SlideShare une entreprise Scribd logo
1  sur  14
Télécharger pour lire hors ligne
Principles and Neurobiological Correlates of
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Craig Mehrmann, BS, and Rakesh Karmacharya, MD, PhD
Keywords: Buddhism, central executive network, default mode network, diffuse attention, focused attention, insight,
intrinsic connectivity network, meditation, mindfulness, salience network
T
he term meditation encompasses a broad variety of
mental-training practices that vary between cultures
and traditions, ranging from techniques designed to
promote physical health, relaxation, and improved concen-
tration, to exercises performed with farther-reaching goals,
such as developing a heightened sense of well-being, cul-
tivating altruistic behaviors, and, for some, attaining en-
lightenment.1
Meditation can be conceptualized as complex
emotional and attentional regulatory practices in which men-
tal and somatic events are affected by specific mental-training
practices.2
Meditation is typically associated with a concur-
rent state of heightened vigilant awareness and reduced
metabolic activity3
—which lead to improved physical health,4
psychological balance, and emotional stability.5
Not all med-
itation practices focus on the training of specific cognitive
skills, and in those that do, the methodologies and out-
comes often vary. It is therefore essential to be explicit
about the type of meditation practice under investigation.
Different types of meditation can be classified based on
how the practitioner’s attentional processes are regulated
and directed.6
In this article we focus on the two most com-
mon styles of meditation derived from Buddhist traditions:
(1) concentrative, or focused-attention, meditation, and
(2) diffuse, or open-monitoring, meditation. Concentrative
meditation involves maintaining and continually refocusing
attention on a chosen object, such as a body sensation, single
point in space, color, object, sound, or affective state such as
compassion. Open-monitoring meditation involves developing
a present-centered and unattached/neutral mode of observa-
tion toward all sensational phenomena, including thoughts.
Concentrative and diffuse meditation practices are of-
ten combined, whether in a single session or over the course
of the practitioner’s training, and are intentionally designed
to train specific cognitive processes.6
In many instances,
the attentional stance cultivated through meditation not
only is characterized by the style of meditation (i.e., concen-
trative or diffuse) but also includes the awareness of certain
philosophical principles (or metaphysical characteristics)
that support practitioners’ ability to impartially perceive
their reality. In other words, the attentional stance often
integrates the continuous awareness of certain metaphys-
ical characteristics that act as a medium through which
practitioners may objectively observe their sensory experi-
ence. Such characteristics often include the perpetual tran-
sience of all phenomena, the illusion of a separate and
permanent self or ego, and the understanding that one will
never ultimately be satisfied by sense objects—hence the
term insight meditation, which refers to a form of diffuse
meditation that is practiced with the continual awareness
or experience of these philosophical principles, and with
an emphasis on experiencing impermanence or change
(i.e., the continual arising and passing of phenomena from
moment to moment).
Western scientific interest in meditation has grown tre-
mendously in recent years, reflecting the increased appli-
cation of meditation-based modalities in clinical settings.
However, the scientific study of meditation has, in large
part, been conducted with minimal reference to its religious
and spiritual origins.7
Buddhist traditions offer widely ap-
plied, extensive, precisely descriptive, and highly detailed
theories about concentrative and diffuse forms of medita-
tion, which have remained intrinsically unchanged for over
2500 years and are presented in a systematic manner befit-
ting a scientific approach.6
Mindfulness-based therapeutic
programs are all derivatives of traditional Buddhist medita-
tion. The theory and practice of concentrative, diffuse, and
insight meditation are at the core of these programs, making
these meditation practices the basis for most applications
in clinical and psychiatric settings.1,8,9
Mindfulness-based
therapeutic programs have become a popular form of
therapeutic intervention: they are efficacious; their princi-
ples are universal; they are cost-effective; and they are
From the Center for Human Genetic Research, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA (Mr. Mehrmann and Dr. Karmacharya); Harvard
Medical School and Chemical Biology Program, Broad Institute of Harvard
and MIT (Dr. Karmacharya); Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Program,
McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA (Dr. Karmacharya).
Supported, in part, by National Institutes of Health Mentored Clinical Sci-
entist Research Career Development Award no. K08MH086846 (RK).
Correspondence: Rakesh Karmacharya, MD, PhD, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Center for Human Genetic Research, 185 Cambridge St., Boston,
MA 02114. Email: karmacharya@mcb.harvard.edu
© 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College
DOI: 10.1097/HRP.0b013e31828e8ef4
NEUROBIOLOGY Editor: Rakesh Karmacharya, MD, PhD
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 205
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
highly adaptable for diverse populations with different cog-
nitive abilities.7,10
Moreover, these programs have made
training in concentrative and diffuse meditation more ac-
cessible by offering what one may consider a nonreligious
alternative for individuals who may be unwilling to adopt
Buddhist terminology.7
A third style of meditation, known as transcendental
meditation (TM), has been extensively researched and en-
compasses the remaining forms of meditation practice.
TM practice is centered on the repetition of a mantra,
and emphasizes the development of a witnessing, thought-
free “transcendental awareness” or “pure consciousness”
that is described by practitioners to be “the original source
of thought.”11
This state is explicitly said to be cultivated
in the absence of any concentration or effort and is charac-
terized by the experiences of “unboundedness” and “loss of
time, space, and body sense,” thus placing TM in a medita-
tion category of its own.1,11
TM is not easily comparable
and is relatively unrelated to mindfulness-based meditation
techniques and therefore will not be discussed here.
The theory and practice of meditation are not grounded
in mysticism or in religious dogma, rites, rituals, or ceremo-
nies, and may rather be accurately described as composing
a detailed and precisely descriptive scientific technique. Me-
ditation is universal in nature; meditation methodologies are
equally applicable to individuals of any religious, cultural,
or spiritual background; and the practice of meditation does
not require any form of conversion or identification with a
religious tradition. Many Westerners, however, are unfamil-
iar with the cultural context in which the terminology of
Buddhist meditation has developed. This lack of knowl-
edge has, in some cases, resulted in misinterpretations of
meditation practices as well as in unwarranted religious or
dogmatic stigma being attached to them. To help bridge this
cultural boundary, we present an introduction to the principles
and clinical applications of concentrative and diffuse forms of
meditation. We also review relevant scientific studies to date
on concentrative and diffuse meditation, and discuss the clin-
ical implications of these findings. Following a discussion of
the philosophical basis and neurological underpinnings of
these practices and how they apply to modern psychiatry,
we suggest further directions for scientific research.
