SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  51
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS
REDUCING BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS THROUGH
FUNCTIONAL
COMMUNICATION TRAINING
EDWARD G. CARR AND V. MARK DURAND
STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK AT STONY BROOK,
STATE UNIVERSITY OF
NEW YORK AT ALBANY, AND SUFFOLK CHILD
DEVELOPMENT CENTER
It is generally agreed that serious misbehavior in children
should be replaced with socially appro-
priate behaviors, but few guidelines exist with respect to
choosing replacement behaviors. We
address this issue in two experiments. In Experiment 1, we
developed an assessment method for
identifying situations in which behavior problems, induding
aggression, tantrums, and self-injury,
were most likely to occur. Results demonstrated that both low
level of adult attention and high
level of task difficulty were discriminative for misbehavior. In
Experiment 2, the assessment data
were used to select replacements for misbehavior. Specifically,
children were taught to solicit atten-
tion or assistance or both verbally from adults. This treatment,
which involved the differential
reinforcement of functional communication, produced replicable
suppression of behavior problems
across four developmentally disabled children. The results were
consistent with an hypothesis stating
that some child behavior problems may be viewed as a
nonverbal means of communication.
According to this hypothesis, behavior problems and verbal
communicative acts, though differing
in form, may be equivalent in function. Therefore, strengthening
the latter should weaken the
former.
DESCRIPTORS: disruptive behavior, assessment, classroom
behavior, communication, devel-
opmentally disabled children
A major portion of child behavior therapy is
justifiably concerned with the treatment of behav-
ior problems, given that such problems can seri-
ously disrupt the educational process (O'Leary &
O'Leary, 1977; Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977)
and in some cases may lead to institutionalization
This investigation was supported in part by U.S.P.H.S.
Biomedical Research Support Grant 2 S07 RR-07067-18
to the State University of New York at Stony Brook to the
first author and a Sigma Xi Grant-in-Aid of Research to the
second author. Portions of this paper were presented at the
annual meeting of the American Psychological Association,
Washington, D.C., August 1982, and Anaheim, California,
August 1983. This research was based on a master's thesis
conducted by the second author under the direction of the
first author.
We thank Martin Hamburg, Executive Director, Suffolk
Child Development Center, for his generous support, and
Roseann D'Evanzo, JoAnn Giles, Terry Leykis, Cathy Sher-
edos, and Doug Walters for assistance with data collection.
Finally, we thank Alan 0. Ross, Susan G. O'Leary, K. Dan-
iel O'Leary, Crighton Newsom, Paul A. Dores, and Daniel
B. Crimmins for their helpful comments.
Requests for reprints or individual data should be sent to
Edward Carr, Department of Psychology, State University
of New York, Stony Brook, New York 11794-2500.
(Quay, 1979). Behaviorists have developed treat-
ment strategies designed to decelerate problem be-
haviors, which indude procedures involving ex-
tinction (Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, & Kassorla, 1965),
time-out (Zeilberger, Sampen, & Sloane, 1968),
response cost (Iwata & Bailey, 1974), overcorrec-
tion (Foxx & Bechtel, 1983), contingent electric
shock (Carr & Lovaas, 1983; Lovaas & Simmons,
1969), and ecological interventions (Durand,
1983). The focus has been on eliminating behavior
problems, particularly those such as self-injury,
aggression, and tantrums, that are serious enough
to jeopardize the safety and effective functioning
of the target children and their peers.
There is consensus among researchers and cli-
nicians that the elimination of behavior problems
is an important first step in remediation. Ulti-
mately, however, the problematic responses must
be replaced with socially usefuil behaviors (Goldia-
mond, 1974). Because there are few guidelines
available to suggest what these replacement be-
haviors should be (Donnellan, Mirenda, Mesaros,
& Fassbender, 1983; Voeltz, Evans, Derer, &
111
1985, 189 111-126 NumBER 2 (summER 1985)
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
Hanashiro, 1983), we sought to develop a method
for identifying and assessing those educational sit-
uations in which behavior problems reliably occur
(Experiment 1) and, second, we sought to use the
assessment information in order to select replace-
ment behaviors (Experiment 2).
Research on the functional analysis of behavior
problems provides a good basis on which to build
identification and assessment methods. This liter-
ature indicates that the factors responsible for the
maintenance of behavior problems fall into two
broad dasses: escape behavior, controlled by neg-
ative reinforcement processes, and attention-seek-
ing behavior, controlled by positive reinforcement
processes (Carr & Durand, 1985).
There is ample evidence to suggest that many
children learn to emit behavior problems in the
presence of aversive stimuli. The display of such
problems frequently results in the removal of these
stimuli, a dear example of a negative reinforce-
ment process (Patterson, 1982). In the dassroom
setting, instructional demands may frequently
function as aversive stimuli and a variety of be-
havior problems induding aggression, self-injury,
and tantrums may serve as escape behaviors that
effectively allow the child to avoid further partic-
ipation in instructional tasks (Carr, 1977; Carr &
Newsom, in press; Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff,
1976, 1980; Durand, 1982; Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer,
Bauman, & Richman, 1982; Romanczyk, Colletti,
& Plotkin, 1980; Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981).
The literature also indicates that the display of
behavior problems often results in the child's re-
ceiving attention, a dear example of a positive
reinforcement process (Patterson, 1982; Wahler,
1976). Many investigators have presented data
congruent with the notion that behavior problems
may be a form of attention-seeking (Carr &
McDowell, 1980; Iwata et al., 1982; Lovaas et
al., 1965; Patterson, 1980; Wahler, 1969). These
studies also imply that children may learn to emit
behavior problems in response to low levels of adult
attention.
Given the results of the empirical investigations
to date, it is dear that any method used to assess
behavior problems should indude an analysis of
the effects of level of attention (to identify possible
attention-seeking functions) as well as an analysis
of the effects of task difficulty (to identify possible
escape functions). This rationale formed the basis
for the assessment procedures used in Experi-
ment 1.
Once it has been determined that a behavior
problem likely serves a specific social function (e.g.,
escape or attention seeking), one is in a position to
consider appropriate replacement behaviors. One
behavioral alternative to escape would be some
form of assistance seeking. For example, it may be
possible to teach the child a response that is effec-
tive in evoking teacher assistance with a difficult
task. Once assistance is provided, the task should
no longer be as aversive and therefore escape be-
haviors should decrease. Likewise, in the case of
behavior problems that are attention seeking, one
could teach the child an appropriate alternative
response that is effective in securing adult atten-
tion.
An important question concerns the form of the
response alternatives to be taught. Several studies
suggest that children can be taught to solicit at-
tention and assistance verbally (Seymour & Stokes,
1976; Stokes, Fowler, & Baer, 1978). Verbal
communication training is not typically used as a
method for controlling behavior problems. None-
theless, the analysis just presented suggests that if
communicative phrases are carefully chosen so that
they serve the same presumptive social functions
as the behavior problems they are to replace, then
deceleration of these problems should be possible.
This training strategy was explored in Experi-
ment 2.
EXPERIMENT 1
METHOD
Children and Setting
Teachers in a day school program for develop-
mentally disabled children were interviewed, and
the first four children who met both the following
criteria were selected for indusion in this study:
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING
They displayed at least one problem behavior per
hour in the classroom and had expressive language
skill consisting, at a minimum, of single word ut-
terances. Based on these criteria, two males and
two females were chosen. Jim and Sue were 13
years old; Eve, 14; and Tom, 7. The medical staff
had diagnosed Jim as autistic; Sue and Eve as brain
damaged; and Tom as developmentally delayed
and severely hearing impaired. Tom wore a hear-
ing aid. On the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test,
the children's mental age scores were: Jim, 3 years;
Sue, 5 years, 10 months; Eve, 2 years, 5 months;
and Tom, 5 years. They displayed a variety of
aggressive, self-destructive, and disruptive behav-
iors that are defined in detail later. Jim and Tom
spoke spontaneously in complete sentences. Sue
spoke spontaneously in phrases of 2-3 words. Eve
was limited to using single noun labels, primarily
when prompted.
Sessions were conducted in a 5 x 10-m auxil-
iary dassroom located next to the regular dass-
room. The experimenter sat between two of the
children at a table on which various task materials
were placed. All materials were placed 0.3 m from
the child so that he or she could readily reach
them. If more than one set of materials was used
at a time, they were evenly spaced in front of the
child, also at 0.3 m distance. Small-group (two
children) instruction was used to approximate the
regular dassroom practice.
Experimental Design and Overview
In a given session, a child received either an easy
task or a difficult task; in addition, a child received
adult attention during either 100% or 33% of the
time intervals into which the session was divided.
The two levels of task difficulty and the two levels
of adult attention were combined to produce three
conditions: easy 100, easy 33, and difficult 100.
Comparison of easy 100 with easy 33 permitted
assessment of the effects of attention level (i.e.,
100% versus 33%) while task difficulty was held
constant. Comparison of easy 100 with difficult
100 permitted assessment of the effects of task
difficulty while attention level was held constant.
Easy 100 was designated as the baseline condition
with which the other two conditions were com-
pared. This designation was based on teacher re-
ports and our own informal dassroom observations
which suggested that a combination of easy tasks
and high levels of teacher attention generally re-
sulted in a low frequency of behavior problems.
The easy 33 and difficult 100 conditions alter-
nated with the baseline easy 100 condition in a
reversal design. The sequence of conditions was
counterbalanced across children to control for order
effects. Session length was always 10 min and 1-
3 sessions were run per day. When multiple ses-
sions were run on the same day, there was a 5-min
break between sessions.
Procedure
Easy 100. In easy 100, a child worked on
receptive labeling and match-to-sample and re-
ceived some form of adult attention for doing so
in 100% of the intervals in each session as deter-
mined by a time sampling procedure described
later. In the receptive labeling task, the child was
presented with several cards from the Peabody Pic-
ture Vocabulary Test, a measure of receptive la-
beling skill that is based on a series of picture cards
graded in difficulty. Each card on this test is di-
vided into quadrants, one picture per quadrant. A
card was placed on the table in front of the child
and the child was asked to point to the relevant
picture named by the adult (e.g., "Point to the
ball"). To ensure that this task was indeed easy,
an additional assessment was conducted prior to
the start of this condition. The child was twice
presented with the Peabody cards, and 20 cards
were selected on which the child always responded
correctly. These cards constituted the materials for
the receptive task.
For the match-to-sample task, 3 cards were se-
lected at random from the group of 20 cards de-
scribed above. One picture from each of the 3
cards was randomly chosen and 11 copies were
made of each picture. The three different pictures
were placed in a row in front of the child. These
pictures constituted the samples. The remaining 30
pictures (i.e., 10 copies of each picture) were mixed
together and placed in a pile 0.1 m behind the
113
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
samples. The adult pointed to the card at the top
of the pile of copies and said to the child "Match
this" or an equivalent statement. The child was
then expected to place the card on top of the cor-
rect sample. The children had considerable expe-
rience on matching tasks and therefore did not
require any prompts to match each card correctly.
Typically, they would match several cards from
the pile following a single command from the adult.
Whenever they finished matching all the cards, the
adult would gather up the copies, mix them to-
gether, and again place them behind the samples.
The procedure was then recyded.
On those rare occasions on which a child made
an error on either the match-to-sample or receptive
labeling tasks, the adult would say "No!" or
"That's not correct!" and go on to the next trial.
Correct responding produced verbal praise (defined
later). The two tasks were alternated, 5 min each,
within each 10-min session. The task sequence was
randomized across sessions.
In each session, the adult would deliver atten-
tion in the form of mands, praise, and comments.
A mand (e.g., "Point to the " on the recep-
tive labeling task, and "Match this" on the match-
to-sample task) was presented in every third re-
cording interval. Praise was given in a different
third of the intervals, either contingent on correct
responding (e.g., "That's right!") or contingent on
general task-related behavior (e.g., "You're work-
ing very nicely!"). While giving verbal approval,
the adult made eye contact with the child, smiled
or nodded or both, and delivered physical approval
in the form of pats on the shoulder, mussing the
child's hair, tickling, and related actions. Finally,
comments were made in yet a different third of
the intervals and consisted of a variety of descrip-
tive statements (e.g., "It's sunny today.").
The adult was cued by a bug-in-the-ear device
as to when to deliver attention (i.e., a beep oc-
curred every 10 s). In addition, the adult kept a
written tally of the various forms of attention de-
livered, which helped ensure that the three forms
of attention were given equally throughout the ses-
sion. Some form of adult attention (i.e., mands,
praise, or comments) was given in every interval
of the session, with the three forms being presented
in a random sequence within and between sessions.
A new trial began, every 30 s on the average, with
the presentation of a mand. Thus, there were 20
mands given during each 10-min session. This
procedure was in effect during all conditions. The
other child who was seated at the table was given
independent desk work to do while the session was
being run. When the adult was not attending to
the target child, the adult attended to (i.e., in-
structed and praised) the second child.
When a child displayed a behavior problem,
the experimenter reacted as follows. If the child
left his or her seat, the experimenter waited 10 s
for the child to return. If the child did not return,
the experimenter led the child back to his or her
seat without comment. The experimenter ignored
