without the open hand cue from the trainer for 80% of
spontaneous language and social interaction across          the teaching period trials (typically 5–10 each session,
4 weeks in all settings (i.e., home, centers, and journal   with 17 trials in the initial session to teach picking up
time). Verbalizations to Molly were typical to the set-     the icon and the exchange). Phase II, Expanding Spon-
tings (i.e., directions for the activity, some questions    taneity, consisted of three steps: the introduction of the
regarding her needs, general commenting). The school        communication board (pictures attached with Velcro),
activities were more independent play with occasional       an increase in the distance of the receiving trainer and
adult interactions, whereas the mother engaged in more      Molly, and an increase in the distance of the board from
chatting during the leisure activities with general com-    Molly. Again, 80% correct, independent requesting was
menting and some contingent questioning (e.g., “What        the criterion. Phase III, Discrimination of Pictures,
do you want?”).                                             consisted of discrimination between multiple pictures
                                                            on the communication board, correspondence checks
Baseline (A2)                                               (use of icons to force correct discriminations based
                                                            upon preferences), and picture size reduction (initial
     During the second baseline condition, the com-
                                                            size 2 2 inches, then 1 1 inches). Phases I–III
munication board with symbols was introduced across
                                                            were taught to criterion in the home setting, and then
all environments, but Molly was not prompted to use
                                                            treatment was implemented in classroom settings.
it. Data were collected under the same conditions as
                                                                  All sessions consisted of both teaching periods and
those described in the A1 condition across 1 week at
                                                            play periods. The procedures of the PECS were taught
home, 12 weeks during centers, and 17 weeks during
                                                            during the teaching periods (5–10 trials, approximately
journal time.
                                                            5 minutes) immediately followed by the free play pe-
                                                            riods (15–20 minutes), during which a choice of items
Treatment (B)
                                                            and activities was available for play contingent on re-
     The PECS was implemented during play activities        questing using the PECS (the same materials as avail-
across all settings following procedures as outlined in     able in baseline, with new items assessed periodically).
the manual (Frost & Bondy, 1994). The PECS is an            Peers at school also received brief training in use of the
AAC system that uses a variety of behavioral tech-          PECS with Molly. Data were collected under the same
niques to teach children to communicate. These tech-        conditions as those described in the A1 and A2 condi-
niques are incorporated into six teaching phases that       tions, during the play periods after training. Note that
target different components of communication (i.e.,         initially during PECS training in the home, reinforcer
spontaneous requesting, discriminating, building sen-       assessment was conducted before training and those
tence structure, responding to questions). Phases I–III     materials were used in training. When free play began,
as outlined in the training protocol were conducted in      however, the mother chose an activity that typically
this study, including reinforcer assessment at the be-      was not an activity from those determined as reinforcers
ginning of each training session (Frost & Bondy, 1994).     based on assessment. Although preferred items from
228                                                                      Kravits, Kamps, Kemmerer, and Potucek

the assessment were still available, the mother directed
Molly to use the item she selected, rather than allow-
ing Molly to choose her activity. Thus, beginning with
the 13th session of the home intervention phase, Molly
was allowed to choose (with the icon initiation) which
items to use during free play. A total of 71 trials over
five training periods was required to reach criterion for
Phases I–III at home, 41 trials over eight sessions were
required in centers, and 15 trials over five sessions were
required in journal time. Once criteria for the phases
were reached, reinforcer assessment and a minimum of
five training trials continued at the beginning of ses-
sions; however, the time for training became much
shorter over time as Molly mastered initiations with
PECS.

Social Intervention with the PECS (C)
     During this condition, the PECS was used in com-
bination with social skills training to increase the du-
ration of Molly’s interaction with her peers. Molly’s
peers were instructed on how to keep her engaged dur-
ing game playing situations (games were used in place
of free choice activities). Peer training was conducted
for four sessions, followed by peer practice (i.e., a few
minutes of models and reminders) conducted before
each play/leisure period. Social skills included sharing
materials, taking turns, asking and answering questions,
and extending the play interactions. Training included
defining the skill, modeling, and practice trials with the
peers and Molly. Data were collected under the same
conditions as described.                                     Fig. 1. Total frequency of spontaneous icon-based language and
                                                             icon-plus verbals (i.e., requests, comments, and expansions) across
                                                             settings.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