CONCENTRATIVE AND DIFFUSE MEDITATION
Concentrative meditation is often practiced in preparation
for practicing diffuse meditation and, as noted earlier, en-
tails voluntarily focusing attention on a chosen object in a
sustained fashion, such as breath sensations or a visual object,
visualized image, sound, or affective state (e.g., compassion).6
To sustain this focus, practitioners must constantly monitor
and regulate their quality of attention, thereby naturally
developing three regulatory skills: (1) to monitor and re-
main vigilant to distractions, thereby enabling practitioners
to recognize when and to what degree their attention has
wandered from the object of focus, (2) to release distractions
from the attentional field without further involvement,
and (3) to restore attention promptly to the chosen object
of focus.1,6
Concentrative meditation develops the three regulative
skills to the point that advanced practitioners have an espe-
cially acute ability to notice when the mind has wandered,
resulting in an automatic and instantaneous restoration of
attention on the chosen object of focus.6
Eventually, atten-
tion rests more readily and stably on the chosen object. As
concentration continues to develop, the regulative skills are
invoked less and less frequently, and soon the ability to sus-
tain focus becomes “effortless,” a state commonly known
as access concentration.1,12
The achievement of access con-
centration is often considered the appropriate time to begin
practicing diffuse meditation; however, this relatively high
degree of concentration is not absolutely necessary for the
beginning stages of diffuse meditation.
Diffuse meditation is done with the aim of becoming
more objectively aware of sensate experience through the de-
velopment of one’s ability to be psychologically present and
equanimous with all forms of sensory phenomena. This
state involves the nonreactive monitoring of the ongoing
stream of experience from moment to moment, without fo-
cusing on any explicit object for a prolonged period of
time.13
Practitioners aim to familiarize themselves with re-
ality at the pure sensate level, without interpretation of, or
identification with, sensational phenomena. In the context
of meditation, sensate refers to anything that can be men-
tally observed, including thoughts.
The prelude to diffuse meditation typically involves con-
centrative meditation to calm and focus the mind, reduce
distractions, and enhance one’s equanimity toward sensa-
tional phenomena. For example, the practitioner may use
concentrative meditation focusing on breath sensations at
the area of the philtrum (the indentation below the nostrils
and above the upper lip). Continually monitoring breath
sensations in the chosen area develops equanimity (or non-
reactivity) toward body sensations and enables the practi-
tioner to disengage from, quiet, and eventually stop the
internal dialogue of the mind. From here, the practitioner
may shift to diffuse meditation even if access concentra-
tion has not yet been established, so long as the awareness
of breath sensations is maintained with relative continuity.
The practitioner would then work at continually disenga-
ging from body-mind sensations as quickly as possible until
a continual stream of awareness, or access concentration,
has been established. Otherwise, as previously described,
practitioners would practice concentrative meditation on
breath sensations until access concentration has been estab-
lished, and then redirect their focus from breath sensations
to begin practicing diffuse meditation. Usually, practi-
tioners direct their attention to the most subtle (or least per-
ceptible) features of experience possible.
Open-monitoring meditation enhances one’s faculty of
present-centered, nonjudgmental awareness of psychosomatic
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
206 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
phenomena, thereby enhancing cognitive flexibility and
reappraisal.1
This state is accomplished through nonreac-
tive, metacognitive monitoring of body-mind sensations—
which gradually increases awareness of “automatic cognitive
and emotional interpretations of sensory, perceptual, and
endogenous stimuli.”1,13–15
With consistent practice, the
practitioner becomes progressively more aware of sub-
tle body-mind sensations, lending access to rich features
of sensational experience, such as emotional tone, ac-
tive cognitive schema, awareness of sensation all over the
body (inside and out), degree of phenomenal intensity, and
bodily vibrational frequency6
(i.e., the literal, physical expe-
rience of the body and mind as “vibrations,” or rapidly os-
cillating particular sensational phenomena whose strength
[amplitude] and speed [frequency] fluctuate over time). One
also develops the ability to experience the arising and
passing of phenomena at a high velocity and at very subtle
levels6
—that is, the literal and distinct physical or mental ex-
perience of the arising and passing of particular phenomena
up to 50 times per second. The subtlety of the practi-
tioners’ experience is in direct accordance with their con-
centrative capacity (the degree of effort exerted to observe
the subtlest sensations possible while remaining continu-
ously aware of, and equanimous toward, the internal sen-
sate experience).
Practitioners continue to practice meditation’s concen-
trative or diffuse techniques in everyday life, which enables
them to maintain a wholesome lifestyle and contributes to
their degree of readily accessible concentrative absorption
during formal meditation. The degree of concentrative ab-
sorption refers to the depth or strength of concentration
on a single-pointed focus and, during formal meditation,
correlates directly with how well the continuity of aware-
ness has been maintained since the previous meditation ses-
sion. For example, practitioners may strive to maintain a
continuous awareness of breath sensations or of an affec-
tive state such as compassion, or try to maintain a continu-
ous superficial “body-scan” throughout the day. Advanced
practitioners report a sense of physical lightness and in-
creased mental and physical vitality as a result of maintaining
a concentrated mind.12
This perception may be explained in
part by a possible correlation between increased concentra-
tion and decreased emotional reactivity, accounting for a
more efficient use of energy both during meditation and
in everyday life.15,16
Significant progress has been made recently in investi-
gating the effects of consistent concentrative and diffuse
meditation practice on systems for sensory processing and
integration, revealing two temporally distinct neural modes
of self-reference: (1) extended self-reference linking subjec-
tive experience across time (narrative focus, also known as
the default mode) and (2) momentary self-reference centered
on the present moment (experiential focus).17
Narrative
focus is characterized by relating to the world through a
construction of narratives that constitute the I or self,
distinguishing the individual self from the selves of others
and objects, whereas experiential focus refers to the neuro-
psychological experience of the present moment, without
ruminative, ego-centered interpretation of sense objects.17
In the context of meditation, it is useful to think of expe-
riential focus as the experience of life through the “internal,
present-centered, objective mental observer.” To observe
means to watch impartially as an outsider. The internal ob-
server is that which enables us to experience a sensation
without identifying with it, or figuratively “stand next to”
a sensation and observe it objectively. Through meditation,
one becomes aware of this observer and develops one’s
ability to make a continuous and objective observation of
one’s sensate reality. Therefore, practicing experiential focus
may be described as practicing present-centered awareness
by “continually disengaging attentional processes from self-
referential elaboration toward sense objects.”17,18
MINDFULNESS-BASED MEDITATION
Mindfulness-based therapeutic techniques have been exten-
sively incorporated into the psychiatric setting as evidence-
based therapeutic programs, such as mindfulness-based
stress reduction (MBSR),9
dialectical behavioral therapy,19
acceptance and commitment therapy,20
relapse-prevention
therapy,21
and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy,22
and
have been shown to be effective in treating depression,23
anxiety,24
drug and alcohol addiction,7,25
and various person-
ality disorders.7,19,26,27
In the clinical setting, these programs
are popular interventions for hypertension, stress, and cardio-
vascular illness, and in slowing and perhaps stopping or re-
versing neurodegeneration and cognitive decline.7,27
Mindfulness may be conceptualized as bringing one’s
complete attention to one’s present experience through the
nonjudgmental observation of the ongoing stream of all per-
ceivable internal and external stimuli as they arise from mo-
ment to moment.28
Thus, mindfulness is remembering to
pay attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the pres-
ent moment, and nonjudgmentally.28
Dakwar and Levin7
have compiled several psychological mechanisms through
which mindfulness may aid in psychiatric symptom reduc-
tion and behavioral change. It has been shown that an
accepting and nonjudgmental mental outlook toward dis-
tressing phenomena can reduce associated stress.15
For ex-
ample, patients with borderline personality disorder who
have learned to observe distressing thoughts and feelings
without dissociation show diminished fear response and
cessation of avoidance behaviors previously triggered by
those stimuli.19
This effect may be attributed to a kind of
habituation/desensitization therapy, where a repetitious,
but intentionally objective, cognitive exposure toward the
distressing stimulus gradually reduces its associated nega-
tive significance.7
Training in mindfulness may also assist in recognizing
maladaptive coping skills and aid in implementing better
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 207
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ones.7
A nonjudgmental attitude toward phenomena fos-
ters cognitive flexibility and reappraisal, and therefore may
lead to changes in thought patterns or attitudes about one’s
thoughts.1,7
The unattached or neutral appraisal of one’s
thoughts and emotions may reduce automatic reactivity to-
ward them, thereby enabling those who have learned mind-
fulness to shift from maladaptive thought patterns and
behaviors to ones that are more adaptive.29
Increased un-
attached awareness of behavioral, cognitive, and emo-
tional stimuli may improve stress tolerance, the ability to
recognize behavioral consequences, and the ability to set
healthy motivations into action.4,7,19,30
Mindfulness-based
acceptance may also aid in reducing dissociative adaptations
toward feelings or thoughts, as seen in borderline personal-
ity disoroder.7
Developing one’s faculty of unattached obser-
vation and acceptance of distressing thoughts or feelings
may reduce the likelihood of maladaptive behavior patterns.