all other behavior problems (i.e., made no com-
ment) and continued with the task at hand unless
the behavior posed a physical risk. In that case,
the experimenter restrained the child. For example,
if the child struck the experimenter hard, the ex-
perimenter would grasp the child's hand and re-
strain it on the child's lap for a period of 5-10 s
while the experimenter would continue with the
task at hand. This procedure was in effect during
all experimental conditions.
In this and subsequent conditions, approxi-
mately one-third of the sessions were conducted by
an adult (randomly chosen from a pool of five
adults) who was naive to the purpose of the ex-
periment. The other sessions were conducted by
the second author.
Easy 33. In the regular dassroom, teacher at-
tention was typically low during independent work
assignments. Because match-to-sample was the
most commonly used task for developing indepen-
dent work skills, we chose it to assess the effects
of low rates of adult attention on the level of be-
havior problems. This test was consistent with
classroom practice and was a task that the children
could complete without error. The sessions were
conducted as in easy 100 but the amount of adult
attention was decreased. Specifically, mands and
praise were each presented during one-third of the
recording intervals as before; however, they were
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING
now programmed within the same interval rather
than in different intervals as had been the case for
easy 100. Also, comments were discontinued. These
changes resulted in a decrease in overall adult at-
tention from 100% to 33% of the intervals, with-
out altering the amount of praise or mands given.
That is, in the one interval out of three that in-
duded attention, both praise and mands occurred
at a level equal to that in easy 100. During those
periods of time in which the adult was not attend-
ing to the target child, the adult worked with the
other child seated at the table.
Difficult 100. In the regular classroom, vocab-
ulary tasks typically generated many errors. Be-
cause receptive labeling of picture cards from a
Peabody language development kit was one of the
most commonly used tasks for building vocabu-
lary, we chose a similar task for difficult 100, to
be consistent with dassroom practice. To ensure
that the task was indeed difficult, we carried out
an additional assessment prior to the start of this
condition. Specifically, cards from the Peabody Pic-
ture Vocabulary Test were selected on which the
child scored at chance levels with respect to recep-
tive labeling (25% correct). The method for se-
lecting these cards was the same as that used in
easy 100. In all other respects, this condition was
run in the same manner as easy 100.
Response Definitions and Reliability
Three classes of responses were recorded: dis-
ruptive behavior, adult attention, and academic
behavior. Based on our preliminary classroom ob-
servations as well as teacher reports, we identified
three to five common topographies of disruptive
behavior for each child. These topographies in-
cluded aggression for all four children (poking or
hitting other people, or pulling their hair; striking
or knocking over objects); tantrums for Jim, Tom,
and Sue (any loud voalization or screaming ac-
companied by whining or crying); self-injury for
Jim, Eve, and Sue (hitting one's head with one's
hand; biting one's hand); opposition for Jim, Eve,
and Sue (saying "No" to an adult's request or
pushing away the task materials); out of seat for
Tom and Sue (child's buttocks breaking contact
with the seat of the chair for 3 s or more); and
stripping in the case of Eve (removing any article
of clothing from one's body).
Adult attention consisted of praise, mands, and
comments. Praise was defined as any form of ver-
bal approval delivered contingent on correct re-
sponding to a task (e.g., "That's right!") or con-
tingent on general cooperative behavior (e.g., "I
like the way you're working today!"). Mands were
defined as any task-related request made by the
adult (e.g., "Point to the truck" for the receptive
labeling task, or "Match this" for the match-to-
sample task). Comments were defined as any de-
scriptive remarks made by the adult (e.g., "There
sure are a lot of pictures," or "It's sunny today.").
Academic behavior was defined separately for
the two tasks. On the receptive task, a correct
response was scored if the child pointed to the
picture named by the adult. An incorrect response
was scored if the child pointed to one of the other
three pictures on the Peabody card or failed to
respond within 10 s. On the match-to-sample task,
a correct response was scored if the child responded
to the adult's command by placing one of the
copies of the pictures on top of the appropriate
sample. An incorrect response was scored if the
child placed the copy on top of the wrong sample
or failed to respond within 10 s.
All responses were recorded using a continuous
10-s interval procedure. Observers sat in a corner
of the room, 2.5 m from the child, and out of the
child's line of sight. A tape recorder equipped with
earphones emitted the recording interval number
at the end of each 10-s interval. The presence or
absence of the responses previously defined was
recorded for each interval.
Reliability observers were drawn two at a time
from a pool of four undergraduate students. All
observers were trained prior to the investigation by
recording in classrooms. Training proceeded until
the observers reached a criterion of 75% agreement
on all behavior categories with one standard ob-
server (an undergraduate who had extensive ex-
perience in behavioral recording). During the ex-
periment, reliability was assessed in 70% of the
sessions conducted for each child. Observer records
115
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
were compared on an interval-by-interval basis. For
disruptive behavior and adult attention, the reli-
ability index used was the number of agreements
divided by the number of agreements plus dis-
agreements multiplied by 100. Academic behav-
iors were scored on a trial-by-trial basis to yield
percent correct figures. The mean interobserver re-
liability was 80% or higher for all response cate-
gories.
REsuLTS
Validation of Task Difficulty and
Adult Attention Manipulations
For brevity, only group averages are reported;
however, in all cases, individual data were consis-
tent with the group means.
With respect to task difficulty, the mean percent
correct observed across the four children was 96.5%
in easy 100, 97.3% in easy 33, and 26.9% in
difficult 100. With respect to adult attention, the
mean level of this variable observed across the four
children was 99.7% in easy 100, 33.4% in easy
33, and 99.9% in difficult 100. Thus, we suc-
ceeded in creating two levels of task difficulty (an
easy level approximating 100% correct and a dif-
ficult level approximating 25% correct), and two
levels of adult attention (a high level approximat-
ing 100% attention and a low level approximating
33% attention). Finally, praise, mands, and com-
ments each occurred at an average of approxi-
mately 33% in all conditions except during easy
33 in which comments occurred at zero or near-
zero levels as planned.
Effects on Disruptive Behavior
Figure 1 shows the percentage of intervals in
which disruptive behavior occurred during each
session for the four children. There were three dis-
tinct patterns of disruptive behavior. Jim and Eve
were disruptive primarily in the difficult 100 con-
dition. Tom was disruptive primarily in the easy
33 condition, and Sue was disruptive in both easy
33 and difficult 100. As expected, disruptive be-
havior in easy 100 was negligible for all children
thus justifying our use of this condition as a base-
line. The filled circles in Figure 1 depict the results
of sessions run by naive experimenters. As can be
seen, there were no systematic differences between
these data points and those generated by the in-
formed experimenter (open circles).
DISCUSSION
The fact that there were several patterns of dis-
ruptive behavior displayed in Experiment 1 sug-
gests that more than one variable was controlling
the children's behavior. There is some evidence in
the published literature (Carmine, 1976) that long
intertrial intervals, such as those we used, may
contribute to off-task behavior. Nonetheless, we
felt that the use of long intervals was justified given
that a major educational goal for these children
involved group instruction, a situation in which
long intervals are inevitable. In fact, long intervals
did not in themselves guarantee disruptive behav-
ior. The nature of the task and level of adult at-
tention were the most reliable predictors.
Consider Jim and Eve. Their behavior problems
became more frequent when demands increased in
difficulty (difficult 100) but these children were
relatively well behaved in the other two conditions.
This behavior pattern is consistent with the notion
of escape responding. Specifically, it is hypothe-
sized that some children have a history of success-
fiully escaping from presumably aversive stimuli
(such as difficult task demands and ensuing failure)
contingent on the display of disruptive behavior.
If a child has this history, then difficult task de-
mands may eventually come to be discriminative
for the emission of problem behavior.
Tom's results suggest the operation of a second
controlling variable. His behavior problems be-
came more frequent when the overall level of adult
attention was reduced (easy 33) but he was rela-
tively well behaved in the other two conditions.
This pattern of behavior is consistent with the no-
tion of attention seeking. Specifically, it is hypoth-
esized that some children have experienced the fol-
lowing set of contingencies. First, the amount of
adult attention given to the child decreases to a
low level. From time to time, when the child mis-
behaves, the adult attends to the child. The cu-
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING
J im
Easy Easy Easy Diff Easy Diff100 5 33 WIOT E 100 100 100
o..0o h o of o .
. .U
E v e
501
21
0-
Tom
100
50J
01
a
II,
E a s y
100
EasyI -i- ~
E asy
100
A _~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~
D i f f
100
Eas
Easy
33
9-7
E asy
100
Easy
33 :
Easy
100
Easy
33
Easy
100
I. . I . . I p
Eas Easy Easy
3 3
Diff
100 EasY10e
I .I I - - i. - - I -
Diff
100
Easy
100 I
b ~JoenOI
Diff Easy Diff Easy
100 1 100 1 100 i 100
Eas ~y,Diff Easy
100 100
Easy
100
Easy
33
11
11
11
11
11
1
E asy
100
Easy
33
Easy
33
Easy
100
I.I.. I - i i
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1012 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 20 21 22 2-3 24 25 i6 27
28 i9 30 31
Figure 1. Percent intervals of disruptive behavior as a function
of level of task difficulty (easy vs. difficult) and overall
level of adult attention (100% vs. 33% of total intervals). Open
cirdes depict sessions conducted by an informed experi-
menter; filled cirdes, sessions conducted by naive
experimenters.
mulative effect of this intermittent reinforcement
is that the misbehavior is strengthened. If a child
has this history, then low levels of adult attention
may eventually come to be discriminative for the
emission of problem behavior.
Sue differed from the other three children in that
her behavior problems appeared to be under the
control of both the occurrence of difficult task de-
mands and the presence of low levels of adult
attention. This response pattern is consistent with
the results of previously cited research indicating
that the behavior problems of a given child can be
controlled by more than one set of variables.
We would like to qualify our analysis by sug-
gesting that the pattern of results observed could
plausibly be attributed to other processes than those
mentioned. For example, behavior problems as-
sociated with task difficulty might stem from a
child's failure to attend to adult instructions. Like-
wise, behavior problems occurring after a shift to
low levels of attention might be viewed as adjunc-
tive behavior induced by a decrease in the richness
of the prevailing schedule of reinforcement. These
alternative explanations need to be evaluated ex-
perimentally; however, our assumption concerning
the primacy of positive and negative reinforcement
processes is at least as plausible and heuristic as
the alternatives just presented.
117
(A
- CZ
4 0
Cc _LUJ
LA>
U. >
0-
u.
LUI
X* Sue
100
50
04
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
SE SS IO N S
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
EXPERIMENT 2
In Experiment 2, we sought to reduce the be-
havior problems identified in Experiment 1 by
teaching the children verbal communicative phras-
es that served to elicit either adult assistance or
adult attention.
METHOD
Children and Setting
These were the same as in Experiment 1.
Experimental Design and Overview
The problematic situations identified for each
child in Experiment 1 constituted the baseline phase
in Experiment 2. The intervention consisted of two
phases: relevant response and irrelevant response.
Consider first the relevant response phase for
Jim, Eve, and Sue. Recall that these children mis-
behaved in the difficult 100 condition. Because
this condition involved difficult tasks, one treat-
ment strategy already noted would be to teach the
children to solicit adult assistance. A relevant com-
municative response for a child to make might be
a phrase such as "I don't understand." This phrase
would serve to prompt a teacher to provide help
on the difficult task. Accordingly, we taught this
phrase to the three children involved.
Consider next the relevant response phase for
Tom and again Sue. Recall that they misbehaved
in the easy 33 condition. Because this condition
involved low levels of adult attention, one treat-
ment strategy noted already would be to teach the
children to solicit praise. A relevant communicative
response for a child to make might be a phrase
such as "Am I doing good work?" This phrase
would serve to prompt a teacher to praise the child.
Accordingly, we taught this phrase to the two chil-
dren involved.
Of course, it might be argued that teaching a
child any communicative phrase would produce a
decrease in disruption. To control for this possi-
bility, we introduced an irrelevant response phase.
Specifically, we taught Jim, Eve, and Sue (who
misbehaved in difficult 100) to ask, "Am I doing
good work?" Because this phrase was pertinent to
soliciting praise but not assistance, it was desig-
nated as a communicatively irrelevant response with
respect to difficult 100. As such, we predicted that
it would function as a control, failing to bring
about any decrease in disruptive behavior. Simi-
larly, we taught Tom and Sue (who misbehaved
in easy 33) to say "I don't understand." Because
this phrase was pertinent to soliciting assistance but
not praise, it was designated as a communicatively
irrelevant response with respect to easy 33. There-
fore, we predicted that it would not produce a
decrease in disruptive behavior in this condition.
In short, during the irrelevant response phase, chil-
dren who misbehaved in the difficult 100 condi-
tion were taught the phrase that was relevant to
easy 33, and children who misbehaved in the easy
33 condition were taught the phrase that was rel-
evant to difficult 100. Figure 2 shows, in flow
chart form, the relationships between the behavior
of the child and the behavior of the experimenter
in Experiment 2.
The relevant and irrelevant response phases were
alternated with the baseline phase in a reversal
design, with the phases counterbalanced across
children. Because Tom was soon to be placed in
another school, he received a shortened version of
the design. Number of sessions per day and session
duration were the same as in Experiment 1.
Procedure
Baseline phase. All procedures and tasks used
in baseline were the same as those described in