     As displayed in Figure 1, the total frequency of        curred in all settings, with 38 learned icons and
spontaneous language using icons or icons plus ver-          4–8 icons used during 10-minute intervention sessions.
bals per 10-minute session increased during settings         Intelligible verbalizations also showed increases in two
when the PECS treatment conditions were imple-               of three settings, home and journal time, with a range
mented. Effects were consistent across home and school       of 15–16 at home and 5–8 at school. Thus, spontaneous
and when PECS was used by the mother, teachers, and          language, which often included icon use but also in-
peers. No use of icons was demonstrated during base-         cluded some verbal language without the icons, in-
line (although icons were present), and with the PECS        creased with the intervention. At home, Molly averaged
instructional protocol, Molly demonstrated successful        8–9 initiations during play in baseline (all verbal) and
use of the augmentative system. An increase in initia-       18 during PECS (icons and verbals). During school cen-
tions at home was noted when materials from the rein-        ters, Molly averaged 3–5 initiations in baseline, with
forcer assessment and training were also available           increases to 11 during the intervention. During journal
during free play, indicating choice as a critical aspect     time, similar effects were noted with a mean of 4 –7
to increasing spontaneity within PECS.                       initiations in baseline and 14 during intervention. These
     Table I. presents mean data by condition for use        findings indicate that increased spontaneous language
of icons, as well as spontaneous verbalizations and du-      included both increased verbalizations and icon use in
ration of social interaction. Reliable use of icons oc-      two settings. The t tests indicated significantly more
PECS                                                                                                                              229

             Table I. Means of Behaviors by Session                tively). These increases reflect increased contact with
                                                                   a small number of peers rather than increases in the
                              A1         A2          B       C
                                                                   number of peers contacted.
Mean frequency of icon use
   Home                       0           0          5.2     4
   Centers                    0           0          4.4     6.5   SUMMARY
   Journal time               0           0.8        7.8     7.6
Mean frequency of intelligible verbalizations                            This study provided an empirical demonstration of
   Home                       9.6         5.8       15.5    16
                                                                   the effectiveness of PECS in increasing spontaneous
   Centers                    2.5         5.3        7.2     5.8
   Journal time               6.0         3.8        6.5     8.3   communication skills for a young child with autism.
Mean frequency of initiations = icon verbalizations or             These findings are consistent with descriptive reports
icons verbalizations or verbalizations alone                       (e.g., Bondy & Frost, 1994; Schwartz et al., 1998) and
   Home                       9.9         8.0       17.4    19.0   experimental reports documenting specific strategies
   Centers                    2.5         5.2       11.2    11.0
                                                                   such as the use of child preference/choice and time
   Journal time               7.7         4.3       14.2    14.3
Mean duration of social interaction with peers                     delay tactics in language intervention (e.g., Dyer, 1989;
(300 seconds total time)                                           Halle, 1982; Koegel et al., 1987). Findings expand prior
   Home                      38           0         16      54     studies by including an older child (kindergarten age)
   Centers                   71          31         54     173     and by including the home as an intervention setting.
   Journal time              60          26        146     183
                                                                   Experimenters also noted that Molly’s use of the sys-
                                                                   tem helped her become a much more spontaneous per-
                                                                   son using the icons in home and school activities. The
                                                                   duration of Molly’s peer interactions increased in jour-
initiations (df    1, F    114.9, p     .01) and verbal-           nal time, and the frequency increased in both, from 0
izations (df    1, F    30.1, p    .01) during interven-           to 2 in baseline to 7 in centers and 13 in journal time
tion sessions over baseline sessions. Observations also            using the PECS, clear documentation for the social va-
indicated, however, that Molly did not significantly in-           lidity of the system in school. Before the PECS, ob-
crease the range of spoken vocabulary during inter-                served communication was nonverbal, more passive
vention. For younger children, it has been reported that           (e.g., gestures, smiles), and primarily directed toward
if verbal language begins, it generally occurs after ap-           the adults. It is unclear, however, what social effects
proximately a year in the PECS program (Bondy, Hoff-               were from PECS alone versus PECS plus the social
man, & Glassberg, 1999). Thus, this case was not a                 skills booster sessions. Limitations thus include the
reliable one for study of the PECS in regards to verbal            confound of the social skills enhancement, a confound
language acquisition due to (a) prior verbal language              to PECS alone as an intervention, and no generaliza-
by Molly as exhibited in baseline and (b) length of the            tion or follow-up probes. Future research with PECS
intervention.                                                      should include (a) implementation with multiple par-
      The duration of social interaction with peers (see           ticipants and those with varying levels of functioning,
Table I) was also monitored to note treatment effects.             (b) long-term study of PECS with completion of the
Minimal changes were noted at home for interaction                 training protocol (advancement through all six training
time with use of the PECS, but this is likely an effect            phases) and use of PECS across longer periods of the
of opportunity as Molly’s brother was the only “peer”              school day, and (c) alternative social interventions in
available at home, with an occasional friend from                  combination with the PECS.
school. At school, however, increases were only noted
with PECS intervention in one of two settings ( journal
                                                                   REFERENCES
time), with increases in the duration to 146 seconds
over baseline levels of 26–60. Differential effects may            Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some dimensions
have been due to peer proximity in the two settings with               of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy-
children seated on the floor 1–2 feet apart in journal                 sis, 1, 91– 97.
                                                                   Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The Picture Exchange Communica-
time play and at tables (3–4 feet apart) during center                 tion System. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 9, 1–19.
play. A few follow-up sessions were conducted using                Bondy, A., Hoffman, A., & Glassberg, W. (January, 1999). The Pic-
PECS and social skills training with peers in centers                  ture Exchange Communication System Training, Kansas City,
                                                                       MO.
and journal time (C), with probes indicating additional            Carta, J., Sainato, D., & Greenwood, C. (1988). Advances in the eco-
increases in both settings (means      173, 183, respec-               logical assessment of classroom instruction for young children
230                                                                                   Kravits, Kamps, Kemmerer, and Potucek