For example, borderline patients who practice accepting
their chronic feelings of emptiness may engage less fre-
quently in self-destructive and self-injurious behaviors that
were previously used to distract from those feelings.7,19
As with Buddhist meditation, participants undergoing
mindfulness-based therapy sit in a calm, but vigilant, posi-
tion and observe their object of attention (e.g., breath, body
parts, thoughts) without judgment or rumination and as
continuously as possible. However, a mindfulness-based ap-
proach to therapy does not necessarily require formal medi-
tation in the strict sense.7
The goal of mindfulness-based
treatment is to promote mindfulness in the patients accord-
ing to their own capacities. For instance, a patient with de-
velopmental disabilities, brain damage, severe depression
or mania, anxiety, or psychosis may be unable to practice
formal sitting meditation. Often, patients may refuse to
practice because they do not clearly understand its purpose.
Therefore, some strategies offer mindfulness-promoting
cognitive exercises that may seem more engaging, less stress-
ful, or more practical to some individuals. Such strategies in-
clude group discussions, lectures, worksheets, readings, or
cognitive exercises such as guided imagery or metaphor to
aid in the development of mindfulness-based insights.7
For example, participants may be asked to visualize their
thoughts as debris floating over a river, watching their
thoughts come and go, giving them as much importance
as they would a piece of passing driftwood.31
Participants
are also encouraged to develop an unattached relationship
to phenomena through intellectual concepts such as the
transient nature of sensations and thoughts, helping them
to understand that even the most difficult or agitating
thoughts and feelings are bound to change. This understand-
ing fosters the ability to experience painful thoughts or
feelings more presently and calmly, allowing space for con-
templation, reevaluation, and the ability to allow them to
pass on without further elaboration.7
Patients are also en-
couraged to practice mindfulness at home and in everyday
life, such as while walking, eating, or talking. If the patient
is in a state of severe psychological turmoil, such as psycho-
sis or suicidal depression, then providing a quiet, peaceful
environment with positive human influence, healthy food,
and necessary medications may be the first step in helping
them to establish a balanced mind capable of developing
the foundations of mindfulness practice. Nevertheless, these
modified treatments consider the practice of meditation
techniques to be central to their therapeutic efficacy.7,9,10
Mindfulness-based meditation works under the premise
that all individuals—health care providers and patients
alike—can benefit from objective self-observation. It has
been shown that health care professionals who regularly
practice mindfulness meditation, particularly those in the
psychiatric setting, are better equipped to treat patients. For
example, a recent study found that participation in an eight-
week mindfulness-based stress reduction program was as-
sociated with improved mental health in graduate-level
psychology students.32
When compared to a control group,
students in the MBSR program reported significant pre/
post declines in perceived stress, negative affect, state and
trait anxiety, and rumination, and a significant increase in
positive affect and self-compassion. Compassion may be
described as a nonpossessive state of loving that is moti-
vated by a person’s own sense of well-being with regard
to his or her interactions and connectedness with others.33
Developing self-compassion is particularly relevant to the
fields of counseling and therapy. It has been purported that
in order to have compassion toward one’s client, which is
an essential aspect of conducting effective therapy,32–34
a
therapist must first have compassion toward him- or herself.
Research shows that counselors and therapists lacking in self-
compassion are more critical and controlling toward them-
selves, and are also more critical and controlling toward their
patients, resulting in poorer patient outcomes.32,35
NEUROBIOLOGICAL STUDIES OF MEDIATION
Many studies have shown that meditation and meditation-
derived therapeutic approaches improve overall physical and
mental health.4,10,36
In this section, we present findings from
imaging, electroencephalography (EEG), and physiological
studies that relate to biological effects observed with
meditation.
Brain Imaging
Intrinsic connectivity networks are large-scale brain net-
works that may function either in an active (engaged) or
passive (resting) state, suggesting that the human brain is
organized into distinct functional networks.37
Significant
progress has been made in investigating the effect of consistent
meditation practice on systems for sensory processing and inte-
gration, revealing two temporally distinct neural modes of
self-reference: (1) momentary self-reference centered on the
present moment (experiential focus), neurologically corre-
lated with the central executive network, and (2) extended
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
208 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
self-reference linking subjective experience across time (nar-
rative focus), neurologically correlated with the default
mode network.17,37,38
The default mode network comprises an integrated sys-
tem for autobiographical, self-centered, and social cognitive
functions characterized by ruminative, often subconscious,
self-referential narrative thought linking subjective experi-
ence across time—which generates one’s concept of self or
identity.39
The default mode network concept emerged from
a body of evidence demonstrating a consistent pattern of de-
activation across a distributed network of brain regions dur-
ing focused mental tasks; these regions become activated in
the absence of such focused mental tasks.39
The brain re-
gions include the posterior cingulate cortex (involving au-
tobiographical memory and self-referential processes), the
ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) (involving social-
cognitive processes related to self and others), the medial
temporal lobe (involved in episodic memory), and the angu-
lar gyrus (involved in semantic processing).37
Conversely,
the central executive network is activated (and default mode
network deactivated) during focused mental tasks and is an-
chored in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and
the posterior parietal cortex.37
There is a third intrinsic con-
nectivity network, known as the salience network, acting as
a switch or “toggle” between the activation/deactivation of
the central executive network and default mode network,
depending on the degree of mental engagement. The salience
network is anchored in the anterior insula (AI) and the ante-
rior cingulate cortex (ACC).37
Abnormal default mode network activity has been impli-
cated in a number of psychiatric diseases, including schizo-
phrenia,40
autism,41,42
epilepsy,43
depression44
and anxiety
disorders,45–47
bipolar disorder,44
attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD),44
and Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s
diseases.44
In many of these cases, particularly in schizo-
phrenia,40
ADHD,44
bipolar disorder,44
and depression/
anxiety disorders,45–47
an inability to “switch off” the hy-
peractive default mode network through focusing the mind
appears to be a major contributor to the disease.44
A recent
study conducted by Whitfield-Gabrieli and colleagues40
used
fMRI to examine potential alterations in default mode net-
work activity in subjects with schizophrenia. While pointing
out that the default mode network is activated during in-
ternal, self-referential thought, the authors noted that the
normal boundary between internal thought and external
perceptions might be blurred by hyperactivity of the default
network.40
This suggestion is consistent with psychotic
symptoms in schizophrenia whereby patients have an exag-
gerated sense of self-reference in the world, as evidenced by
paranoia, and a blurring of internal thoughts and external
perception, which may give rise to hallucinations.40
In line
with these observations, results indicate that the default
mode network is hyperactive and hyperconnected in people
with schizophrenia and that the strength of connectivity and
the defect in appropriate deactivation of the default mode
network are correlated with more prominent psychotic
symptoms.40
The study also found a significant increase in
hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default mode
network in immediate family members of schizophrenic
individuals as compared to individuals from families with
no occurrence of schizophrenia.