Experiment 1 with two exceptions. First, after every
incorrect response in difficult 100, and after every
30 s on the average in easy 33, the experimenter
would ask, "Do you have any questions?" It was
necessary to indude this question in baseline be-
cause it was subsequently used in the relevant and
irrelevant response phases. The question was scored
as a mand. Second, during difficult 100, we used
40 different Peabody cards rather than 20. This
change was necessary because we anticipated that
a large number of cards might be needed during
intervention.
In this and subsequent conditions, approxi-
mately two-thirds of the sessions were run by an
adult (randomly chosen from a pool of five adults)
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING
DI F F ICU LT IO0
( J i m , E ve, Sue)
EX PERIMENTE R C H L D EX PER I MEN T ER
E A S Y 3 3
( T o m , S u e)
BAS E LINED,, N o NetC
-~R es p onse Command
| N o W ait
R esp ons e S cn
Figure 2. Relationships between the behaviors of the child and
the behaviors of the experimenter during baseline (a
condition in which child verbal responses had not yet been
taught or were ignored if they had been taught), relevant
response (a condition in which the child's verbal response was
relevant to the situation evoking the problem behavior), and
irrelevant response (a condition in which the child's verbal
response was not relevant to the situation evoking the problem
behavior).
who was naive to the purpose of the experiment.
The other sessions were run by the second author.
Relevant response phase. This phase was iden-
tical to baseline except that it was preceded by a
training period. Specifically, each child was taught
to respond to the query, "Do you have any ques-
tions?" with either "Am I doing good work?" or
"I don't understand" depending on which state-
ment was relevant to the problem situation at hand.
Training was carried out in a single session con-
sisting of three stages. In stage one, the child was
verbally prompted by the experimenter to make
the requisite response (i.e., "Say, 'I don't under-
stand"' or "Say, 'Am I doing good work?"').
When the child correctly imitated on at least 8 out
of 10 trials, stage two began. In this stage, imi-
tation training was continued in the context of the
task itself. Specifically, in difficult 100 the exper-
imenter asked the child to point to the correct
picture. When the child made an error, the exper-
imenter said "That's not correct!" (as before) and
added, "Do you have any questions? Say, 'I don't
understand."' When the child imitated correctly,
the experimenter said a variation of the sentence,
"O.K., I'll show you" and pointed to the appro-
priate picture while verbalizing its label (e.g., "This
is a horse."). In easy 33, the experimenter asked
the child to match the pictures as before. Every 30
s, the experimenter inquired, "Do you have any
questions? Say, 'Am I doing good work?"' When
119
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
the children imitated correctly, the experimenter
said a variation of the sentence, "I like the way
you're working today. You're putting all the pic-
ture where they belong!" Verbal praise was always
accompanied by smiles and nods as well as physical
approval such as tickling and pats on the back.
When the child imitated correctly on at least 8 out
of 10 trials, stage three began. In this stage, the
adult's verbal prompts were faded out. That is,
the adult spoke the sentence "Say, ' ' more
and more quietly over trials.
Training was considered complete when the child
was responding correctly to "Do you have any
questions?" on 10 consecutive trials without any
prompts. At this point, regular sessions were again
conducted with each child as per the general base-
line procedure described previously. The adult con-
tinued to respond to each child's trained verbal
response with the appropriate consequence (i.e.,
assistance or praise). Because children in difficult
100 were now receiving assistance, they would oc-
casionally learn a new label. Therefore, to keep the
difficult 100 condition difficult, we replaced any
card that the child labeled correctly, twice in a row,
with a new card drawn from the pool of 40 cards.
Because Sue misbehaved in both difficult 100
and easy 33, she was trained in each condition
separately. Each day she received training in both
difficult 100 and easy 33 with sessions randomly
alternated between the two conditions.
Irrelevant response phase. This phase was con-
ducted in the same manner as the previous one
except that each child was taught to respond to
the experimenter's question in a manner opposite
that specified in the previous phase. That is, in
difficult 100, the child was now taught to answer,
"Am I doing good work?" and, in easy 33, to say
"I don't understand."
During difficult 100, the experimenter respond-
ed to the child's communicative phrase by praising
(e.g., "I like the way you're trying today."). How-
ever, assistance was not provided. During easy 33,
the experimenter responded by providing assis-
tance (e.g., "The picture goes here [pointing] just
where you put it last time."). However, praise was
not provided. In short, during the irrelevant re-
sponse phase, the children were taught phrases that
produced consequences that were presumably ir-
relevant to the problem situation. Specifically, the
children received praise in difficult 100, a situation
that called for assistance; and they received assis-
tance in easy 33, a situation that called for praise.
Response Definitions and Reliability
Disruptive behavior, adult attention, and aca-
demic behavior were recorded using the definitions
described in Experiment 1. In addition, two dasses
of child verbal responses were recorded. A relevant
response was defined as the phrase "Am I doing
good work?" emitted during easy 33, or the phrase
"I don't understand" emitted during difficult 100.
An irrelevant response was defined as the phrase
"I don't understand" emitted during easy 33, or
the phrase "Am I doing good work?" emitted
during difficult 100.
Reliability was assessed in 70% of the sessions
conducted for each child. The reliability procedure
and computation method were carried out as per
Experiment 1. The mean interobserver reliability
was 80% or greater for all response categories with
the exception of disruptive behavior for Sue, which
was 74%.
REsuLTs
Validation of Task Difficulty and Adult
Attention Manipulations
The validation data directly parallel those re-
ported in Experiment 1 and will therefore be pre-
sented briefly. In all cases, individual data were
consistent with group means.
With respect to task difficulty, the mean percent
correct across children varied from 25.0% to 27.4%
during all phases of difficult 100 and from 94.9%
to 95.4% during all phases of easy 33. During the
relevant response phase of difficult 100, a child's
score in some sessions ranged as high as 33% cor-
rect, reflecting the fact that, occasionally, the child
learned a new label. However, our practice of re-
placing learned picture cards with new ones en-
sured that the overall percent correct remained dose
to the desired 25% level. With respect to adult
attention, the mean level of this variable ranged
120
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING
from 98.4% to 99.1% during all phases of difficult
100 and from 35.2% to 35.6% during all phases
of easy 33. Because both attention level and task
difficulty were held constant across phases, changes
in the amount of disruptive behavior across phases
cannot be attributed to changes in either of these
two variables. Finally, praise, mands, and com-
ments each occurred at an average of approxi-
mately 33% in all conditions except during easy
33 in which comments occurred at near-zero levels
as planned.
Child Verbal Responses
The relevant response category occurred during
an average of 36.8% of the intervals across chil-
dren during the relevant response phase. During
the other two phases, namely, baseline and irrele-
vant response, the relevant response category oc-
curred in only 2.3% and 3.7% of the intervals,
respectively. The irrelevant response category oc-
curred during an average of 10.1% of the intervals
across children during the irrelevant response phase.
In contrast, during the baseline and relevant re-
sponse phases, the irrelevant response category oc-
curred in only 1.7% and 1.1% of the intervals,
respectively. It should be noted that in the initial
baseline, none of the trained responses (i.e., rele-
vant or irrelevant) was observed to occur. These
responses were only observed during baseline ses-
sions that were conducted later in the experiment
(i.e., after training had taken place). Table 1 shows
the mean percent intervals in which each child
displayed either a relevant response or an irrelevant
response across the three phases of Experiment 2.
These individual data are consistent with the group
means just reported.
Effects on Disruptive Behavior
Disruptive behavior was reduced to low levels
after a child was trained to emit a relevant com-
municative response but remained high after a child
was taught an irrelevant communicative response.
The top panel of Figure 3 shows the percentage
of intervals in which disruptive behavior occurred
during each session for Jim. In baseline (before
Table 1
Child Verbal Response (Percent Occurrence) in Each Phase
of Experiment 2
Child verbal response
Child Relevant Irrelevant
Jim (Difficult 100)
Baseline
Relevant
response
Irrelevant
response
Eve (Difficult 100)
Baseline
Relevant
response
Irrelevant
response
Tom (Easy 33)
Baseline
Relevant
response
Irrelevant
response
Sue (Easy 33)
Baseline
Relevant
response
Irrelevant
response
Sue (Difficult 100)
Baseline
Relevant
response
Irrelevant
response
2.8 (0-18) 0
33.2 (22-55) 0.4 (0-5)
0 10.8 (0-30)
1.2 (0-10)
17.9 (5-35)
0.8 (0-5)
5.0 (0-25)
0.2 (0-3)
1.3 (0-7)
12.7 (0-30)
4.3 (0-22)
33.9 (23-65) 4.4 (0-17)
0 9.3 (0-23)
1.3 (0-15)
32.3 (15-70)
6.7 (0-25)
1.3 (0-20)
46.3 (20-65)
5.0 (0-30)
0
0
8.3 (0-25)
0
0.4 (0-7)
9.0 (0-20)
Note. AU numbers shown are percentages. Numbers outside pa-
rentheses are means; numbers inside are ranges.
any communicative responses were trained), Jim
displayed an average level of disruption of 36.2%.
Disruptive behavior during the irrelevant response
phase averaged 48.8%. In sharp contrast to the
above figures, Jim's disruption fell to a level of
0.5% in the relevant response phase. The data for
the other three children are similar to those for
Jim.
The filled circles in Figure 3 depict the results
of sessions conducted by naive experimenters. As
can be seen, there were no systematic differences
121
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
Figure 3. Percent intervals of disruptive behavior during
baseline, relevant response, and irrelevant response phases.
Open cirdes depict sessions conducted by an informed
experimenter; filled cirdes, sessions conducted by naive
experimenters.
The level of relevant verbal responses is indicated by stippled
bars and that of irrelevant verbal responses, by hatched bars.
between these data points and those generated by
the informed experimenter (open circles).
Figure 3 also displays for each child the level of
relevant and irrelevant responses across each phase
of the experiment. Consider Jim's data (top panel).
During the initial relevant response phase, Jim dis-
played sustained rates of the relevant response cat-
egory (stippled bars) across the entire phase. Sus-
tained rates of the relevant response were also
maintained during the later relevant response phas-
es. In contrast, consider the initial irrelevant re-
sponse phase. During this phase, the rate of irrel-
evant response (hatched bars) was not sustained
across sessions. By the end of the phase, the re-
sponse level had decreased to zero. This pattern is
also evident during the later irrelevant response
phase. It should be noted that all the response
patterns described above for Jim were also char-
acteristic of the responding of the other three chil-
dren. Finally, consider the baseline phases for the
four children. Occasionally, either relevant or ir-
relevant responses or both would occur during the
early sessions of these phases. In all cases, however,
responding showed a steady decline, generally to a
zero level, as the phase progressed.
DISCUSSION
In Experiment 2, task difficulty, overall atten-
tion level, and the level of praise, mands, and
comments were all held constant across phases for
each child. Therefore, decreases in the level of dis-
ruptive behavior cannot be attributed to changes
122
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING
in any of these variables. It thus appears that the
establishment of a functional communicative re-
sponse was the effective variable in reducing be-
havior problems.
The mechanism by which communication train-
ing works may be conceptualized as follows. Con-
sider first those children who exhibited behavior
problems in the difficult task condition. If difficult
task demands and subsequent failure are discrim-
inative for high rates of behavior problems, then
a procedure that reduces task difficulty and thereby
limits incorrect responding will effectively remove
the stimuli that are discriminative for problem be-
havior. The relevant communicative response (i.e.,
"I don't understand") functioned to elicit adult
assistance that resulted in the child's making a
correct (though prompted) response. In other
words, each time that the child made the verbal
request, the adult provided assistance, thereby sim-
plifying the difficult task and averting failure on
the part of the child. Thus, the stimuli controlling
high rates of behavior problems were effectively
eliminated and, not surprisingly, the child's be-
havior improved.
Consider next those children who exhibited be-
havior problems in the low attention condition.
Recall that in the baseline phase of this condition,
the adult attended to behavior problems on an
intermittent basis (e.g., the adult held down the
child's hands following a severe aggressive act but
simply ignored less severe aggressive acts). In other
words, attention-seeking behavior problems were
responded to inconsistently. In contrast, following
relevant response training, the child's attention-
seeking verbal requests (i.e., "Am I doing good
work?") were consistently reinforced. It is a well-
known operant principle that consistent reinforce-
ment is a more effective means of strengthening an
operant than inconsistent reinforcement (Sulzer-
Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). Therefore, it is plausible
that the observed decrease in attention-seeking be-
havior problems and concomitant increase in at-
tention-seeking verbal requests reflect the fact that
the communication contingency provided highly
consistent reinforcement for the latter behavior in
the face of highly inconsistent reinforcement for
the former behavior. The result was that the verbal
requests were strengthened and replaced the prob-
lem behaviors.
The conceptualization just outlined is further
supported by the results of the irrelevant response
phase. Consider first those children whose behavior
problems were set off by the presence of difficult
demands and ensuing failure. Merely teaching such
children to solicit praise (the irrelevant response)
would not remove the stimuli that evoke the be-
havior problems. Hence, one would predict that
the problems would remain frequent, a result that
was in fact observed. The praise-seeking response
was nonfunctional in the difficult demand condi-
tion since it did not evoke adult assistance. There-
fore, one could make a second prediction, namely,
that the response should extinguish and, indeed,
it did.
Consider next those children whose behavior
problems were hypothesized to be attention-seek-
ing. Simply teaching such children to solicit assis-
tance in the low attention condition should have
no effect. That is, an assistance-seeking response
would be nonfunctional because it would merely