     with handicaps. In S. L. Odom & M. B. Karnes (Eds.), Early in-      Mayer-Johnson, R. (1990). The Picture Communication Symbols,
     tervention for infants and children with handicaps: An empiri-          Vols. I, II, and III. Solana Beach, CA: Mayer-Johnson Co.
     cal base (pp. 217–239). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.      Mirenda, P., & Dattilo, J. (1987). Instructional techniques in alterna-
Charlop, M., Schriebman, L., & Thibodeau, M. (1985). Increasing              tive communication for students with severe intellectual handi-
     spontaneous verbal responding in autistic children using time           caps. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 3, 143–152.
     delay procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18,          Reichle, J., & Sigafoos, J. (1991). Establishing an initial repertoire
     155–166.                                                                of requesting. In J. Reichle, J. York, & J. Sigafoos (Eds.),
Dyer, K. (1989). The effects of preference on spontaneous verbal re-         Implementing augmentative and alternative communication
     quests in individuals with autism. Journal of the Association for       strategies for learners with severe disabilities (pp. 89–114). Bal-
     Persons with Severe Handicaps, 14(3), 184 –189.                         timore: Brookes.
Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (1994). The Picture Communication Exchange        Schwartz, I., Garfinkle, A., & Bauer, J. (1998). The Picture Exchange
     System: Training manual. Pyramid Educational Consultants,               Communication System: Communicative outcomes for young
     Inc., Newark, Delaware.                                                 children with disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special
Halle, J. W. (1982). Teaching functional language to the handi-              Education, 18, 144–159.
     capped: An integrative model of natural environment teaching        Shafer, E. (1993). Teaching topography-based and stimulus selec-
     techniques. Journal of the Association for the Severely Handi-          tion-based verbal behavior to developmentally disabled indi-
     capped, 7, 29–37.                                                       viduals: Some considerations. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior,
Kamps, D., Potucek, J., Gonzalez-Lopez, A., Kravits, T., & Kem-              11, 117–134.
     merer, K. (1997). The use of peer networks across multiple set-     Tapp, J., Wehby, J. H., & Ellis, D. N. (1992). A multiple option ob-
     tings to improve interaction for students with autism. Journal          servation system for experimental studies (M.O.O.S.E.S).
     of Behavioral Education, 7, 335–357.                                    Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University.
Koegel, R. L., O’Dell, M. C., & Koegel, L. K. (1987). A natural lan-     Zangari, C., Lloyd, L. L., & Vicker, B. (1994). Augmentative and
     guage teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Jour-          alternative communication: An historic perspective. Augmenta-
     nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 187–200.                 tive and Alternative Communication, 10, 27–59.

Contenu connexe

En vedette (6)

A review on ipce and pec measurements and materials p.basnet
A review on ipce and pec measurements and materials p.basnetA review on ipce and pec measurements and materials p.basnet
A review on ipce and pec measurements and materials p.basnet
 
Effective presentation strategies
Effective presentation strategiesEffective presentation strategies
Effective presentation strategies
 
Pecs Picture Exchange Communication System
Pecs Picture Exchange Communication SystemPecs Picture Exchange Communication System
Pecs Picture Exchange Communication System
 
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
 
Writing a Report (Tips and Sample of Reports)
Writing a Report (Tips and Sample of Reports)Writing a Report (Tips and Sample of Reports)
Writing a Report (Tips and Sample of Reports)
 
Report Writing - Introduction section
Report Writing - Introduction sectionReport Writing - Introduction section
Report Writing - Introduction section
 

Similaire à Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student with Autism Using the Picture Exchange Communication System

Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docxRunning Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
cowinhelen
 
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxRunning title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
agnesdcarey33086
 
Project proposal final
Project proposal finalProject proposal final
Project proposal final
AnnieDurack
 
Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx
Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docxDevelopmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx
Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx
hcheryl1
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
croysierkathey
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
karenahmanny4c
 
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
Phil Sparrow
 
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
drruether
 
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
drruether
 
A 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docx
A 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docxA 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docx
A 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docx
ransayo
 

Similaire à Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student with Autism Using the Picture Exchange Communication System (20)

Sanjesh
SanjeshSanjesh
Sanjesh
 
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docxRunning Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN                  .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF AUTISM IN CHILDREN .docx
 
Storybook Interventions and Signing DHH Children's Vocabulary
Storybook Interventions and Signing DHH Children's VocabularyStorybook Interventions and Signing DHH Children's Vocabulary
Storybook Interventions and Signing DHH Children's Vocabulary
 
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxRunning title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
 
11.effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder
11.effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder11.effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder
11.effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder
 
Effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder
Effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorderEffectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder
Effectiveness of social stories in children with semantic pragmatic disorder
 
Project proposal final
Project proposal finalProject proposal final
Project proposal final
 
Assistive Technology for Students with Learning Disabilities.pdf
Assistive Technology for Students with Learning Disabilities.pdfAssistive Technology for Students with Learning Disabilities.pdf
Assistive Technology for Students with Learning Disabilities.pdf
 
Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx
Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docxDevelopmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx
Developmental Science. 2018;21e12610. wileyonlinelibrary.com.docx
 
Action Speaks Louder Than Words Young Children Differentially Weight Percept...
Action Speaks Louder Than Words  Young Children Differentially Weight Percept...Action Speaks Louder Than Words  Young Children Differentially Weight Percept...
Action Speaks Louder Than Words Young Children Differentially Weight Percept...
 