Another recent fMRI study analyzed working-memory,
task-induced modulation of default mode network activity
in subjects with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) com-
pared to healthy controls.47
The control group showed sig-
nificantly stronger connectivity in areas implicated in the
salience and executive networks, such as the right inferior
frontal gyrus and the right inferior parietal lobule. How-
ever, the PTSD group showed stronger connectivity in areas
implicated in the default mode network, such as stronger
connectivity between the posterior cingulate cortex and the
right superior frontal gyrus, and between the medial pre-
frontal cortex (MPFC) and the left parahippocampal gyrus.
Subjects with PTSD showed a significantly impaired abil-
ity to switch from an idling state into a task-oriented state
when compared to healthy controls, as implied by hypercon-
nectivity in the default mode network during the working-
memory task in the PTSD group.47
The different patterns
of connectivity suggest significant group differences in the
ability to disengage from the default mode network and
to engage the central executive network during tasks that
require focused attention.47
The psychological characteristics associated with default
mode network activation are commonly known distrac-
tions in the meditation milieu, such as daydreaming, mind
wandering, and projections into the past or future, which
has led researchers to investigate the effects of meditation
on default mode network activity.52
An fMRI study com-
pared differential brain-activation patterns during two dis-
tinct modes of perception: narrative focus correlated with
default mode network activation, and experiential focus
correlated with the central executive network activation,
in both novice participants and participants who had at-
tended an eight-week course in mindfulness meditation.17
In novices, experiential focus yielded focal reductions in self-
referential cortical midline regions associated with narrative
focus. In trained participants, experiential focus resulted
in more marked and pervasive reductions in the MPFC, in-
cluding the rostral subregions of the dorsal MPFC and ven-
tral MPFC, suggesting that present-centered experience may
rely on suppression of MPFC representations supporting
narrative focus. Trained participants also showed marked
reductions in the MPFC and increased recruitment of a
right lateralized network, comprising the right lateral PFC
and viscerosomatic areas such as the insula, secondary so-
matosensory cortex, and inferior parietal lobule. Moreover,
right insular functional connectivity analysis revealed a
strong coupling between the right insula and the VMPFC
in novices that was uncoupled in the mindfulness group, sug-
gesting viscerosomatic signals are by default associated with
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 209
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
activation in the VMPFC.17
This decoupling in the trained
mindfulness group was replaced by an increased coupling
of the right insula with the DLPFC during formal meditation,
which may reflect a neuronal reorganization that supports a
continuous, present-centered attentional stance.17
In another fMRI study, brain activity patterns observed
during concentrative and diffuse meditation were contrasted
in an experiment between expert Buddhist monks and lay
novices with an integrated concentrative/diffuse paradigm,
where involvement of differential brain activations was an-
alyzed between groups during concentrative and diffuse
meditation with reference to rest.38
No particular interest
was given to identifying previously theorized large-scale
brain networks, such as the aforementioned intrinsic con-
nectivity network. Striking differences between activity pat-
terns were found between the groups. Activity patterns
during diffuse meditation resembled the ordinary brain-
resting state in monks, but not in controls, suggesting that
monks also maintain an open-monitoring attentional stance
during nonformal meditation conditions (i.e., in everyday
life).38
There may be a meditation-related functional reor-
ganization of brain activity patterns in the prefrontal cortex
and insula in expert practitioners, reflected by a substan-
tially increased reliance on the central executive network
and decreased reliance on the default mode network during
nonformal meditation conditions.38
Neuronal populations
in regions typically associated with self-referential processing
during everyday life may have been reallocated to support
areas that are involved in metacognition of phenomenal ex-
perience; that is, objective, present-centered awareness.13
Several crucial aspects of Buddhist meditation practice are
relevant here—all of which relate to transcending concep-
tions of a separate self or ego—such as the cultivation of
mental states of unconditional love, compassion, and kind-
ness, or remaining continually aware of impermanence, or
the arising and passing of particular sensational phenomena
from moment to moment.8,33,38,48,49
Moreover, this finding
is consistent with the goal of meditation, which is to disin-
tegrate the experiential psychological distinction between
meditation and normative functioning over time.48,49
The study also found that the ACC and DLPFC play an-
tagonistic roles in the executive control of attention setting
in monks during meditation.38
A negative correlation be-
tween ACC activation and DLPFC deactivation during rest
was found, possibly suggesting increased reliance upon the
salience network when distraction is more likely, such as
during resting state.38
This may be a functional reflection
of increased reliance on the ACC/salience network and de-
creased reliance on the DLPFC/central executive network
during rest, and a decreased reliance on the ACC/salience
network and increased reliance on the DLPFC/central exec-
utive network during concentrative meditation. Note that
concentrative meditation is used to establish (or reestablish)
concentration when concentration is weak. Predominant
ACC activation during rest, as part of the salience network,
may be a neurological reflection of continually disengaging
from distractors caused by default mode network activa-
tion and reengaging with the object of focus during focused
mental processing (i.e., toggling between the default mode
network and central executive network). However, the
DLPFC, as part of the central executive network, is pre-
dominantly activated during continual focused attention,
which is confirmed by predominant DLPFC activation dur-
ing formal concentrative meditation. It would be interest-
ing to see if there is a gradual reduction in ACC/salience
and default mode network activation with a concurrent,
corresponding increase in DLPFC/central executive network
activation during a concentrative meditation session in ex-
pert participants—and, if so, how this result correlates with
the reported strength of concentration during the session.