evoke aid in the form of a brief confirmatory state-
ment (i.e., "The picture goes here.") rather than
effusive attention in the form of praise (e.g., "I
like the way you're working!") coupled with nods,
smiles, and physical approval (e.g., tickling). Again,
such a nonfunctional response would be expected
to have no decelerative effect on behavior problems
and the response would therefore be likely to de-
crease over time. Both expectations were con-
firmed. To sum up, the irrelevant response phase
demonstrated that not all communicative phrases
will be effective in reducing behavior problems.
The phrase that is chosen must be functionally
related to the specific nature of each child's behav-
ior problems. Or, to put it in other terms, the
specific form of the verbal response is not impor-
tant. What matters most is the consequence that
follows a particular verbal communicative act.
Of course, one could argue that behavior prob-
lems might also have been reduced without any
communication training; that is, one could simply
provide the children with assistance or praise where
123
EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
appropriate and not teach the children to request
these events verbally. However, this procedure
would place the child in a passive role, a role that
developmentally disabled children already play too
often. Communication training, in contrast, pro-
vides an important educative function in that chil-
dren learn that communicative behavior can pro-
duce consistent and useful social effects such as
eliciting assistance on academic work and praise
for good performance. In this manner, the child's
role becomes that of an active participant rather
than a passive recipient. Ultimately, one would
also like to drop the adult query, "Do you have
any questions?" and have the child emit the verbal
response spontaneously. This type of spontaneity
is an important focus of our current research deal-
ing with functional communication training as a
treatment for behavior problems.
Finally, we note that certain problem situations,
at least initially, may call for an intervention strat-
egy other than communication training. Children
who pose serious physical danger to themselves or
others through their behavior may require deceler-
ative treatments on a crisis intervention basis.
Eventually, however, the issue of teaching alter-
native behaviors will arise, and we offer commu-
nication training as one possibility.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Behavior problems can be reduced by teaching
children communicative phrases that are effective
in altering the stimulus conditions that control the
problems. In Experiment 1, we demonstrated an
assessment method for identifying, within an in-
structional context, those stimulus situations (i.e.,
difficult demands or low adult attention or both)
that were discriminative for child misbehavior. In
Experiment 2, we demonstrated that the com-
municative phrase that is selected to replace the
behavior problem must be functionally related to
the controlling stimuli. Communicative phrases that
were functionally unrelated were ineffective in re-
ducing behavior problems.
These data are consistent with a broader con-
ceptualization of misbehavior that has recently
emerged within the field. This conceptualization
has been referred to as the communication hy-
pothesis of child behavior problems. The hypoth-
esis states that behavior problems may function
like nonverbal communicative acts to request (i.e.,
mand) specific reinforcers that are socially mediat-
ed (Carr, 1985; Carr & Durand, 1985; Neel et
al., 1983; Reichle & Yoder, 1979).
There are several sources of data bearing on this
hypothesis. Within the field of psycholinguistics,
research suggests that many nonverbal behaviors
displayed by very young children, such as pointing
or showing objects to an adult, serve communi-
cative functions (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra,
1975; Leung & Rheingold, 1981). Some of these
behaviors serve as nonverbal requests for adult at-
tention and others, as requests for specific rein-
forcing objects and events. Several developmental
psychologists have extended this analysis to other
nonverbal behaviors, in particular, those behaviors
that adults find disturbing. For example, studies
of early social behavior (Bell & Ainsworth, 1972;
Brownlee & Bakeman, 1981; Wolff, 1969) sug-
gest that crying and aggression in infants and tod-
dlers may also function as nonverbal requests that
are effective in reliably securing attention and ma-
terial reinforcers from adults as well as in termi-
nating aversive situations. Finally, in the area of
developmental disabilities, there is a substantial
literature demonstrating an inverse relationship be-
tween level of communicative skill and frequency
of behavior problems (Casey, 1978; Foxx & Liv-
esay, 1984; Shodell & Reiter, 1968; Talkington,
Hall, & Altman, 1971). This inverse relationship
has led some investigators (e.g., Shodell & Reiter,
1968; Talkington et al., 1971) to speculate that
behavior problems may function like nonverbal
communicative acts and that, once children are
taught more reliable ways to gain attention or es-
cape aversives (e.g., through speech), the behavior
problems are no longer as effective and disappear.
The communication hypothesis has heuristic
value in that it may lead to the formulation of
specific remediation strategies such as those de-
scribed above. It is likely that we will never be
able to demonstrate definitively that a particular
124
BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION
TRAINING 125
child intended to ask for assistance or attention by
means of his or her misbehavior. Nonetheless, we
may find it helpful to view misbehavior as if it
were a form of nonverbal communication, specif-
ically, a request for certain behaviors on the part
of others. Then, reasoning analogically, we may
proceed by teaching those children whose misbe-
havior is construed as communicating a nonverbal
request for attention, a verbal means for obtaining
the same. In addition, we may teach those children
whose misbehavior is construed as communicating
a nonverbal request to escape, a verbal means for
obtaining assistance. Seen in this light, the key
notion underlying the effectiveness of communi-
cation training is that of functional equivalence.
Specifically, although two behaviors may differ in
form (e.g., aggression versus the phrase, "Am I
doing good work?"), they may nonetheless be
identical in function (i.e., both aggression and the
verbal request produce attention). The communi-
cation training strategy capitalizes on this equiva-
lence by strengthening a socially desirable form
(i.e., a verbal request) that serves the same pre-
sumptive function as a socially undesirable form
(e.g., aggression) thereby weakening or eliminating
the latter.
It is worth emphasizing that the strategy we
have described will fail unless there is functional
equivalence between the two forms. This point was
made dear in the irrelevant response phase during
which, for example, a verbal request for attention
was taught in a situation in which the misbehavior
ostensibly served an escape function. In this in-
stance, the verbal request and the misbehavior were
functionally nonequivalent and, as we have noted,
misbehavior did not decrease. The necessity for
equivalence emphasizes the extreme importance of
first performing a functional analysis of behavior
problems (as was done in Experiment 1) prior to
selecting specific communicative phrases designed
to replace these behaviors. Clearly, then, the notion
of functional equivalence is a cornerstone of the
communication hypothesis of behavior problems
and its associated method of treatment. In this
regard, the hypothesis is consistent with the central
thesis of the behavioral approach to psychology,
namely, that the understanding of human behavior
is best advanced through the analysis of function
rather than form.
REFERENCES
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The ac-
quisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205-226.
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying
and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43,
1171-1190.
Brownlee, J. R., & Bakeman, R. (1981). Hitting in tod-
dler-peer interaction. Child Development, 52, 1076-
1079.
Carnine, D. W. (1976). Effects of two teacher-presenta-
tion rates on off-task behavior, answering correctly, and
participation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
9, 199-206.
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious be-
havior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological
Bulletin, 84, 800-816.
Carr, E. G. (1985). Behavioral approaches to language
and communication. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.),
Current issues in autism: Volume 3. Communication
problems in autism (pp. 37-57). New York: Plenum.
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). The social-com-
municative basis of severe behavior problems in chil-
dren. In S. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues
in behavior therapy (pp. 219-254). New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Carr, E. G., & Lovaas, O. I. (1983). Contingent electric
shock as a treatment for severe behavior problems. In S.
Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.), Punishment: Its effects on
human behavior (pp. 221-245). New York: Academic
Press.
Carr, E. G., & McDowell, J. J. (1980). Social control of
self-injurious behavior of organic etiology. Behavior
Therapy, 11, 402-409.
Carr, E. G., & Newsom, C. D. (in press). Demand-related
tantrums: Conceptualization and treatment. Behavior
Modification.
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976).
Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a psy-
chotic child. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 4,
139-153.
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980).
Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior of two
retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-
ysis, 13, 101-117.
Casey, L. 0. (1978). Development of communicative be-
havior in autistic children: A parent program using man-
ual signs. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizo-
phrenia, 8, 45-59.
Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P. L., Mesaros, R. A., & Fass-
bender, L. L. (1983, November). A strategy for ana-
lyzing the communicative functions of behavior. Paper
126 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND
presented at the meeting of The Association for the Se-
verely Handicapped, San Francisco.
Durand, V. M. (1982). Analysis and intervention of self-
injurious behavior. Journal of the Association for the
Severely Handicapped, 7, 44-53.
Durand, V. M. (1983). Behavioral ecology of a staff in-
centive program: Effects on absenteeism and resident
disruptive behavior. Behavior Modification, 7, 165-
181.
Foxx, R. M., & Bechtel, D. R. (1983). Overcorrection: A
review and analysis. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.),
Punishment: Its effects on human behavior (pp. 133-
220). New York: Academic Press.
Foxx, R. M., & Livesay, J. (1984). Maintenance of re-
sponse suppression following overcorrection. Analysis
and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 4, 65-
79.
Goldiamond, I. (1974). Toward a constructional approach
to social problems. Behaviorism, 2, 1-84.
Iwata, B. A., & Bailey, J. S. (1974). Reward versus cost
token systems: An analysis of the effects on students and
teacher. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 567-
576.
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
& Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional anal-
ysis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Devel-
opmental Disabilities, 2, 3-20.
Leung, E. H. L., & Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Develop-
ment of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental
Psychology, 17, 215-220.
Lovaas, 0. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C.
(1965). Experimental studies in childhood schizophre-
nia: Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal ofEx-
perimental Child Psychology, 2, 67-84.
Lovaas, 0. I., & Simmons, J. Q. (1969). Manipulation
of self-destruction in three retarded children. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 143-157.
Neel, R. S., Billingsley, F. F., McCarty, F., Symonds, D.,
Lambert, C., Lewis-Smith, N., & Hanashiro, R. (1983).
Innovative model program for autistic children and
their teachers. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Washington, Seattle.
O'Leary, K. D., & O'Leary, S. G. (1977). Classroom
management (2nd ed.). New York: Pergamon.
Patterson, G. R. (1980). Mothers: The unacknowledged
victims. Monographs ofthe Society for Research in Child
Development, 45, (5, Whole No. 186).
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene,
OR: Castalia.
Quay, H. C. (1979). Residential treatment. In H. C. Quay
& J. S. Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders of
children (2nd ed.) (pp. 387-410). New York: John
Wiley.
Reichle, J. E., & Yoder, D. E. (1979). Assessment and
early stimulation of communication in the severely and
profoundly mentally retarded. In R. L. York & E. Edgar
(Eds.), Teaching the severely handicapped. Volume 4
(pp. 180-218). Seattle: American Association for the
Education of the Severely/Profoundly Handicapped.
Romanczyk, R. G., Colletti, G., & Plotkin, R. (1980).
Punishment of self-injurious behavior: Issues of behavior
analysis, generalization, and the right to treatment. Child
Behavior Therapy, 2, 37-54.
Seymour, F. W., & Stokes, T. F. (1976). Self-recording
in training girls to increase work and evoke staff praise
in an institution for offenders. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 9, 41-54.
Shodell, M. J., & Reiter, H. H. (1968). Self-mutilative
behavior in verbal and nonverbal schizophrenic children.
Archives of General Psychiatty, 19, 453-455.
Stokes, T. F., Fowler, S. A., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Train-
ing preschool children to recruit natural communities of
reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
11, 285-303.
Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977). Applying be-
havior analysis procedures with children and youth.
New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Talkington, L. W., Hall, S., & Altman, R. (1971). Com-
munication deficits and aggression in the mentally re-
tarded. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 76,
235-237.
Voeltz, L. M., Evans, I. M., Derer, K. R., & Hanashiro, R.
(1983). Targeting excess behavior for change: A clinical
decision model for selecting priority goals in educational
contexts. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 5, 17-
35.
Wahler, R. G. (1969). Oppositional children: A quest for
parental reinforcement control. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 2, 159-170.
Wahler, R. G. (1976). Deviant child behavior within the
family: Developmental speculations and behavior change
strategies. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook ofbehavior
modification and behavior therapy (pp. 516-543). En-
glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Task difficulty
and aberrant behavior in severely handicapped students.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 449-463.
Wolff, P. H. (1969). The natural history of crying and
other vocalizations in early infancy. In B. M. Foss (Ed.),
Determinants of infant behavior. Volume 4 (pp. 81-
109). London: Methuen.
Zeilberger, J., Sampen, S. E., & Sloane, H. N., Jr. (1968).
Modification of a child's behavior problems in the home
with the mother as therapist. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 1, 47-53.
Received September 17, 1984
Final acceptance March 2, 1985
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx

Contenu connexe

Similaire à JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx

Coping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle School
Coping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle SchoolCoping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle School
Coping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle SchoolGabriela Rocha
 
Avaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunriseAvaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunriseglaucohp
 
Back to-school night
Back to-school nightBack to-school night
Back to-school nightAnne Francois
 
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxData Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxsimonithomas47935
 
Directions For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docx
Directions For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docxDirections For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docx
Directions For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docxmariona83
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docxEffectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docxSALU18
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFMarissa Schneider
 
1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children
1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children
1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of ChildrenNatalie Schneberger
 
Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012
Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012
Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012Monika S
 
Aba and autism.
Aba and autism.Aba and autism.
Aba and autism.pagonea
 
Power point aba
Power point abaPower point aba
Power point abapagonea
 

Similaire à JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx (20)

Coping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle School
Coping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle SchoolCoping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle School
Coping with Verbal and Social Bullying in Middle School
 
Avaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunriseAvaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunrise
 
Back to-school night
Back to-school nightBack to-school night
Back to-school night
 
LeGoff 2004
LeGoff 2004LeGoff 2004
LeGoff 2004
 
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxData Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
 
Directions For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docx
Directions For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docxDirections For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docx
Directions For each classmate post below reply with 200 words, de.docx
 
Who is Your Role Model?
Who is Your Role Model?Who is Your Role Model?
Who is Your Role Model?
 
observation.pdf
observation.pdfobservation.pdf
observation.pdf
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docxEffectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
Effectiveness of the KEEP foster parent intervention during an.docx
 
Appleby college research
Appleby college researchAppleby college research
Appleby college research
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
 
1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children
1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children
1013.05 Psy Cognitive Disabilities in the Learning Processes of Children
 
CCHD_2007
CCHD_2007CCHD_2007
CCHD_2007
 
Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012
Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012
Kidd palmeriaslin_Cognition Study 2012
 
Aba and autism.
Aba and autism.Aba and autism.
Aba and autism.
 