Anecdotal Records Valuable Tools For Assessing Young Children S Development
Anecdotal Records  Valuable Tools For Assessing Young Children S DevelopmentAnecdotal Records  Valuable Tools For Assessing Young Children S Development
Anecdotal Records Valuable Tools For Assessing Young Children S Development
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
 
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
 
symbol and referent (Tesha amadea)
symbol and referent (Tesha amadea)symbol and referent (Tesha amadea)
symbol and referent (Tesha amadea)
 
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
 
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
Cognition, culture, & learning media presentation ruether s educ 8401
 
Video analysis of mother-child interactions: The role of experienced clinicia...
Video analysis of mother-child interactions: The role of experienced clinicia...Video analysis of mother-child interactions: The role of experienced clinicia...
Video analysis of mother-child interactions: The role of experienced clinicia...
 
IMPROVING THE COMMUNICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH SPEECH DISORDERS USING THE SMAR...
IMPROVING THE COMMUNICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH SPEECH DISORDERS USING THE SMAR...IMPROVING THE COMMUNICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH SPEECH DISORDERS USING THE SMAR...
IMPROVING THE COMMUNICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH SPEECH DISORDERS USING THE SMAR...
 
A 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docx
A 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docxA 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docx
A 10-year-old nonverbal Greek boy, C.Z., who had beendiagnos.docx
 

Plus de David Pastor Calle

Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...
Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...
Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...
David Pastor Calle
 
Neurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez Alcántara
Neurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez AlcántaraNeurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez Alcántara
Neurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez Alcántara
David Pastor Calle
 

Plus de David Pastor Calle (7)

La Terapia del Lenguaje en el Síndrome X-Frágil - R. Fürgang C.
La Terapia del Lenguaje en el Síndrome X-Frágil - R. Fürgang C.La Terapia del Lenguaje en el Síndrome X-Frágil - R. Fürgang C.
La Terapia del Lenguaje en el Síndrome X-Frágil - R. Fürgang C.
 
Derechos humanos - Alma Pastor Calle
Derechos humanos - Alma Pastor Calle Derechos humanos - Alma Pastor Calle
Derechos humanos - Alma Pastor Calle
 
ENFOQUE DE ATENCIÓN MULTIDISCIPLINAR A UN USUARIO CON ESCLEROSIS TUBEROSA (A...
ENFOQUE DE ATENCIÓN MULTIDISCIPLINAR A UN USUARIO CON ESCLEROSIS TUBEROSA  (A...ENFOQUE DE ATENCIÓN MULTIDISCIPLINAR A UN USUARIO CON ESCLEROSIS TUBEROSA  (A...
ENFOQUE DE ATENCIÓN MULTIDISCIPLINAR A UN USUARIO CON ESCLEROSIS TUBEROSA (A...
 
Taller basico elaboracion Ayudas Visuales - Aura Romero Calavia, Carmen López...
Taller basico elaboracion Ayudas Visuales - Aura Romero Calavia, Carmen López...Taller basico elaboracion Ayudas Visuales - Aura Romero Calavia, Carmen López...
Taller basico elaboracion Ayudas Visuales - Aura Romero Calavia, Carmen López...
 
Comunicación con P.E.C.S. como regulación de la conducta - Rita M. Arteta y P...
Comunicación con P.E.C.S. como regulación de la conducta - Rita M. Arteta y P...Comunicación con P.E.C.S. como regulación de la conducta - Rita M. Arteta y P...
Comunicación con P.E.C.S. como regulación de la conducta - Rita M. Arteta y P...
 
Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...
Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...
Parents’ perspectives on inclusion and schooling of students with angelman sy...
 
Neurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez Alcántara
Neurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez AlcántaraNeurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez Alcántara
Neurobiologia del autismo - J.Enrique Álvarez Alcántara
 

Dernier

Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
ZurliaSoop
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
ciinovamais
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
QucHHunhnh
 
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
KarakKing
 

Dernier (20)

ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptxDyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
Dyslexia AI Workshop for Slideshare.pptx
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptxSKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
SKILL OF INTRODUCING THE LESSON MICRO SKILLS.pptx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
 

Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student with Autism Using the Picture Exchange Communication System

  • 1. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 32, No. 3, June 2002 (© 2002) Brief Reports Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student with Autism Using the Picture Exchange Communication System Tamara R. Kravits,1 Debra M. Kamps,1,4 Katie Kemmerer,2 and Jessica Potucek3 The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) on the spontaneous communication skills of a 6-year-old girl with autism across her home and school environments. The effects of the PECS were also examined for social interaction. Results indicated increases in spontaneous language (i.e., requests and com- ments) including use of the icons and verbalizations across those settings in which PECS was implemented. Intelligible verbalizations increased in two of three settings, and changes in peer social interaction were noted in one of the two school settings. KEY WORDS: Picture Exchange Communication System; augmentative communication; peer training. INTRODUCTION sembles a more naturalistic approach to teaching in that communication is child initiated rather than controlled Augmentative communication systems (AACs) by adult verbal cues. The PECS was developed to have been shown to be a successful language inter- (1) provide an effective AAC for nonverbal children, vention for many nonverbal children (e.g., Reichle & while simultaneously minimizing the prompt depen- Sigafoos, 1991; Shafer, 1993; Zangari, Lloyd, & dency by teaching children to spontaneously initiate Vicker, 1994). Unfortunately, instructional strategies their wants/needs through an exchange of a picture for using AAC systems may rely on the overuse of verbal the corresponding item /activity, and (2) to provide ver- and/or physical prompts such as “What do you want?” bal models of language with use of the picture exchange or “Point to what you want.” As a result, some children to encourage oral language. In addition, PECS combines become prompt dependent and lack spontaneity in their previously researched procedures into the teaching pro- communication (Mirenda & Dattilo, 1987). tocol to encourage child initiation/motivation including A promising instructional intervention with AAC (a) child choice and preference (e.g., Dyer, 1989; is the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987), (b) time delay (e.g., developed by Bondy and Frost (1994), which addresses Charlop, Schreibman, & Thibodeau, 1985; Halle, 1982), these concerns. PECS is structured in a way that re- (c) environmental arrangement (e.g., Carta, Sainato, & Greenwood, 1988), and (d) differential reinforcement 1 University of Kansas, Juniper Gardens Children’s Project, Kansas (e.g., Koegel et al., 1987; Reichle & Sigafoos, 1991). City, Kansas 66101. In descriptive reports, Bondy and Frost (1994) 2 Autism Training Center, University of Louisville, Louisville, Ken- noted improved communication for children with tucky. autism using the PECS as well as increases in sponta- 3 Project S.A.I.L., 10401 Holmes Road, Suite 440, Kansas City, Mis- neous language acquisition; however, few empirical souri 64131. 4 Correspondence should be addressed to Debra M. Kamps, Juniper demonstrations have been reported. In a second de- Gardens Children’s Project, 650 Minnesota Ave., 2nd Floor, Kansas scriptive study with 31 preschoolers with disabilities, City, Kansas 66101; e-mail: dkamps@ukans.edu the use of PECS showed increased spontaneous use of 225 0162-3257/02/0600-0225/0 © 2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation
  • 2. 226 Kravits, Kamps, Kemmerer, and Potucek icons and generalization to novel school settings Measurement (Schwartz, Garfinkle, & Bauer, 1998). Verbalizations increased for talkers but not for nonverbal children The frequency of spontaneous language including in the study. The purpose of the current study was to requests (i.e., words or approximations asking for items (a) evaluate the effectiveness of teaching PECS on the or help), comments (i.e., labeling or describing items, spontaneous communication of an elementary-aged situations), or expansions (adding new elements to pre- child with autism, (b) determine feasibility of use by vious utterances) was selected as the target behavior the mother, classroom teachers, and peers across home (dependent variable) to be treated with the PECS. and school environments, and (c) note effects in con- Requests and comments were counted only if they junction with social skills training for social interaction occurred without prompting (i.e., no instruction, ques- behaviors. tion within 5 seconds prior). Data were collected by the experimenters for lan- guage behaviors that occurred within 10-minute sam- METHOD ple periods. When the student communicated, the observer recorded what the student communicated, the Participant, Settings, Materials mode (e.g., verbal, symbol, sign), the function (e.g., Molly was a 6-year-old girl with a diagnosis of request, comment), and to whom (i.e., adult or peer) the autism. She was integrated, with the assistance of a para- student was communicating. Repeated verbalizations professional, in a half-day kindergarten program in the were recorded as one communicative episode, ending public school. In addition, she received 30–60 minutes after a 5-second pause. of special education services per day from the learning Social interaction data were collected in 5-minute center