The findings also address the question of whether medita-
tive states are associated with a transient deactivation50,51
or activation11,13
of executive brain areas. The findings
clearly show that executive areas are activated during
meditation.38
Another recent study examined meditation-induced cor-
tical gyrification to shed light on anatomical correlates of
meditation and meditation-related neuroplasticity through-
out the lifespan.52
Gyrification refers to the pattern and
degree of cortical folding on the surface of the brain. Par-
ticipants consisted of a large sample (n = 100) of long-term
insight meditators and age- and sex-matched controls. The
degree of cortical gyrification was established by calculat-
ing mean curvature with a 3-D version of a well-known
gyrification magnetic resonance imaging index across thou-
sands of cortices on individual cortical surface models. When
applying more liberal significance thresholds (p < 0.05),
gyrification was larger in meditators as well as in controls
overall.52
However, meditators showed larger gyrification
in some of the areas where prior analyses revealed thicker
gray matter cortices in meditators as compared to nonmedi-
tators.52,53
Regions included the anterior insula,53
left ante-
rior temporal gyrus,54,55
left central sulcus and vicinity,52,56
and right parietal operculum, which houses the secondary
somatosensory cortex.52,56
Stricter significance thresholds
(p < 0.01) revealed five distinct regions where cortical gyr-
ification was exclusively larger in meditators: the left pre-
central gyrus, left and right anterior insula, right fusiform
gyrus, and left cuneus, showing a global maximum in the
right AI.52
As previously mentioned, the AI has been shown
to play a crucial role in switching between the default mode
network and central executive network.37
The current study
suggests a correlation between AI gyral complexity and the
number of years spent practicing meditation.52
Although
the findings of this study should be interpreted with caution
since it did not correct for multiple comparisons, previ-
ous studies have likewise shown structural differences be-
tween meditators and nonmeditators within the AI.53–55,57
Moreover, functional analyses have shown activation of
the insula during meditation,52
supporting the notion that
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
210 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
the salience network is acting to shift the mind from a state
of wandering to a state of present-centered awareness during
meditation.
The AI has also been described as a “hub” for auto-
nomic, affective, and cognitive integration, as it has been
shown to integrate a wide array of stimuli, including cogni-
tive, socio-emotional, olfactory-gustatory, interoceptive sen-
sation, and pain processing.52,58–61
Meditators learn to
master their faculties of introspective awareness, emotional
control, and self-regulation, as well as techniques oriented
toward enhancing wholesome and unconditional mental
states.52
Therefore, the AI may be a contributing neural
correlate of a fundamental, all-inclusive awareness that acts
to shift the mind from a state of self-referential perception of
sensation (i.e., identification with sensation) to a state of all-
inclusive, present-centered awareness with regard to any
possible sensation. This category includes all body sensa-
tion, thoughts, beliefs, emotional states—everything that
can be observed, which is literally everything but awareness
itself. Therefore, the salience network may act as the bridge
between the subconscious reactive experience of being iden-
tified with sensation, associated with default mode network
activation, and an all-inclusive, “transcendent” state of ob-
jective, present-centered awareness of sensate experience,
contributing to a sense of “oneness” or interconnectedness
that is associated with central executive network activation.
Electroencephalography Studies
EEG coherence is characterized by the synchronous firing
of neural networks at a particular frequency. It is theorized
that the amplitude of a brain wave is positively correlated
with the degree of precision with which cells oscillate and
the size of the neural population through which the wave
is occurring.62,63
It has been found that increases in EEG
coherence correlate with increased brain processing capacity
in areas such as attention, working memory, learning, and
conscious perception.63–66
Synchronization of oscillatory neu-
ral discharges, particularly of the gamma-band frequency
(25–70 Hz), appears to play a key role in developing the
ability to integrate distributed neural processes into one over-
arching network with highly ordered cognitive and affective
functions, suggesting that gamma synchrony may also play
a crucial role in neuroplasticity.63
In an EEG study, long-term Buddhist meditators were
found to possess the ability to induce high-amplitude gamma
synchrony during meditation, where robust gamma-band
oscillations and long-range phase synchrony were found
during a nonreferential compassion meditation state.63
The
amplitude of activity in some of these practitioners was
found to be the highest reported outside of pathological
contexts.63,67
This finding suggests that massive distributed
neural assemblies were oscillating with high temporal preci-
sion during meditation.63
In expert meditators, the transition
from basal cognition to the meditation state typically took
5–15 seconds. Time to develop deep meditative states
characterized by neural synchronization at high amplitude
frequencies was found to be proportional to the size of
the neural assembly being stimulated. The goal of medita-
tion is to transform normative functioning into a consistent
meditative state by disintegrating the distinction between
meditation and normative functioning over time—which
is reported to occur naturally with consistent practice.8,48,49
In support of this claim, Lutz and colleagues63
found differ-
ences in basal gamma activity between novice and expert
meditators before meditation, with expert meditators show-
ing more gamma activity during resting state. Further testing
found that the duration of time spent meditating over the
lifespan, and not age, predicted differences in gamma activ-
ity during baseline cognitive functioning.63
A recent neurophysiology study investigated the effects
of attentional training on the global precedence effect (i.e.,
the faster detection of targets on a global, rather than on
a local, level) in eight highly trained Buddhist monks and
nuns and in eight age- and education-matched controls with
no previous meditation experience.68
Analysis of reaction
times showed a significantly reduced global precedence ef-
fect in meditators but not in controls, implying that the
meditators had a unique ability to willingly, quickly, and ef-
fectively disengage their attention from the dominant global
level.68
Analysis of event-related potentials revealed that,
due to an enhanced processing of target-level information,
the meditators had an acute ability to select the respective
target level in which the target number was embedded.68
Moreover, meditators selected target-level information ear-
lier in the processing sequence than controls. These results
suggest that with mental training, meditators are able to in-
crease the speed and accuracy with which they allocate atten-
tional resources, thereby increasing the depth and breadth of
information processing while reducing response latency to
stimuli.68
As described by the methodology involved in concentra-
tive and diffuse meditation, these findings support the no-
tion that concentrative and diffuse meditation practices
result in an increased capacity to process sensory informa-
tion by training the practitioner to not “get stuck” on spe-
cific stimuli, possibly contributing to the ability to notice
fine details more quickly and accurately. To reiterate, con-
centrative meditators center their attention on a specific ob-
ject of focus for extended periods of time, and when their
attention is diverted from the object of focus, they learn
to disengage quickly from the distractor and to reengage
with the object of focus until access concentration is estab-
lished. A similar process may be used to establish access con-
centration during diffuse meditation, though instead of
maintaining attentional focus on a specific object, the medi-
tator practices continually disengaging from sense objects
throughout the body/mind as quickly as possible until a con-
tinual awareness of the ongoing stream of experience is
established. The above results also support the traditional
notion that meditation “concentrates” the mind—that is,
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 211
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
makes the mind progressively more acutely aware of the
details of momentary experience.