Power point aba
Power point abaPower point aba
Power point aba
 
Final presentation
Final presentationFinal presentation
Final presentation
 
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
 
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
2 Gen Zyd Robert Bibera.pdf
 

Plus de croysierkathey

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docxcroysierkathey
 

Plus de croysierkathey (20)

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
 

Dernier

PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docxPoojaSen20
 
Gardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch Letter
Gardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch LetterGardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch Letter
Gardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch LetterMateoGardella
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesCeline George
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingTeacherCyreneCayanan
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Shubhangi Sonawane
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxAreebaZafar22
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Disha Kariya
 

Dernier (20)

PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
 
Gardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch Letter
Gardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch LetterGardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch Letter
Gardella_PRCampaignConclusion Pitch Letter
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
 

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx

  • 1. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS REDUCING BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS THROUGH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING EDWARD G. CARR AND V. MARK DURAND STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK AT STONY BROOK, STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK AT ALBANY, AND SUFFOLK CHILD DEVELOPMENT CENTER It is generally agreed that serious misbehavior in children should be replaced with socially appro- priate behaviors, but few guidelines exist with respect to choosing replacement behaviors. We address this issue in two experiments. In Experiment 1, we developed an assessment method for identifying situations in which behavior problems, induding aggression, tantrums, and self-injury, were most likely to occur. Results demonstrated that both low level of adult attention and high level of task difficulty were discriminative for misbehavior. In Experiment 2, the assessment data were used to select replacements for misbehavior. Specifically, children were taught to solicit atten- tion or assistance or both verbally from adults. This treatment, which involved the differential reinforcement of functional communication, produced replicable suppression of behavior problems across four developmentally disabled children. The results were
  • 2. consistent with an hypothesis stating that some child behavior problems may be viewed as a nonverbal means of communication. According to this hypothesis, behavior problems and verbal communicative acts, though differing in form, may be equivalent in function. Therefore, strengthening the latter should weaken the former. DESCRIPTORS: disruptive behavior, assessment, classroom behavior, communication, devel- opmentally disabled children A major portion of child behavior therapy is justifiably concerned with the treatment of behav- ior problems, given that such problems can seri- ously disrupt the educational process (O'Leary & O'Leary, 1977; Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977) and in some cases may lead to institutionalization This investigation was supported in part by U.S.P.H.S. Biomedical Research Support Grant 2 S07 RR-07067-18 to the State University of New York at Stony Brook to the first author and a Sigma Xi Grant-in-Aid of Research to the second author. Portions of this paper were presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., August 1982, and Anaheim, California, August 1983. This research was based on a master's thesis conducted by the second author under the direction of the first author. We thank Martin Hamburg, Executive Director, Suffolk Child Development Center, for his generous support, and Roseann D'Evanzo, JoAnn Giles, Terry Leykis, Cathy Sher- edos, and Doug Walters for assistance with data collection. Finally, we thank Alan 0. Ross, Susan G. O'Leary, K. Dan-
  • 3. iel O'Leary, Crighton Newsom, Paul A. Dores, and Daniel B. Crimmins for their helpful comments. Requests for reprints or individual data should be sent to Edward Carr, Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Stony Brook, New York 11794-2500. (Quay, 1979). Behaviorists have developed treat- ment strategies designed to decelerate problem be- haviors, which indude procedures involving ex- tinction (Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, & Kassorla, 1965), time-out (Zeilberger, Sampen, & Sloane, 1968), response cost (Iwata & Bailey, 1974), overcorrec- tion (Foxx & Bechtel, 1983), contingent electric shock (Carr & Lovaas, 1983; Lovaas & Simmons, 1969), and ecological interventions (Durand, 1983). The focus has been on eliminating behavior problems, particularly those such as self-injury, aggression, and tantrums, that are serious enough to jeopardize the safety and effective functioning of the target children and their peers. There is consensus among researchers and cli- nicians that the elimination of behavior problems is an important first step in remediation. Ulti- mately, however, the problematic responses must be replaced with socially usefuil behaviors (Goldia- mond, 1974). Because there are few guidelines available to suggest what these replacement be- haviors should be (Donnellan, Mirenda, Mesaros, & Fassbender, 1983; Voeltz, Evans, Derer, & 111 1985, 189 111-126 NumBER 2 (summER 1985)
  • 4. EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND Hanashiro, 1983), we sought to develop a method for identifying and assessing those educational sit- uations in which behavior problems reliably occur (Experiment 1) and, second, we sought to use the assessment information in order to select replace- ment behaviors (Experiment 2). Research on the functional analysis of behavior problems provides a good basis on which to build identification and assessment methods. This liter- ature indicates that the factors responsible for the maintenance of behavior problems fall into two broad dasses: escape behavior, controlled by neg- ative reinforcement processes, and attention-seek- ing behavior, controlled by positive reinforcement processes (Carr & Durand, 1985). There is ample evidence to suggest that many children learn to emit behavior problems in the presence of aversive stimuli. The display of such problems frequently results in the removal of these stimuli, a dear example of a negative reinforce- ment process (Patterson, 1982). In the dassroom setting, instructional demands may frequently function as aversive stimuli and a variety of be- havior problems induding aggression, self-injury, and tantrums may serve as escape behaviors that effectively allow the child to avoid further partic- ipation in instructional tasks (Carr, 1977; Carr & Newsom, in press; Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, 1976, 1980; Durand, 1982; Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982; Romanczyk, Colletti,
  • 5. & Plotkin, 1980; Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981). The literature also indicates that the display of behavior problems often results in the child's re- ceiving attention, a dear example of a positive reinforcement process (Patterson, 1982; Wahler, 1976). Many investigators have presented data congruent with the notion that behavior problems may be a form of attention-seeking (Carr & McDowell, 1980; Iwata et al., 1982; Lovaas et al., 1965; Patterson, 1980; Wahler, 1969). These studies also imply that children may learn to emit behavior problems in response to low levels of adult attention. Given the results of the empirical investigations to date, it is dear that any method used to assess behavior problems should indude an analysis of the effects of level of attention (to identify possible attention-seeking functions) as well as an analysis of the effects of task difficulty (to identify possible escape functions). This rationale formed the basis for the assessment procedures used in Experi- ment 1. Once it has been determined that a behavior problem likely serves a specific social function (e.g., escape or attention seeking), one is in a position to consider appropriate replacement behaviors. One behavioral alternative to escape would be some form of assistance seeking. For example, it may be possible to teach the child a response that is effec- tive in evoking teacher assistance with a difficult task. Once assistance is provided, the task should no longer be as aversive and therefore escape be-
  • 6. haviors should decrease. Likewise, in the case of behavior problems that are attention seeking, one could teach the child an appropriate alternative response that is effective in securing adult atten- tion. An important question concerns the form of the response alternatives to be taught. Several studies suggest that children can be taught to solicit at- tention and assistance verbally (Seymour & Stokes, 1976; Stokes, Fowler, & Baer, 1978). Verbal communication training is not typically used as a method for controlling behavior problems. None- theless, the analysis just presented suggests that if communicative phrases are carefully chosen so that they serve the same presumptive social functions as the behavior problems they are to replace, then deceleration of these problems should be possible. This training strategy was explored in Experi- ment 2. EXPERIMENT 1 METHOD Children and Setting Teachers in a day school program for develop- mentally disabled children were interviewed, and the first four children who met both the following criteria were selected for indusion in this study: BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING
  • 7. They displayed at least one problem behavior per hour in the classroom and had expressive language skill consisting, at a minimum, of single word ut- terances. Based on these criteria, two males and two females were chosen. Jim and Sue were 13 years old; Eve, 14; and Tom, 7. The medical staff had diagnosed Jim as autistic; Sue and Eve as brain damaged; and Tom as developmentally delayed and severely hearing impaired. Tom wore a hear- ing aid. On the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, the children's mental age scores were: Jim, 3 years; Sue, 5 years, 10 months; Eve, 2 years, 5 months; and Tom, 5 years. They displayed a variety of aggressive, self-destructive, and disruptive behav- iors that are defined in detail later. Jim and Tom spoke spontaneously in complete sentences. Sue spoke spontaneously in phrases of 2-3 words. Eve was limited to using single noun labels, primarily when prompted. Sessions were conducted in a 5 x 10-m auxil- iary dassroom located next to the regular dass- room. The experimenter sat between two of the children at a table on which various task materials were placed. All materials were placed 0.3 m from the child so that he or she could readily reach them. If more than one set of materials was used at a time, they were evenly spaced in front of the child, also at 0.3 m distance. Small-group (two children) instruction was used to approximate the regular dassroom practice. Experimental Design and Overview In a given session, a child received either an easy
  • 8. task or a difficult task; in addition, a child received adult attention during either 100% or 33% of the time intervals into which the session was divided. The two levels of task difficulty and the two levels of adult attention were combined to produce three conditions: easy 100, easy 33, and difficult 100. Comparison of easy 100 with easy 33 permitted assessment of the effects of attention level (i.e., 100% versus 33%) while task difficulty was held constant. Comparison of easy 100 with difficult 100 permitted assessment of the effects of task difficulty while attention level was held constant. Easy 100 was designated as the baseline condition with which the other two conditions were com- pared. This designation was based on teacher re- ports and our own informal dassroom observations which suggested that a combination of easy tasks and high levels of teacher attention generally re- sulted in a low frequency of behavior problems. The easy 33 and difficult 100 conditions alter- nated with the baseline easy 100 condition in a reversal design. The sequence of conditions was counterbalanced across children to control for order effects. Session length was always 10 min and 1- 3 sessions were run per day. When multiple ses- sions were run on the same day, there was a 5-min break between sessions. Procedure Easy 100. In easy 100, a child worked on receptive labeling and match-to-sample and re- ceived some form of adult attention for doing so in 100% of the intervals in each session as deter-
  • 9. mined by a time sampling procedure described later. In the receptive labeling task, the child was presented with several cards from the Peabody Pic- ture Vocabulary Test, a measure of receptive la- beling skill that is based on a series of picture cards graded in difficulty. Each card on this test is di- vided into quadrants, one picture per quadrant. A card was placed on the table in front of the child and the child was asked to point to the relevant picture named by the adult (e.g., "Point to the ball"). To ensure that this task was indeed easy, an additional assessment was conducted prior to the start of this condition. The child was twice presented with the Peabody cards, and 20 cards were selected on which the child always responded correctly. These cards constituted the materials for the receptive task. For the match-to-sample task, 3 cards were se- lected at random from the group of 20 cards de- scribed above. One picture from each of the 3 cards was randomly chosen and 11 copies were made of each picture. The three different pictures were placed in a row in front of the child. These pictures constituted the samples. The remaining 30 pictures (i.e., 10 copies of each picture) were mixed together and placed in a pile 0.1 m behind the 113 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND samples. The adult pointed to the card at the top of the pile of copies and said to the child "Match
  • 10. this" or an equivalent statement. The child was then expected to place the card on top of the cor- rect sample. The children had considerable expe- rience on matching tasks and therefore did not require any prompts to match each card correctly. Typically, they would match several cards from the pile following a single command from the adult. Whenever they finished matching all the cards, the adult would gather up the copies, mix them to- gether, and again place them behind the samples. The procedure was then recyded. On those rare occasions on which a child made an error on either the match-to-sample or receptive labeling tasks, the adult would say "No!" or "That's not correct!" and go on to the next trial. Correct responding produced verbal praise (defined later). The two tasks were alternated, 5 min each, within each 10-min session. The task sequence was randomized across sessions. In each session, the adult would deliver atten- tion in the form of mands, praise, and comments. A mand (e.g., "Point to the " on the recep- tive labeling task, and "Match this" on the match- to-sample task) was presented in every third re- cording interval. Praise was given in a different third of the intervals, either contingent on correct responding (e.g., "That's right!") or contingent on general task-related behavior (e.g., "You're work- ing very nicely!"). While giving verbal approval, the adult made eye contact with the child, smiled or nodded or both, and delivered physical approval in the form of pats on the shoulder, mussing the child's hair, tickling, and related actions. Finally, comments were made in yet a different third of
  • 11. the intervals and consisted of a variety of descrip- tive statements (e.g., "It's sunny today."). The adult was cued by a bug-in-the-ear device as to when to deliver attention (i.e., a beep oc- curred every 10 s). In addition, the adult kept a written tally of the various forms of attention de- livered, which helped ensure that the three forms of attention were given equally throughout the ses- sion. Some form of adult attention (i.e., mands, praise, or comments) was given in every interval of the session, with the three forms being presented in a random sequence within and between sessions. A new trial began, every 30 s on the average, with the presentation of a mand. Thus, there were 20 mands given during each 10-min session. This procedure was in effect during all conditions. The other child who was seated at the table was given independent desk work to do while the session was being run. When the adult was not attending to the target child, the adult attended to (i.e., in- structed and praised) the second child. When a child displayed a behavior problem, the experimenter reacted as follows. If the child left his or her seat, the experimenter waited 10 s for the child to return. If the child did not return, the experimenter led the child back to his or her seat without comment. The experimenter ignored all other behavior problems (i.e., made no com- ment) and continued with the task at hand unless the behavior posed a physical risk. In that case, the experimenter restrained the child. For example, if the child struck the experimenter hard, the ex- perimenter would grasp the child's hand and re-
  • 12. strain it on the child's lap for a period of 5-10 s while the experimenter would continue with the task at hand. This procedure was in effect during all experimental conditions. In this and subsequent conditions, approxi- mately one-third of the sessions were conducted by an adult (randomly chosen from a pool of five adults) who was naive to the purpose of the ex- periment. The other sessions were conducted by the second author. Easy 33. In the regular dassroom, teacher at- tention was typically low during independent work assignments. Because match-to-sample was the most commonly used task for developing indepen- dent work skills, we chose it to assess the effects of low rates of adult attention on the level of be- havior problems. This test was consistent with classroom practice and was a task that the children could complete without error. The sessions were conducted as in easy 100 but the amount of adult attention was decreased. Specifically, mands and praise were each presented during one-third of the recording intervals as before; however, they were BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING now programmed within the same interval rather than in different intervals as had been the case for easy 100. Also, comments were discontinued. These changes resulted in a decrease in overall adult at- tention from 100% to 33% of the intervals, with-
  • 13. out altering the amount of praise or mands given. That is, in the one interval out of three that in- duded attention, both praise and mands occurred at a level equal to that in easy 100. During those periods of time in which the adult was not attend- ing to the target child, the adult worked with the other child seated at the table. Difficult 100. In the regular classroom, vocab- ulary tasks typically generated many errors. Be- cause receptive labeling of picture cards from a Peabody language development kit was one of the most commonly used tasks for building vocabu- lary, we chose a similar task for difficult 100, to be consistent with dassroom practice. To ensure that the task was indeed difficult, we carried out an additional assessment prior to the start of this condition. Specifically, cards from the Peabody Pic- ture Vocabulary Test were selected on which the child scored at chance levels with respect to recep- tive labeling (25% correct). The method for se- lecting these cards was the same as that used in easy 100. In all other respects, this condition was run in the same manner as easy 100. Response Definitions and Reliability Three classes of responses were recorded: dis- ruptive behavior, adult attention, and academic behavior. Based on our preliminary classroom ob- servations as well as teacher reports, we identified three to five common topographies of disruptive behavior for each child. These topographies in- cluded aggression for all four children (poking or hitting other people, or pulling their hair; striking or knocking over objects); tantrums for Jim, Tom,
  • 14. and Sue (any loud voalization or screaming ac- companied by whining or crying); self-injury for Jim, Eve, and Sue (hitting one's head with one's hand; biting one's hand); opposition for Jim, Eve, and Sue (saying "No" to an adult's request or pushing away the task materials); out of seat for Tom and Sue (child's buttocks breaking contact with the seat of the chair for 3 s or more); and stripping in the case of Eve (removing any article of clothing from one's body). Adult attention consisted of praise, mands, and comments. Praise was defined as any form of ver- bal approval delivered contingent on correct re- sponding to a task (e.g., "That's right!") or con- tingent on general cooperative behavior (e.g., "I like the way you're working today!"). Mands were defined as any task-related request made by the adult (e.g., "Point to the truck" for the receptive labeling task, or "Match this" for the match-to- sample task). Comments were defined as any de- scriptive remarks made by the adult (e.g., "There sure are a lot of pictures," or "It's sunny today."). Academic behavior was defined separately for the two tasks. On the receptive task, a correct response was scored if the child pointed to the picture named by the adult. An incorrect response was scored if the child pointed to one of the other three pictures on the Peabody card or failed to respond within 10 s. On the match-to-sample task, a correct response was scored if the child responded to the adult's command by placing one of the copies of the pictures on top of the appropriate sample. An incorrect response was scored if the