teacher and language therapist. Molly’s score on intervals using a laptop computer programmed with the the Vineland indicated a 2 years 8 months performance Multi Option Observation System for Experimental level. Scores on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Studies (MOOSES) developed by Tapp, Wehby, and Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-R) were within the 27th Ellis (1992). This system was used to code the dura- percentile for verbal behavior and the 1st percentile for tion of interaction between Molly and her peers or the adaptive behavior. Her developmental age was 2–2.5 adults in her environment. Once an interaction (i.e., ini- years on the Psychoeducational Profile-Revised (PEP- tiation followed by a response within 5 seconds) was R). When prompted, Molly communicated using 1- to observed, it was keyed in and continuously recorded 2-word utterances (e.g., “Want cookie”), but her speech until the observer keyed the end of the interaction; see was difficult to understand, and her frequency of initi- prior reports for program description (Kamps, Potucek, ations was very low. She also used gestures and eye con- Gonzalez-Lopez, Kravits, & Kemmerer, 1997). tact to communicate, but she initiated and used more Language samples and social interaction data were verbalizations at home with her mother than with teach- collected at least once every session across all settings ers or peers at school. Thus, the PECS was chosen to (i.e., two times a week in journal time and centers, two provide Molly with an effective communication system or three times a week at home). across settings and to increase her spontaneous initia- Interobserver agreement was collected for each tions and interactions with others in her environment. language variable for 11% of the observation sessions. Settings for the study included leisure and snack The mean percentage of agreement for content of the time in Molly’s home and play periods with peers dur- verbalizations was 93% in baseline and 91% in inter- ing journal (writing/coloring in notebooks followed by vention; 97% in baseline for mode (icon versus sign ver- play) and center activities (free play) at school. All set- sus verbal) and 96% in intervention; and 89% in tings were similar to free time with play with others as baseline for function (i.e., spontaneous request versus a primary activity and the inclusion of peers at school. comment) and 86% in intervention. Reliability was Materials consisted of food (e.g., popcorn, candy, soda, computed on an item-by-item basis, with the number cookies), toys (e.g., Casio piano, Koosh Balls, Slinkys, of agreements divided by the number of agreements markers, Silly Putty), and games (e.g., Don’t Break the plus disagreements. Reliability for social interaction Ice). Graphic symbols/icons (line drawings) of items data for 14% of the MOOSES files was computed me- were taken from the Mayer-Johnson Picture Commu- chanically using software that matched the files of the nication Symbols (1990) or were hand drawn (2 2- primary and second observers with the mean agreement inch black and white). at 86%.
  • 3. PECS 227 Design and Procedures Phase I, Physically-Assisted Exchange, consisted of teaching Molly to initiate a communicative exchange A multiple baseline design across settings (Baer, by giving her a picture of a desired item/activity to the Wolf, & Risley, 1968) was used to document treatment trainer. Training consisted of placing the item slightly effectiveness. The experimental conditions included out of her reach. When she reached for the item, a two baseline conditions and two treatment phases that prompter (seated behind Molly) assisted her using occurred during play activities at home and school. “hand over hand” to pick up the picture and hand it to the trainer. If she did not reach within 3–4 seconds, the Baseline (A1) trainer prompted from behind. The receiving trainer Molly was observed while in play situations across held out her hand serving as a cue. Once the picture all settings before teaching using the PECS. Reinforcer touched the hand of the receiving trainer, the trainer assessment was also conducted once before beginning stated “Oh, you want _____,” and the requested item baseline. Then in baseline, sessions included a variety was given to the student. Delays were increased to pro- of available materials, including those noted as pre- mote spontaneity and discrimination. The criterion for ferred items from the reinforcer assessment. These were Phase I consisted of Molly exchanging the pictures in- available without contingent requests during the play dependently without the prompter’s assistance and time. Data were collected on the frequency of Molly’s without the open hand cue from the trainer for 80% of spontaneous language and social interaction across the teaching period trials (typically 5–10 each session, 4 weeks in all settings (i.e., home, centers, and journal with 17 trials in the initial session to teach picking up time). Verbalizations to Molly were typical to the set- the icon and the exchange). Phase II, Expanding Spon- tings (i.e., directions for the activity, some questions taneity, consisted of three steps: the introduction of the regarding her needs, general commenting). The school communication board (pictures attached with Velcro), activities were more independent play with occasional an increase in the distance of the receiving trainer and adult interactions, whereas the mother engaged in more Molly, and an increase in the distance of the board from chatting during the leisure activities with general com- Molly. Again, 80% correct, independent requesting was menting and some contingent questioning (e.g., “What the criterion. Phase III, Discrimination of Pictures, do you want?”). consisted of discrimination between multiple pictures on the communication board, correspondence checks Baseline (A2) (use of icons to force correct discriminations based upon preferences), and picture size reduction (initial During the second baseline condition, the com- size 2 2 inches, then 1 1 inches). Phases I–III munication board with symbols was introduced across were taught to criterion in the home setting, and then all environments, but Molly was not prompted to use treatment was implemented in classroom settings. it. Data were collected under the same conditions as All sessions consisted of