Physiological Studies
It has been known that regular meditation practice can re-
duce psychological stress and enhance perceived well-
being.8,9,48,49,69
Some attention has been given to the
physiological correlates of meditation-derived health bene-
fits as a means to understanding the mechanistic links be-
tween psychological functioning and physical health. An
early physiology study compared the effect of Buddhist
meditation on several physiological markers (serum corti-
sol, total protein levels, blood pressure, pulse rate, lung vol-
ume, and reaction time) between 52 male meditators and
30 age- and sex-matched nonmeditators.4
Measurements
of these markers were taken after three and six weeks of
practice. At three weeks, results showed significant reduc-
tions in serum cortisol level, blood pressure, pulse rate,
and reaction time in meditators compared to controls.4
Vi-
tal lung capacity, tidal volume, and maximal voluntary
ventilation were significantly lower following each medita-
tion session than before, indicating a lower need for oxy-
gen as a result of meditation.4
In addition, the serum total
protein level significantly increased by the six-week time
point. Meditators also showed a 23% reduction in reaction
time compared to measurements taken before practice, sug-
gesting that a high degree of present-centered attentional
acuity was developed during practice.4
The control group
showed a 7% reduction in reaction time at six weeks, most
likely due to task repetition, indicating that meditation
practice was not entirely responsible for the reduced reac-
tion time in the experimental group. These results are con-
sistent with the current conception of meditation as being
associated with a concurrent state of heightened vigilant
awareness and reduced metabolic activity that elicits im-
proved physical health, psychological balance, and emo-
tional stability.3,6,15,49
A recent study investigated whether meditation practice
is associated with telomerase activity and whether this asso-
ciation is at least partly explained by changes in two major
contributors to the experience of stress—“perceived control”
and “neuroticism.”70
Telomeres are terminal DNA protein
complexes consisting of repetitive nucleotide sequences that
are located at the ends of chromosomes.71
They are known
to ensure genomic stability during cellular replication by pro-
tecting the ends of chromosomes from deterioration and from
fusing with other chromosomes. However, telomeres shorten
under conditions of oxidative stress, as well as with each
cell-division cycle. Telomerase is a ribonucleoprotein that
counteracts this process by adding DNA to the 3’ end in
the telomere regions.71
As cells divide during the aging pro-
cess, telomere length steadily decreases and hence is an in-
dicator of the biological age of a cell.70,72
Cell division
can no longer occur below a critical telomere length, result-
ing in a state of senescence.71,73
Therefore, telomere length
acts as an effective “psychobiomarker” in predicting physi-
cal health and longevity by linking stress and disease.70,74–76
Recent longitudinal studies indicate that telomere length
in peripheral blood mononuclear cells can increase over time,
suggesting that there may be determinable mechanisms—and
therefore possible interventions—that regulate the rate of
telomere length alteration.70,77–79
Greater perceived stress,
negative affect, and stress-related cardiovascular risk are
factors associated with lower telomerase activity that is re-
sponsible for protecting against telomere shortening.70,80,81
Individuals who respond with suppressed vagal tone when
presented with an acute stressor and who show correspond-
ingly increased measures of negative affect and vulnerability
to psychological stressors show reduced telomerase activ-
ity.70,82,83
Therefore, telomerase activity may play a key role
in mediating the relation between psychological stress and
disease.70
This controlled, longitudinal study by Jacobs
and colleagues70
investigated the effects of a three-month
meditation retreat on telomerase activity and on two ma-
jor contributors to the experience of stress—perceived con-
trol (associated with decreased stress) and neuroticism
(associated with increased subjective stress). Perceived con-
trol is a marker of stress resilience and refers to a feeling
of adequate control that has been associated with reduced
psychological stress and more adaptive responses to stress-
ful events.70,84–89
For example, an internal locus of control
refers to the extent to which individuals believe they can
control their external circumstance and has been shown
to mediate responses to stressful events by improving cop-
ing strategies90,91
and reducing anxiety.70,92
High trait neg-
ative affectivity, or neuroticism, refers to chronic feelings of
tension, anxiety, lability, or insecurity, and typically ampli-
fies the stress response to acute stressors, causing “greater
stress vulnerability.”70,93–96
The study by Jacobs and colleagues70
also examined the
causal relationship between meaning-finding and affect,
with specific regard to how these factors are influenced by
meditation. Longitudinal studies have shown that the abil-
ity to integrate stressful events into an encompassing sense
of meaning or purpose leads to a change in affect.70,97
In
the context of meditation, Fredrickson and colleagues98
showed that the amount of time per week spent in “loving-
kindness,” or compassion, meditation over a two-month
period predicted a cumulative increase in positive affect,
contributing to an increase in life satisfaction and reduced
depressive symptoms as mediated by one’s degree of per-
ceived purpose in life and mindfulness. Jacobs and collea-
gues70
found that telomerase activity was significantly
greater in retreat participants than in controls at the end
of the retreat (p < 0.05). Moreover, increases in perceived
control, decreases in neuroticism, and increases in both
mindfulness and perceived purpose in life were greater in
the retreat group (p < 0.01).70
The data suggest that
increases in perceived control and decreases in negative af-
fectivity, as influenced by meditation practice, contributed
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
212 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
to an increase in telomerase activity. If so, consistent medita-
tion could potentially result in increases in telomere length
and immune cell longevity throughout the lifespan.70
Hence,
the practice of meditation seems to affect cellular processes
that are influenced by stress and linked to disease.
Another recent study examined the effect of compassion
meditation, a form of emotional diffuse meditation, on in-
nate immune, neuroendocrine, and behavioral responses to
psychological stress, and evaluated the degree to which en-
gagement in meditation practice influenced stress reactiv-
ity.99
Sixty-one healthy adults were randomized to either
six weeks of training in compassion meditation (n = 33)
or participation in a health discussion control group (n =
28). These were followed by exposure to a standardized
laboratory stressor (Trier Social Stress Test (TSST)) that
consists of public speaking followed by mental arithmetic.
Physiologic and behavioral responses to the TSST were de-
termined by repeated assessments of plasma concentrations
of interleukin-6 (IL-6) and cortisol, and by total distress
scores on the Profile of Mood States (POMS). Initially, no
differences between groups were found on TSST response
scores for IL-6, cortisol, or POMS scores. However, in-
creased meditation practice as measured by mean number
of practice sessions per week was strongly correlated with
decreased TSST-induced IL-6 (p = 0.008) and POMS scores
(p = 0.014). Because no differences based on group assign-
ment were found (meditation versus controls), further anal-
ysis was done to compare individuals who spent more time
meditating (above the median, high practice) to individuals
who spent less time meditating (below the median, low
practice) and controls (no practice). High-practice indivi-
duals (n = 17) exhibited significantly lower TSST-induced
IL-6 and POMS scores compared to low-practice indivi-
duals and controls (p = 0.005). These data suggest that en-
gagement in compassion meditation may progressively
reduce stress-induced immune and behavioral responses
relative to one’s degree of experience with meditation.99
Yet another study examined the effects of mindfulness
on HPA activity and sleep by comparing morning cortisol
samples and also sleep duration and quality between long-
and short-term meditators.100
This study was conducted
with the specific aim of clarifying existing contradictory
conclusions from numerous previous studies on the effects
of mindfulness on health by investigating the relationship
between traditional Buddhist meditation, mindfulness-based
stress reduction, HPA activity, and sleep among long-term
practitioners of meditation (Buddhist monks and nuns, and
mindfulness teachers) compared to those with no prior med-
itation experience (novices). Long-term meditators showed
decreased morning cortisol levels that corresponded with
length of practice over the lifespan (p < 0.03). Similarly,
novices who took an eight-week introductory MBSR course
showed reduced morning cortisol levels (p = 0.04) and sub-
stantially improved sleep (p = 0.008) and self-attribution of
mindfulness (p = 0.001). However, cortisol levels did not
change between the beginning and end of individual MBSR
sessions. These results support the view that meditation and
meditation-derived therapeutic techniques such as MBSR
have beneficial effects on biomarkers of stress regulation,
such as cortical secretion and quality of sleep, and also seem
to improve one’s ability to remain mindful. 35
These results
provide support for MBSR as a nonpharmacological inter-
vention alongside standard therapies, as well as in nonclinical
settings to foster well-being. Moreover, the empirical physio-
logical data summarized from this study support the validity
of the self-attributed data previously discussed in this section.