  • 15. child placed the copy on top of the wrong sample or failed to respond within 10 s. All responses were recorded using a continuous 10-s interval procedure. Observers sat in a corner of the room, 2.5 m from the child, and out of the child's line of sight. A tape recorder equipped with earphones emitted the recording interval number at the end of each 10-s interval. The presence or absence of the responses previously defined was recorded for each interval. Reliability observers were drawn two at a time from a pool of four undergraduate students. All observers were trained prior to the investigation by recording in classrooms. Training proceeded until the observers reached a criterion of 75% agreement on all behavior categories with one standard ob- server (an undergraduate who had extensive ex- perience in behavioral recording). During the ex- periment, reliability was assessed in 70% of the sessions conducted for each child. Observer records 115 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND were compared on an interval-by-interval basis. For disruptive behavior and adult attention, the reli- ability index used was the number of agreements divided by the number of agreements plus dis- agreements multiplied by 100. Academic behav- iors were scored on a trial-by-trial basis to yield percent correct figures. The mean interobserver re-
  • 16. liability was 80% or higher for all response cate- gories. REsuLTS Validation of Task Difficulty and Adult Attention Manipulations For brevity, only group averages are reported; however, in all cases, individual data were consis- tent with the group means. With respect to task difficulty, the mean percent correct observed across the four children was 96.5% in easy 100, 97.3% in easy 33, and 26.9% in difficult 100. With respect to adult attention, the mean level of this variable observed across the four children was 99.7% in easy 100, 33.4% in easy 33, and 99.9% in difficult 100. Thus, we suc- ceeded in creating two levels of task difficulty (an easy level approximating 100% correct and a dif- ficult level approximating 25% correct), and two levels of adult attention (a high level approximat- ing 100% attention and a low level approximating 33% attention). Finally, praise, mands, and com- ments each occurred at an average of approxi- mately 33% in all conditions except during easy 33 in which comments occurred at zero or near- zero levels as planned. Effects on Disruptive Behavior Figure 1 shows the percentage of intervals in which disruptive behavior occurred during each session for the four children. There were three dis- tinct patterns of disruptive behavior. Jim and Eve were disruptive primarily in the difficult 100 con-
  • 17. dition. Tom was disruptive primarily in the easy 33 condition, and Sue was disruptive in both easy 33 and difficult 100. As expected, disruptive be- havior in easy 100 was negligible for all children thus justifying our use of this condition as a base- line. The filled circles in Figure 1 depict the results of sessions run by naive experimenters. As can be seen, there were no systematic differences between these data points and those generated by the in- formed experimenter (open circles). DISCUSSION The fact that there were several patterns of dis- ruptive behavior displayed in Experiment 1 sug- gests that more than one variable was controlling the children's behavior. There is some evidence in the published literature (Carmine, 1976) that long intertrial intervals, such as those we used, may contribute to off-task behavior. Nonetheless, we felt that the use of long intervals was justified given that a major educational goal for these children involved group instruction, a situation in which long intervals are inevitable. In fact, long intervals did not in themselves guarantee disruptive behav- ior. The nature of the task and level of adult at- tention were the most reliable predictors. Consider Jim and Eve. Their behavior problems became more frequent when demands increased in difficulty (difficult 100) but these children were relatively well behaved in the other two conditions. This behavior pattern is consistent with the notion of escape responding. Specifically, it is hypothe- sized that some children have a history of success-
  • 18. fiully escaping from presumably aversive stimuli (such as difficult task demands and ensuing failure) contingent on the display of disruptive behavior. If a child has this history, then difficult task de- mands may eventually come to be discriminative for the emission of problem behavior. Tom's results suggest the operation of a second controlling variable. His behavior problems be- came more frequent when the overall level of adult attention was reduced (easy 33) but he was rela- tively well behaved in the other two conditions. This pattern of behavior is consistent with the no- tion of attention seeking. Specifically, it is hypoth- esized that some children have experienced the fol- lowing set of contingencies. First, the amount of adult attention given to the child decreases to a low level. From time to time, when the child mis- behaves, the adult attends to the child. The cu- BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING J im Easy Easy Easy Diff Easy Diff100 5 33 WIOT E 100 100 100 o..0o h o of o . . .U E v e 501 21
  • 19. 0- Tom 100 50J 01 a II, E a s y 100 EasyI -i- ~ E asy 100 A _~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ D i f f 100 Eas Easy 33 9-7 E asy 100 Easy
  • 20. 33 : Easy 100 Easy 33 Easy 100 I. . I . . I p Eas Easy Easy 3 3 Diff 100 EasY10e I .I I - - i. - - I - Diff 100 Easy 100 I b ~JoenOI Diff Easy Diff Easy 100 1 100 1 100 i 100 Eas ~y,Diff Easy 100 100 Easy
  • 21. 100 Easy 33 11 11 11 11 11 1 E asy 100 Easy 33 Easy 33 Easy 100 I.I.. I - i i 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1012 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 20 21 22 2-3 24 25 i6 27 28 i9 30 31 Figure 1. Percent intervals of disruptive behavior as a function of level of task difficulty (easy vs. difficult) and overall level of adult attention (100% vs. 33% of total intervals). Open cirdes depict sessions conducted by an informed experi-
  • 22. menter; filled cirdes, sessions conducted by naive experimenters. mulative effect of this intermittent reinforcement is that the misbehavior is strengthened. If a child has this history, then low levels of adult attention may eventually come to be discriminative for the emission of problem behavior. Sue differed from the other three children in that her behavior problems appeared to be under the control of both the occurrence of difficult task de- mands and the presence of low levels of adult attention. This response pattern is consistent with the results of previously cited research indicating that the behavior problems of a given child can be controlled by more than one set of variables. We would like to qualify our analysis by sug- gesting that the pattern of results observed could plausibly be attributed to other processes than those mentioned. For example, behavior problems as- sociated with task difficulty might stem from a child's failure to attend to adult instructions. Like- wise, behavior problems occurring after a shift to low levels of attention might be viewed as adjunc- tive behavior induced by a decrease in the richness of the prevailing schedule of reinforcement. These alternative explanations need to be evaluated ex- perimentally; however, our assumption concerning the primacy of positive and negative reinforcement processes is at least as plausible and heuristic as the alternatives just presented. 117
  • 23. (A - CZ 4 0 Cc _LUJ LA> U. > 0- u. LUI X* Sue 100 50 04 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 SE SS IO N S EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND EXPERIMENT 2 In Experiment 2, we sought to reduce the be- havior problems identified in Experiment 1 by teaching the children verbal communicative phras-
  • 24. es that served to elicit either adult assistance or adult attention. METHOD Children and Setting These were the same as in Experiment 1. Experimental Design and Overview The problematic situations identified for each child in Experiment 1 constituted the baseline phase in Experiment 2. The intervention consisted of two phases: relevant response and irrelevant response. Consider first the relevant response phase for Jim, Eve, and Sue. Recall that these children mis- behaved in the difficult 100 condition. Because this condition involved difficult tasks, one treat- ment strategy already noted would be to teach the children to solicit adult assistance. A relevant com- municative response for a child to make might be a phrase such as "I don't understand." This phrase would serve to prompt a teacher to provide help on the difficult task. Accordingly, we taught this phrase to the three children involved. Consider next the relevant response phase for Tom and again Sue. Recall that they misbehaved in the easy 33 condition. Because this condition involved low levels of adult attention, one treat- ment strategy noted already would be to teach the children to solicit praise. A relevant communicative response for a child to make might be a phrase such as "Am I doing good work?" This phrase would serve to prompt a teacher to praise the child.
  • 25. Accordingly, we taught this phrase to the two chil- dren involved. Of course, it might be argued that teaching a child any communicative phrase would produce a decrease in disruption. To control for this possi- bility, we introduced an irrelevant response phase. Specifically, we taught Jim, Eve, and Sue (who misbehaved in difficult 100) to ask, "Am I doing good work?" Because this phrase was pertinent to soliciting praise but not assistance, it was desig- nated as a communicatively irrelevant response with respect to difficult 100. As such, we predicted that it would function as a control, failing to bring about any decrease in disruptive behavior. Simi- larly, we taught Tom and Sue (who misbehaved in easy 33) to say "I don't understand." Because this phrase was pertinent to soliciting assistance but not praise, it was designated as a communicatively irrelevant response with respect to easy 33. There- fore, we predicted that it would not produce a decrease in disruptive behavior in this condition. In short, during the irrelevant response phase, chil- dren who misbehaved in the difficult 100 condi- tion were taught the phrase that was relevant to easy 33, and children who misbehaved in the easy 33 condition were taught the phrase that was rel- evant to difficult 100. Figure 2 shows, in flow chart form, the relationships between the behavior of the child and the behavior of the experimenter in Experiment 2. The relevant and irrelevant response phases were alternated with the baseline phase in a reversal design, with the phases counterbalanced across
  • 26. children. Because Tom was soon to be placed in another school, he received a shortened version of the design. Number of sessions per day and session duration were the same as in Experiment 1. Procedure Baseline phase. All procedures and tasks used in baseline were the same as those described in Experiment 1 with two exceptions. First, after every incorrect response in difficult 100, and after every 30 s on the average in easy 33, the experimenter would ask, "Do you have any questions?" It was necessary to indude this question in baseline be- cause it was subsequently used in the relevant and irrelevant response phases. The question was scored as a mand. Second, during difficult 100, we used 40 different Peabody cards rather than 20. This change was necessary because we anticipated that a large number of cards might be needed during intervention. In this and subsequent conditions, approxi- mately two-thirds of the sessions were run by an adult (randomly chosen from a pool of five adults) BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING DI F F ICU LT IO0 ( J i m , E ve, Sue) EX PERIMENTE R C H L D EX PER I MEN T ER
  • 27. E A S Y 3 3 ( T o m , S u e) BAS E LINED,, N o NetC -~R es p onse Command | N o W ait R esp ons e S cn Figure 2. Relationships between the behaviors of the child and the behaviors of the experimenter during baseline (a condition in which child verbal responses had not yet been taught or were ignored if they had been taught), relevant response (a condition in which the child's verbal response was relevant to the situation evoking the problem behavior), and irrelevant response (a condition in which the child's verbal response was not relevant to the situation evoking the problem behavior). who was naive to the purpose of the experiment. The other sessions were run by the second author. Relevant response phase. This phase was iden- tical to baseline except that it was preceded by a training period. Specifically, each child was taught to respond to the query, "Do you have any ques- tions?" with either "Am I doing good work?" or "I don't understand" depending on which state- ment was relevant to the problem situation at hand. Training was carried out in a single session con- sisting of three stages. In stage one, the child was verbally prompted by the experimenter to make the requisite response (i.e., "Say, 'I don't under-
  • 28. stand"' or "Say, 'Am I doing good work?"'). When the child correctly imitated on at least 8 out of 10 trials, stage two began. In this stage, imi- tation training was continued in the context of the task itself. Specifically, in difficult 100 the exper- imenter asked the child to point to the correct picture. When the child made an error, the exper- imenter said "That's not correct!" (as before) and added, "Do you have any questions? Say, 'I don't understand."' When the child imitated correctly, the experimenter said a variation of the sentence, "O.K., I'll show you" and pointed to the appro- priate picture while verbalizing its label (e.g., "This is a horse."). In easy 33, the experimenter asked the child to match the pictures as before. Every 30 s, the experimenter inquired, "Do you have any questions? Say, 'Am I doing good work?"' When 119 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND the children imitated correctly, the experimenter said a variation of the sentence, "I like the way you're working today. You're putting all the pic- ture where they belong!" Verbal praise was always accompanied by smiles and nods as well as physical approval such as tickling and pats on the back. When the child imitated correctly on at least 8 out of 10 trials, stage three began. In this stage, the adult's verbal prompts were faded out. That is, the adult spoke the sentence "Say, ' ' more and more quietly over trials.
  • 29. Training was considered complete when the child was responding correctly to "Do you have any questions?" on 10 consecutive trials without any prompts. At this point, regular sessions were again conducted with each child as per the general base- line procedure described previously. The adult con- tinued to respond to each child's trained verbal response with the appropriate consequence (i.e., assistance or praise). Because children in difficult 100 were now receiving assistance, they would oc- casionally learn a new label. Therefore, to keep the difficult 100 condition difficult, we replaced any card that the child labeled correctly, twice in a row, with a new card drawn from the pool of 40 cards. Because Sue misbehaved in both difficult 100 and easy 33, she was trained in each condition separately. Each day she received training in both difficult 100 and easy 33 with sessions randomly alternated between the two conditions. Irrelevant response phase. This phase was con- ducted in the same manner as the previous one except that each child was taught to respond to the experimenter's question in a manner opposite that specified in the previous phase. That is, in difficult 100, the child was now taught to answer, "Am I doing good work?" and, in easy 33, to say "I don't understand." During difficult 100, the experimenter respond- ed to the child's communicative phrase by praising (e.g., "I like the way you're trying today."). How- ever, assistance was not provided. During easy 33, the experimenter responded by providing assis-
  • 30. tance (e.g., "The picture goes here [pointing] just where you put it last time."). However, praise was not provided. In short, during the irrelevant re- sponse phase, the children were taught phrases that produced consequences that were presumably ir- relevant to the problem situation. Specifically, the children received praise in difficult 100, a situation that called for assistance; and they received assis- tance in easy 33, a situation that called for praise. Response Definitions and Reliability Disruptive behavior, adult attention, and aca- demic behavior were recorded using the definitions described in Experiment 1. In addition, two dasses of child verbal responses were recorded. A relevant response was defined as the phrase "Am I doing good work?" emitted during easy 33, or the phrase "I don't understand" emitted during difficult 100. An irrelevant response was defined as the phrase "I don't understand" emitted during easy 33, or the phrase "Am I doing good work?" emitted during difficult 100. Reliability was assessed in 70% of the sessions conducted for each child. The reliability procedure and computation method were carried out as per Experiment 1. The mean interobserver reliability was 80% or greater for all response categories with the exception of disruptive behavior for Sue, which was 74%. REsuLTs Validation of Task Difficulty and Adult Attention Manipulations
  • 31. The validation data directly parallel those re- ported in Experiment 1 and will therefore be pre- sented briefly. In all cases, individual data were consistent with group means. With respect to task difficulty, the mean percent correct across children varied from 25.0% to 27.4% during all phases of difficult 100 and from 94.9% to 95.4% during all phases of easy 33. During the relevant response phase of difficult 100, a child's score in some sessions ranged as high as 33% cor- rect, reflecting the fact that, occasionally, the child learned a new label. However, our practice of re- placing learned picture cards with new ones en- sured that the overall percent correct remained dose to the desired 25% level. With respect to adult attention, the mean level of this variable ranged 120 BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING from 98.4% to 99.1% during all phases of difficult 100 and from 35.2% to 35.6% during all phases of easy 33. Because both attention level and task difficulty were held constant across phases, changes in the amount of disruptive behavior across phases cannot be attributed to changes in either of these two variables. Finally, praise, mands, and com- ments each occurred at an average of approxi- mately 33% in all conditions except during easy 33 in which comments occurred at near-zero levels
  • 32. as planned. Child Verbal Responses The relevant response category occurred during an average of 36.8% of the intervals across chil- dren during the relevant response phase. During the other two phases, namely, baseline and irrele- vant response, the relevant response category oc- curred in only 2.3% and 3.7% of the intervals, respectively. The irrelevant response category oc- curred during an average of 10.1% of the intervals across children during the irrelevant response phase. In contrast, during the baseline and relevant re- sponse phases, the irrelevant response category oc- curred in only 1.7% and 1.1% of the intervals, respectively. It should be noted that in the initial baseline, none of the trained responses (i.e., rele- vant or irrelevant) was observed to occur. These responses were only observed during baseline ses- sions that were conducted later in the experiment (i.e., after training had taken place). Table 1 shows the mean percent intervals in which each child displayed either a relevant response or an irrelevant response across the three phases of Experiment 2. These individual data are consistent with the group means just reported. Effects on Disruptive Behavior Disruptive behavior was reduced to low levels after a child was trained to emit a relevant com- municative response but remained high after a child was taught an irrelevant communicative response. The top panel of Figure 3 shows the percentage of intervals in which disruptive behavior occurred
  • 33. during each session for Jim. In baseline (before Table 1 Child Verbal Response (Percent Occurrence) in Each Phase of Experiment 2 Child verbal response Child Relevant Irrelevant Jim (Difficult 100) Baseline Relevant response Irrelevant response Eve (Difficult 100) Baseline Relevant response Irrelevant response Tom (Easy 33) Baseline Relevant response Irrelevant response
  • 34. Sue (Easy 33) Baseline Relevant response Irrelevant response Sue (Difficult 100) Baseline Relevant response Irrelevant response 2.8 (0-18) 0 33.2 (22-55) 0.4 (0-5) 0 10.8 (0-30) 1.2 (0-10) 17.9 (5-35) 0.8 (0-5) 5.0 (0-25) 0.2 (0-3) 1.3 (0-7)