both teaching periods and those described in the A1 condition across 1 week at play periods. The procedures of the PECS were taught home, 12 weeks during centers, and 17 weeks during during the teaching periods (5–10 trials, approximately journal time. 5 minutes) immediately followed by the free play pe- riods (15–20 minutes), during which a choice of items Treatment (B) and activities was available for play contingent on re- The PECS was implemented during play activities questing using the PECS (the same materials as avail- across all settings following procedures as outlined in able in baseline, with new items assessed periodically). the manual (Frost & Bondy, 1994). The PECS is an Peers at school also received brief training in use of the AAC system that uses a variety of behavioral tech- PECS with Molly. Data were collected under the same niques to teach children to communicate. These tech- conditions as those described in the A1 and A2 condi- niques are incorporated into six teaching phases that tions, during the play periods after training. Note that target different components of communication (i.e., initially during PECS training in the home, reinforcer spontaneous requesting, discriminating, building sen- assessment was conducted before training and those tence structure, responding to questions). Phases I–III materials were used in training. When free play began, as outlined in the training protocol were conducted in however, the mother chose an activity that typically this study, including reinforcer assessment at the be- was not an activity from those determined as reinforcers ginning of each training session (Frost & Bondy, 1994). based on assessment. Although preferred items from
  • 4. 228 Kravits, Kamps, Kemmerer, and Potucek the assessment were still available, the mother directed Molly to use the item she selected, rather than allow- ing Molly to choose her activity. Thus, beginning with the 13th session of the home intervention phase, Molly was allowed to choose (with the icon initiation) which items to use during free play. A total of 71 trials over five training periods was required to reach criterion for Phases I–III at home, 41 trials over eight sessions were required in centers, and 15 trials over five sessions were required in journal time. Once criteria for the phases were reached, reinforcer assessment and a minimum of five training trials continued at the beginning of ses- sions; however, the time for training became much shorter over time as Molly mastered initiations with PECS. Social Intervention with the PECS (C) During this condition, the PECS was used in com- bination with social skills training to increase the du- ration of Molly’s interaction with her peers. Molly’s peers were instructed on how to keep her engaged dur- ing game playing situations (games were used in place of free choice activities). Peer training was conducted for four sessions, followed by peer practice (i.e., a few minutes of models and reminders) conducted before each play/leisure period. Social skills included sharing materials, taking turns, asking and answering questions, and extending the play interactions. Training included defining the skill, modeling, and practice trials with the peers and Molly. Data were collected under the same conditions as described. Fig. 1. Total frequency of spontaneous icon-based language and icon-plus verbals (i.e., requests, comments, and expansions) across settings. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION As displayed in Figure 1, the total frequency of curred in all settings, with 38 learned icons and spontaneous language using icons or icons plus ver- 4–8 icons used during 10-minute intervention sessions. bals per 10-minute session increased during settings Intelligible verbalizations also showed increases in two when the PECS treatment conditions were imple- of three settings, home and journal time, with a range mented. Effects were consistent across home and school of 15–16 at home and 5–8 at school. Thus, spontaneous and when PECS was used by the mother, teachers, and language, which often included icon use but also in- peers. No use of icons was demonstrated during base- cluded some verbal language without the icons, in- line (although icons were present), and with the PECS creased with the intervention. At home, Molly averaged instructional protocol, Molly demonstrated successful 8–9 initiations during play in baseline (all verbal) and use of the augmentative system. An increase in initia- 18 during PECS (icons and verbals). During school cen- tions at home was noted when materials from the rein- ters, Molly averaged 3–5 initiations in baseline, with forcer assessment and training were also available increases to 11 during the intervention. During journal during free play, indicating choice as a critical aspect time, similar effects were noted with a mean of 4 –7 to increasing spontaneity within PECS. initiations in baseline and 14 during intervention. These Table I. presents mean data by condition for use findings indicate that increased spontaneous language of icons, as well as spontaneous verbalizations and du- included both increased verbalizations and icon use in ration of social interaction. Reliable use of icons oc- two settings. The t tests indicated significantly more
  • 5. PECS 229 Table I. Means of Behaviors by Session tively). These increases reflect increased contact with a small number of peers rather than increases in the A1 A2 B C number of peers contacted. Mean frequency of icon use Home 0 0 5.2 4 Centers 0 0 4.4 6.5 SUMMARY Journal time 0 0.8 7.8 7.6 Mean frequency of intelligible verbalizations This study provided an empirical demonstration of Home 9.6 5.8 15.5 16 the effectiveness of PECS in increasing spontaneous Centers 2.5 5.3 7.2 5.8 Journal time 6.0 3.8 6.5 8.3 communication skills for a young child with autism. Mean frequency of initiations = icon verbalizations or These findings are consistent with descriptive reports icons verbalizations or verbalizations alone (e.g., Bondy & Frost, 1994; Schwartz et al., 1998) and Home 9.9 8.0 17.4 19.0 experimental reports documenting specific strategies Centers 2.5 5.2 11.2 11.0 such as the use of child preference/choice and time Journal time 7.7 4.3 14.2 14.3 Mean duration of social interaction with peers delay tactics in language intervention (e.g., Dyer, 1989; (300 seconds total time) Halle, 