For example, increased perceived control, reduced negative
affectivity, and increased life satisfaction scores would be
expected in individuals with improved biomarkers of stress
regulation.
DISCUSSION
Western medical traditions have progressed by way of hy-
pothetical deduction, relying heavily upon pharmaceuticals
and technology-based interventions to treat or suppress dis-
eases caused by pathophysiologic processes. While these have
had a substantial impact in improving human health globally,
the effectiveness of Western medicine remains limited, espe-
cially when treating chronic diseases and mental illness in
which stress and the environment play major roles.101
The
study and integration of Eastern healing techniques may bring
about a complementary approach that will help develop a more
comprehensive and holistic attitude toward patient care.
Meditation is an ancient Eastern technique that has re-
cently been accepted as a form of clinical intervention in the
West. The study and practice of Buddhist meditation, along
with the increasing use of Buddhist-derived, mindfulness-
based therapeutic modalities in the clinical setting, have
piqued scientific interest. Here, we have introduced the ba-
sic concepts of concentrative, diffuse, and insight medita-
tion, as well as some of the relevant clinical applications
and scientific studies that suggest significant physiological
and neurological correlates of these meditative practices.
Neuroscientists have made strides in elucidating some of
the basic biological mechanisms underlying the benefits of
meditation. Our understanding, however, is rudimentary
at this stage.
Compelling scientific evidence supports the role of med-
itation in promoting health in general20,21,53,70
and in treat-
ing numerous clinical and psychiatric disorders.20–30,36,44,47
Buddhist meditation induces what one may consider a low-
entropy state of equanimous, vigilant awareness,3
bringing
the body to a “hypometabolic state of parasympathetic dom-
inance” that serves to rejuvenate the organism’s capacity for
resilience and adaptability.7
Long-term Buddhist medita-
tion has been correlated with reduced stress response and
lower cortisol levels overall,4,36,70
possibly contributing to
reduced chronic stress and a restored healthy response to
acute stress,4
longevity,70
reduced stress-induced craving,
and increased craving tolerance.7
Developed meditators
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 213
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
also have the ability to access “highly reinforced” (i.e.,
deep) meditative states,5,6,16,38,48
which may contribute to
cognitive resilience and adaptation in the face of even
highly agitating stressors.7
Buddhist meditation has been
found to increase cerebral plasticity1,6,36,38
and cerebral
blood flow and thickening in brain regions associated with
the mental exercises practiced by concentrative and diffuse
meditation,15–17,36,38,50–56
suggesting a role in potentially
preserving and enhancing brain regions affected by medita-
tion.7
This notion is supported by increased proficiency in
several cognitive skills associated with consistent concentra-
tive and diffuse meditation, such as the highly developed abil-
ity to disengage from distractors during concentration and the
ability to observe one’s internal states in a non-evaluative
way.7–9,33,48,49
Buddhist meditation has been found to increase EEG co-
herence through the synchronization of neural discharges, par-
ticularly in the gamma-band frequency (25–70 Hz).1,5,6,11,37
It is theorized that a brain wave’s amplitude is positively
correlated with the degree of precision with which cells os-
cillate and with the size of the neural population through
which the wave is occurring.62,63
Increased EEG coherence
is correlated with increased brain-processing capacity in
areas such as attention, working memory, learning, and
conscious perception.63–66
Synchronization of oscillatory
neural discharges, particularly of the gamma-band fre-
quency, appears to play a key role in developing the ability
to integrate distributed neural processes into one overarch-
ing network with highly ordered cognitive and affective
functions, suggesting that gamma synchrony may also play
a crucial role in meditation-induced neuroplasticity.63,67
Recent findings have shown that a gradual, meditation-
induced, functional reorganization of brain activity pat-
terns occurs between the default mode network, salience
network, and central executive network; reliance upon the
central executive network and salience network seems to
increase, and reliance upon the default mode network seems
to decrease.37,38
Abnormal activity in this intrinsic connec-
tivity network has been implicated in a number of psychiat-
ric diseases (such as ADHD, bipolar disorder, borderline
personality disorder, depression and anxiety disorders, and
schizophrenia),37,38,44
and an inability to “switch off” the
hyperactive default mode network through focusing the
mind appears to be a major contributor to the disease pro-
cess,44
suggesting that meditation may aid in reducing symp-
toms in these disorders. Future research should consider the
state of the anterior insula in individuals with these disor-
ders. The AI plays a critical role for the initiation of network
switching by the salience network,37
suggesting that AI in-
tegrity or connectivity may be compromised in individuals
with these disorders.
In light of meditation’s effect on this intrinsic connectiv-
ity network, meditation may be contraindicated in cases in
which there are existing deficits in default mode network
function. For example, autistic patients show reduced and
abnormal default mode network activation overall, which
may be neurologically related to difficulty in structuring so-
cial relationships, and they also show an increased reliance
on the central executive network,37,41,42
suggesting that
meditation may act to solidify or exacerbate existing symp-
toms. Therefore, more research is needed to determine how
meditation affects large-scale brain networks associated
with psychopathology in order to ensure the safe and effec-
tive application of meditation in clinical and psychiatric
settings.
The skills developed through meditation practice have
significant implications for the treatment of various psychi-
atric disorders. The neural correlates of these skills may be
partially explained by mechanisms involved in neuroplasti-
city over the lifespan. These findings also support the no-
tion that concentrative and diffuse meditation practices
aid in behavior modification, in attenuating stress-related
illness, and in slowing, and perhaps stopping and rehabili-
tating, neurodegeneration and cognitive decline.7
Future
meditation research should aid in developing a clearer un-
derstanding of how large-scale brain network organization
in the human brain produces cognition. Recent evidence
suggests that cognitive function depends on a dynamic in-
teraction of dissociable large-scale brain networks, such as
recently discovered intrinsic connectivity networks.1,37–39
Determining dissociable large-scale brain networks and
their cognitive and behavioral correlates in healthy individ-
uals would aid in studying psychiatric diseases by providing
a reference point for comparison. Developed meditators
may provide researchers with a more objective point of ref-
erence for healthy brain function than may be found in the
general population.