  • 35. 12.7 (0-30) 4.3 (0-22) 33.9 (23-65) 4.4 (0-17) 0 9.3 (0-23) 1.3 (0-15) 32.3 (15-70) 6.7 (0-25) 1.3 (0-20) 46.3 (20-65) 5.0 (0-30) 0 0 8.3 (0-25) 0 0.4 (0-7) 9.0 (0-20) Note. AU numbers shown are percentages. Numbers outside pa- rentheses are means; numbers inside are ranges.
  • 36. any communicative responses were trained), Jim displayed an average level of disruption of 36.2%. Disruptive behavior during the irrelevant response phase averaged 48.8%. In sharp contrast to the above figures, Jim's disruption fell to a level of 0.5% in the relevant response phase. The data for the other three children are similar to those for Jim. The filled circles in Figure 3 depict the results of sessions conducted by naive experimenters. As can be seen, there were no systematic differences 121 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND Figure 3. Percent intervals of disruptive behavior during baseline, relevant response, and irrelevant response phases. Open cirdes depict sessions conducted by an informed experimenter; filled cirdes, sessions conducted by naive experimenters. The level of relevant verbal responses is indicated by stippled bars and that of irrelevant verbal responses, by hatched bars. between these data points and those generated by the informed experimenter (open circles). Figure 3 also displays for each child the level of relevant and irrelevant responses across each phase of the experiment. Consider Jim's data (top panel). During the initial relevant response phase, Jim dis- played sustained rates of the relevant response cat- egory (stippled bars) across the entire phase. Sus-
  • 37. tained rates of the relevant response were also maintained during the later relevant response phas- es. In contrast, consider the initial irrelevant re- sponse phase. During this phase, the rate of irrel- evant response (hatched bars) was not sustained across sessions. By the end of the phase, the re- sponse level had decreased to zero. This pattern is also evident during the later irrelevant response phase. It should be noted that all the response patterns described above for Jim were also char- acteristic of the responding of the other three chil- dren. Finally, consider the baseline phases for the four children. Occasionally, either relevant or ir- relevant responses or both would occur during the early sessions of these phases. In all cases, however, responding showed a steady decline, generally to a zero level, as the phase progressed. DISCUSSION In Experiment 2, task difficulty, overall atten- tion level, and the level of praise, mands, and comments were all held constant across phases for each child. Therefore, decreases in the level of dis- ruptive behavior cannot be attributed to changes 122 BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING in any of these variables. It thus appears that the establishment of a functional communicative re-
  • 38. sponse was the effective variable in reducing be- havior problems. The mechanism by which communication train- ing works may be conceptualized as follows. Con- sider first those children who exhibited behavior problems in the difficult task condition. If difficult task demands and subsequent failure are discrim- inative for high rates of behavior problems, then a procedure that reduces task difficulty and thereby limits incorrect responding will effectively remove the stimuli that are discriminative for problem be- havior. The relevant communicative response (i.e., "I don't understand") functioned to elicit adult assistance that resulted in the child's making a correct (though prompted) response. In other words, each time that the child made the verbal request, the adult provided assistance, thereby sim- plifying the difficult task and averting failure on the part of the child. Thus, the stimuli controlling high rates of behavior problems were effectively eliminated and, not surprisingly, the child's be- havior improved. Consider next those children who exhibited be- havior problems in the low attention condition. Recall that in the baseline phase of this condition, the adult attended to behavior problems on an intermittent basis (e.g., the adult held down the child's hands following a severe aggressive act but simply ignored less severe aggressive acts). In other words, attention-seeking behavior problems were responded to inconsistently. In contrast, following relevant response training, the child's attention- seeking verbal requests (i.e., "Am I doing good work?") were consistently reinforced. It is a well-
  • 39. known operant principle that consistent reinforce- ment is a more effective means of strengthening an operant than inconsistent reinforcement (Sulzer- Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). Therefore, it is plausible that the observed decrease in attention-seeking be- havior problems and concomitant increase in at- tention-seeking verbal requests reflect the fact that the communication contingency provided highly consistent reinforcement for the latter behavior in the face of highly inconsistent reinforcement for the former behavior. The result was that the verbal requests were strengthened and replaced the prob- lem behaviors. The conceptualization just outlined is further supported by the results of the irrelevant response phase. Consider first those children whose behavior problems were set off by the presence of difficult demands and ensuing failure. Merely teaching such children to solicit praise (the irrelevant response) would not remove the stimuli that evoke the be- havior problems. Hence, one would predict that the problems would remain frequent, a result that was in fact observed. The praise-seeking response was nonfunctional in the difficult demand condi- tion since it did not evoke adult assistance. There- fore, one could make a second prediction, namely, that the response should extinguish and, indeed, it did. Consider next those children whose behavior problems were hypothesized to be attention-seek- ing. Simply teaching such children to solicit assis- tance in the low attention condition should have no effect. That is, an assistance-seeking response
  • 40. would be nonfunctional because it would merely evoke aid in the form of a brief confirmatory state- ment (i.e., "The picture goes here.") rather than effusive attention in the form of praise (e.g., "I like the way you're working!") coupled with nods, smiles, and physical approval (e.g., tickling). Again, such a nonfunctional response would be expected to have no decelerative effect on behavior problems and the response would therefore be likely to de- crease over time. Both expectations were con- firmed. To sum up, the irrelevant response phase demonstrated that not all communicative phrases will be effective in reducing behavior problems. The phrase that is chosen must be functionally related to the specific nature of each child's behav- ior problems. Or, to put it in other terms, the specific form of the verbal response is not impor- tant. What matters most is the consequence that follows a particular verbal communicative act. Of course, one could argue that behavior prob- lems might also have been reduced without any communication training; that is, one could simply provide the children with assistance or praise where 123 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND appropriate and not teach the children to request these events verbally. However, this procedure would place the child in a passive role, a role that developmentally disabled children already play too often. Communication training, in contrast, pro-
  • 41. vides an important educative function in that chil- dren learn that communicative behavior can pro- duce consistent and useful social effects such as eliciting assistance on academic work and praise for good performance. In this manner, the child's role becomes that of an active participant rather than a passive recipient. Ultimately, one would also like to drop the adult query, "Do you have any questions?" and have the child emit the verbal response spontaneously. This type of spontaneity is an important focus of our current research deal- ing with functional communication training as a treatment for behavior problems. Finally, we note that certain problem situations, at least initially, may call for an intervention strat- egy other than communication training. Children who pose serious physical danger to themselves or others through their behavior may require deceler- ative treatments on a crisis intervention basis. Eventually, however, the issue of teaching alter- native behaviors will arise, and we offer commu- nication training as one possibility. GENERAL DISCUSSION Behavior problems can be reduced by teaching children communicative phrases that are effective in altering the stimulus conditions that control the problems. In Experiment 1, we demonstrated an assessment method for identifying, within an in- structional context, those stimulus situations (i.e., difficult demands or low adult attention or both) that were discriminative for child misbehavior. In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that the com- municative phrase that is selected to replace the
  • 42. behavior problem must be functionally related to the controlling stimuli. Communicative phrases that were functionally unrelated were ineffective in re- ducing behavior problems. These data are consistent with a broader con- ceptualization of misbehavior that has recently emerged within the field. This conceptualization has been referred to as the communication hy- pothesis of child behavior problems. The hypoth- esis states that behavior problems may function like nonverbal communicative acts to request (i.e., mand) specific reinforcers that are socially mediat- ed (Carr, 1985; Carr & Durand, 1985; Neel et al., 1983; Reichle & Yoder, 1979). There are several sources of data bearing on this hypothesis. Within the field of psycholinguistics, research suggests that many nonverbal behaviors displayed by very young children, such as pointing or showing objects to an adult, serve communi- cative functions (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975; Leung & Rheingold, 1981). Some of these behaviors serve as nonverbal requests for adult at- tention and others, as requests for specific rein- forcing objects and events. Several developmental psychologists have extended this analysis to other nonverbal behaviors, in particular, those behaviors that adults find disturbing. For example, studies of early social behavior (Bell & Ainsworth, 1972; Brownlee & Bakeman, 1981; Wolff, 1969) sug- gest that crying and aggression in infants and tod- dlers may also function as nonverbal requests that are effective in reliably securing attention and ma- terial reinforcers from adults as well as in termi-
  • 43. nating aversive situations. Finally, in the area of developmental disabilities, there is a substantial literature demonstrating an inverse relationship be- tween level of communicative skill and frequency of behavior problems (Casey, 1978; Foxx & Liv- esay, 1984; Shodell & Reiter, 1968; Talkington, Hall, & Altman, 1971). This inverse relationship has led some investigators (e.g., Shodell & Reiter, 1968; Talkington et al., 1971) to speculate that behavior problems may function like nonverbal communicative acts and that, once children are taught more reliable ways to gain attention or es- cape aversives (e.g., through speech), the behavior problems are no longer as effective and disappear. The communication hypothesis has heuristic value in that it may lead to the formulation of specific remediation strategies such as those de- scribed above. It is likely that we will never be able to demonstrate definitively that a particular 124 BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND COMMUNICATION TRAINING 125 child intended to ask for assistance or attention by means of his or her misbehavior. Nonetheless, we may find it helpful to view misbehavior as if it were a form of nonverbal communication, specif- ically, a request for certain behaviors on the part of others. Then, reasoning analogically, we may proceed by teaching those children whose misbe- havior is construed as communicating a nonverbal
  • 44. request for attention, a verbal means for obtaining the same. In addition, we may teach those children whose misbehavior is construed as communicating a nonverbal request to escape, a verbal means for obtaining assistance. Seen in this light, the key notion underlying the effectiveness of communi- cation training is that of functional equivalence. Specifically, although two behaviors may differ in form (e.g., aggression versus the phrase, "Am I doing good work?"), they may nonetheless be identical in function (i.e., both aggression and the verbal request produce attention). The communi- cation training strategy capitalizes on this equiva- lence by strengthening a socially desirable form (i.e., a verbal request) that serves the same pre- sumptive function as a socially undesirable form (e.g., aggression) thereby weakening or eliminating the latter. It is worth emphasizing that the strategy we have described will fail unless there is functional equivalence between the two forms. This point was made dear in the irrelevant response phase during which, for example, a verbal request for attention was taught in a situation in which the misbehavior ostensibly served an escape function. In this in- stance, the verbal request and the misbehavior were functionally nonequivalent and, as we have noted, misbehavior did not decrease. The necessity for equivalence emphasizes the extreme importance of first performing a functional analysis of behavior problems (as was done in Experiment 1) prior to selecting specific communicative phrases designed to replace these behaviors. Clearly, then, the notion of functional equivalence is a cornerstone of the communication hypothesis of behavior problems
  • 45. and its associated method of treatment. In this regard, the hypothesis is consistent with the central thesis of the behavioral approach to psychology, namely, that the understanding of human behavior is best advanced through the analysis of function rather than form. REFERENCES Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The ac- quisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205-226. Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171-1190. Brownlee, J. R., & Bakeman, R. (1981). Hitting in tod- dler-peer interaction. Child Development, 52, 1076- 1079. Carnine, D. W. (1976). Effects of two teacher-presenta- tion rates on off-task behavior, answering correctly, and participation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 199-206. Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious be- havior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 800-816. Carr, E. G. (1985). Behavioral approaches to language and communication. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Current issues in autism: Volume 3. Communication problems in autism (pp. 37-57). New York: Plenum.
  • 46. Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). The social-com- municative basis of severe behavior problems in chil- dren. In S. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 219-254). New York: Aca- demic Press. Carr, E. G., & Lovaas, O. I. (1983). Contingent electric shock as a treatment for severe behavior problems. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.), Punishment: Its effects on human behavior (pp. 221-245). New York: Academic Press. Carr, E. G., & McDowell, J. J. (1980). Social control of self-injurious behavior of organic etiology. Behavior Therapy, 11, 402-409. Carr, E. G., & Newsom, C. D. (in press). Demand-related tantrums: Conceptualization and treatment. Behavior Modification. Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976). Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a psy- chotic child. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 4, 139-153. Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980). Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior of two retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal- ysis, 13, 101-117. Casey, L. 0. (1978). Development of communicative be- havior in autistic children: A parent program using man- ual signs. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizo- phrenia, 8, 45-59. Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P. L., Mesaros, R. A., & Fass-
  • 47. bender, L. L. (1983, November). A strategy for ana- lyzing the communicative functions of behavior. Paper 126 EDWARD G. CARR and V. MARK DURAND presented at the meeting of The Association for the Se- verely Handicapped, San Francisco. Durand, V. M. (1982). Analysis and intervention of self- injurious behavior. Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped, 7, 44-53. Durand, V. M. (1983). Behavioral ecology of a staff in- centive program: Effects on absenteeism and resident disruptive behavior. Behavior Modification, 7, 165- 181. Foxx, R. M., & Bechtel, D. R. (1983). Overcorrection: A review and analysis. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.), Punishment: Its effects on human behavior (pp. 133- 220). New York: Academic Press. Foxx, R. M., & Livesay, J. (1984). Maintenance of re- sponse suppression following overcorrection. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 4, 65- 79. Goldiamond, I. (1974). Toward a constructional approach to social problems. Behaviorism, 2, 1-84. Iwata, B. A., & Bailey, J. S. (1974). Reward versus cost token systems: An analysis of the effects on students and teacher. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 567- 576.
  • 48. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional anal- ysis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Devel- opmental Disabilities, 2, 3-20. Leung, E. H. L., & Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Develop- ment of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215-220. Lovaas, 0. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C. (1965). Experimental studies in childhood schizophre- nia: Analysis of self-destructive behavior. Journal ofEx- perimental Child Psychology, 2, 67-84. Lovaas, 0. I., & Simmons, J. Q. (1969). Manipulation of self-destruction in three retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 143-157. Neel, R. S., Billingsley, F. F., McCarty, F., Symonds, D., Lambert, C., Lewis-Smith, N., & Hanashiro, R. (1983). Innovative model program for autistic children and their teachers. Unpublished manuscript, University of Washington, Seattle. O'Leary, K. D., & O'Leary, S. G. (1977). Classroom management (2nd ed.). New York: Pergamon. Patterson, G. R. (1980). Mothers: The unacknowledged victims. Monographs ofthe Society for Research in Child Development, 45, (5, Whole No. 186). Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia. Quay, H. C. (1979). Residential treatment. In H. C. Quay
  • 49. & J. S. Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders of children (2nd ed.) (pp. 387-410). New York: John Wiley. Reichle, J. E., & Yoder, D. E. (1979). Assessment and early stimulation of communication in the severely and profoundly mentally retarded. In R. L. York & E. Edgar (Eds.), Teaching the severely handicapped. Volume 4 (pp. 180-218). Seattle: American Association for the Education of the Severely/Profoundly Handicapped. Romanczyk, R. G., Colletti, G., & Plotkin, R. (1980). Punishment of self-injurious behavior: Issues of behavior analysis, generalization, and the right to treatment. Child Behavior Therapy, 2, 37-54. Seymour, F. W., & Stokes, T. F. (1976). Self-recording in training girls to increase work and evoke staff praise in an institution for offenders. Journal of Applied Be- havior Analysis, 9, 41-54. Shodell, M. J., & Reiter, H. H. (1968). Self-mutilative behavior in verbal and nonverbal schizophrenic children. Archives of General Psychiatty, 19, 453-455. Stokes, T. F., Fowler, S. A., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Train- ing preschool children to recruit natural communities of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 285-303. Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977). Applying be- havior analysis procedures with children and youth. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Talkington, L. W., Hall, S., & Altman, R. (1971). Com-
  • 50. munication deficits and aggression in the mentally re- tarded. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 76, 235-237. Voeltz, L. M., Evans, I. M., Derer, K. R., & Hanashiro, R. (1983). Targeting excess behavior for change: A clinical decision model for selecting priority goals in educational contexts. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 5, 17- 35. Wahler, R. G. (1969). Oppositional children: A quest for parental reinforcement control. Journal of Applied Be- havior Analysis, 2, 159-170. Wahler, R. G. (1976). Deviant child behavior within the family: Developmental speculations and behavior change strategies. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook ofbehavior modification and behavior therapy (pp. 516-543). En- glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Task difficulty and aberrant behavior in severely handicapped students. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 449-463. Wolff, P. H. (1969). The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior. Volume 4 (pp. 81- 109). London: Methuen. Zeilberger, J., Sampen, S. E., & Sloane, H. N., Jr. (1968). Modification of a child's behavior problems in the home with the mother as therapist. Journal of Applied Be- havior Analysis, 1, 47-53. Received September 17, 1984 Final acceptance March 2, 1985