1982; Koegel et al., 1987). Findings expand prior Home 38 0 16 54 studies by including an older child (kindergarten age) Centers 71 31 54 173 and by including the home as an intervention setting. Journal time 60 26 146 183 Experimenters also noted that Molly’s use of the sys- tem helped her become a much more spontaneous per- son using the icons in home and school activities. The duration of Molly’s peer interactions increased in jour- initiations (df 1, F 114.9, p .01) and verbal- nal time, and the frequency increased in both, from 0 izations (df 1, F 30.1, p .01) during interven- to 2 in baseline to 7 in centers and 13 in journal time tion sessions over baseline sessions. Observations also using the PECS, clear documentation for the social va- indicated, however, that Molly did not significantly in- lidity of the system in school. Before the PECS, ob- crease the range of spoken vocabulary during inter- served communication was nonverbal, more passive vention. For younger children, it has been reported that (e.g., gestures, smiles), and primarily directed toward if verbal language begins, it generally occurs after ap- the adults. It is unclear, however, what social effects proximately a year in the PECS program (Bondy, Hoff- were from PECS alone versus PECS plus the social man, & Glassberg, 1999). Thus, this case was not a skills booster sessions. Limitations thus include the reliable one for study of the PECS in regards to verbal confound of the social skills enhancement, a confound language acquisition due to (a) prior verbal language to PECS alone as an intervention, and no generaliza- by Molly as exhibited in baseline and (b) length of the tion or follow-up probes. Future research with PECS intervention. should include (a) implementation with multiple par- The duration of social interaction with peers (see ticipants and those with varying levels of functioning, Table I) was also monitored to note treatment effects. (b) long-term study of PECS with completion of the Minimal changes were noted at home for interaction training protocol (advancement through all six training time with use of the PECS, but this is likely an effect phases) and use of PECS across longer periods of the of opportunity as Molly’s brother was the only “peer” school day, and (c) alternative social interventions in available at home, with an occasional friend from combination with the PECS. school. At school, however, increases were only noted with PECS intervention in one of two settings ( journal REFERENCES time), with increases in the duration to 146 seconds over baseline levels of 26–60. Differential effects may Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some dimensions have been due to peer proximity in the two settings with of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy- children seated on the floor 1–2 feet apart in journal sis, 1, 91– 97. Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The Picture Exchange Communica- time play and at tables (3–4 feet apart) during center tion System. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 9, 1–19. play. A few follow-up sessions were conducted using Bondy, A., Hoffman, A., & Glassberg, W. (January, 1999). The Pic- PECS and social skills training with peers in centers ture Exchange Communication System Training, Kansas City, MO. and journal time (C), with probes indicating additional Carta, J., Sainato, D., & Greenwood, C. (1988). Advances in the eco- increases in both settings (means 173, 183, respec- logical assessment of classroom instruction for young children
  • 6. 230 Kravits, Kamps, Kemmerer, and Potucek with handicaps. In S. L. Odom & M. B. Karnes (Eds.), Early in- Mayer-Johnson, R. (1990). The Picture Communication Symbols, tervention for infants and children with handicaps: An empiri- Vols. I, II, and III. Solana Beach, CA: Mayer-Johnson Co. cal base (pp. 217–239). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. Mirenda, P., & Dattilo, J. (1987). Instructional techniques in alterna- Charlop, M., Schriebman, L., & Thibodeau, M. (1985). Increasing tive communication for students with severe intellectual handi- spontaneous verbal responding in autistic children using time caps. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 3, 143–152. delay procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, Reichle, J., & Sigafoos, J. (1991). Establishing an initial repertoire 155–166. of requesting. In J. Reichle, J. York, & J. Sigafoos (Eds.), Dyer, K. (1989). The effects of preference on spontaneous verbal re- Implementing augmentative and alternative communication quests in individuals with autism. Journal of the Association for strategies for learners with severe disabilities (pp. 89–114). Bal- Persons with Severe Handicaps, 14(3), 184 –189. timore: Brookes. Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (1994). The Picture Communication Exchange Schwartz, I., Garfinkle, A., & Bauer, J. (1998). The Picture Exchange System: Training manual. Pyramid Educational Consultants, Communication System: Communicative outcomes for young Inc., Newark, Delaware. children with disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Halle, J. W. (1982). Teaching functional language to the handi- Education, 18, 144–159. capped: An integrative model of natural environment teaching Shafer, E. (1993). Teaching topography-based and stimulus selec- techniques. Journal of the Association for the Severely Handi- tion-based verbal behavior to developmentally disabled indi- capped, 7, 29–37. viduals: Some considerations. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, Kamps, D., Potucek, J., Gonzalez-Lopez, A., Kravits, T., & Kem- 11, 117–134. merer, K. (1997). The use of peer networks across multiple set- Tapp, J., Wehby, J. H., & Ellis, D. N. (1992). A multiple option ob- tings to improve interaction for students with autism. Journal servation system for experimental studies (M.O.O.S.E.S). of Behavioral Education, 7, 335–357. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University. Koegel, R. L., O’Dell, M. C., & Koegel, L. K. (1987). A natural lan- Zangari, C., Lloyd, L. L., & Vicker, B. (1994). Augmentative and guage teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Jour- alternative communication: An historic perspective. Augmenta- nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 187–200. tive and Alternative Communication, 10, 27–59.