Research in the field of neurometacognition has already
made progress in understanding the neural correlates of
cognition and complex behavior.1,2,6,13,16,34–38
Continued
research in this field may eventually give rise to a coherent
theoretical framework for the neurobiology of conscious-
ness. Meditation research can provide a framework for
study that will aid in correlating electrophysiological record-
ings with simultaneously activated brain areas and their as-
sociated cognitive and behavioral analogues. Research has
suggested that long-range phase synchrony may serve as a
dynamic mechanism underlying functional interactions in
large-scale brain networks.37
Large-scale functional net-
works (such as the intrinsic connectivity network) and their
cognitive and behavioral correlates may be able to be iden-
tified and differentiated through the comparison of concen-
trative and diffuse meditation conditions using respectively
different fields of experiential awareness (e.g., concentra-
tive meditation on breath sensations versus an external vi-
sual point, and diffuse meditation on body versus thought/
emotion versus external sensory fields), while simulta-
neously recording brain-imaging activity patterns and
concurrent electrophysiology. Participants with expert at-
tentional control are able to direct their attention to
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
214 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
differential fields of awareness at will and continuously,
providing a well-defined and highly controlled activation
of specific attentional fields and associated functional net-
work activity, without the residual activation that may be
produced in untrained populations by the lack attentional
control and by “cortical noise.”16,37,38
Therefore, expert
meditation practitioners may prove to be a valuable popu-
lation for investigating a wide variety of phenomena that
may aid in defining neural correlates of cognition and also
the specific altered states of consciousness, including the
neural correlates of attentional stance; mechanisms underly-
ing dynamic engagement and disengagement of large-scale
networks; how differential large-scale networks cooperate,
compete, or coordinate their activity; how knowledge is
neurologically constructed; how large-scale network imag-
ing correlates with synchronized ocillatory neural activity;
and the neural-physiological correlates of specific meditation-
induced states of consciousness.37
Buddhist practitioners
and literature provide meditation instructions within a
comprehensive theoretical framework of the states of con-
sciousness that may be experienced through meditation,
which may prove useful in guiding and expediting the pro-
cess of meditation itself.33,48,49,102–104
Moreover, this pro-
cess may provide a strong and invaluable unification of
two diametrically opposed methodologies used for the
scientific investigation of consciousness—namely, the intel-
lectual and deductive approach taken by Western scien-
tists and the experiential and inductive approach taken
by Eastern spiritual practitioners.
The complex cognitive methodology involved in medita-
tion presents researchers with unique challenges when de-
termining appropriate controls for the specific variables
under investigation. Many experiments have failed to con-
trol for the type of meditation practice under investigation,
making it difficult or impossible to compare results. As pre-
viously mentioned, meditation styles can differ significantly
in their methodologies and intended results because each
style is explicitly designed to train specific cognitive facul-
ties. Moreover, the neurological correlates of specific prac-
tices differ significantly between populations, such as the
differences one may find between monks, lay persons, adults,
children, and individuals with psychopathology. Therefore,
researchers must be specific about the style of meditation
practice and the population under investigation, or else risk
causing experimental confounds that produce results with
limited or no potential for broader scientific comparison
or application. Similarly, while mindfulness-based treat-
ments in the clinical setting have been found to be effective,
the majority of mindfulness-based treatments rely on other
therapeutic modalities in cooperation with meditation,
such as cognitive therapy, making it difficult to determine the
efficacy of mindfulness practice itself during mindfulness-
based treatment. In order to make results derived from
mindfulness-based practitioners more comparable to other
populations, it may be helpful to determine the effects of
mindfulness practice itself through, for example, longitudi-
nal studies on individuals who have maintained their prac-
tice after the therapy program has been completed. In a
similar vein, longitudinal studies on Buddhist meditation
are lacking overall and would be of great use in determining
neural development and reorganization with meditation ex-
perience and in verifying behavioral and experiential modifi-
cations over time in relation to constant information (e.g.,
Buddhist texts). Longitudinal studies would also aid in
separating out the specific neurological/behavioral factors
affected by meditation itself from those found across indi-
viduals who are already likely to practice meditation.
Research on how meditation affects neurobiological
pathways governing the stress response is lacking overall.
Numerous studies have shown, however, that meditation
has a normalizing effect on psychobiomarkers of stress reg-
ulation and that it produces overall improvements in self-
attributed mood scores.8,9,48,49,69
Moreover, the practice
of meditation has been shown to positively affect cellular
processes that are affected by stress and linked to disease,
as is shown by increased telomerase activity with long-
term meditation practice.70
The aspects of Buddhist theory that have been scientifi-
cally investigated thus far have served mainstream psychol-
ogy and cognitive neuroscience well, making Buddhist
theory a potential avenue for in-depth and critical scientific
investigation. Such investigations may provide a useful and
arguably necessary broadening of perspective with regard
to defining and treating psychopathology. It has been
suggested that the mechanisms underlying psychiatric symp-
tom reduction occur at a level of “personality reorganization,”
because almost all of the commonly defined personality
types—healthy, neurotic, borderline, psychotic, and so
on—can be recognized in terms of personality develop-
ment and organization over time.105
Therefore, in the
context of meditation in the clinical setting, it may be use-
ful to think of mental illness in terms of a symptomatic
continuum, from most to least pathological, with the
aim of progressively reducing symptoms through shifting
the mind and body toward a state of natural balance.105
The Western scientific study of meditation has, in large
part, been conducted with minimal reference to its religious
and spiritual origins, and only a brief overview of the most
fundamental and commonly practiced forms of Buddhist
meditation has been presented here. However, Buddhism
has been successful in maintaining the practice and teach-
ing of insight meditation for over 2500 years, and an exten-
sive and detailed literature is devoted to theoretical aspects
of the stages of meditative experience, or the “process of
insight.”102–105
Investigation of these texts reveals a
theoretical science with corresponding technical medi-
tation instructions that guide the practitioner, in a step-
wise manner, through a process of coming out of suffering
(or attachment to ego) by gradually deepening his or her
knowledge of experiential reality. In short, this process
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 215
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
involves a gradual cumulative development of specific fa-
culties of awareness that the individual uses to literally heal
his or her own body. A detailed synopsis of this process and
its implications is beyond the scope of this article. How-
ever, a wide body of literature is available for readers who
are interested in further exploration.102–105
Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of
interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content
and writing of the article.
REFERENCES
1. Slagter H, Davidson R, Lutz A. Mental training as a tool in
the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plastic-
ity. Front Hum Neurosci 2011;5:1–12.
2. Raffone A, Srinivasan N. The exploration of meditation in
the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cogn Pro-
cess 2010;11:1–7.
3. Young JDE, Taylor E. Meditation as a voluntary hypo-
metabolic state of biological estivation. News Physiol Sci
1998;13:149–53.
4. Sudsaung R, Chantanez V, Veluvan K. Effects of Buddhist
meditation on serum cortisol and total protein levels, blood
pressure, pulse rate, lung volume and reaction time. Physiol
Behav 1990;50:543–8.
5. Lutz A, Brefczyski-Lewis J, Johnstone T, Davidson R. Regu-
lation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion med-
itation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS One 2008;3:
e1897.
6. Lutz A, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Meditation and the neuro-
science of consciousness: an introduction. In: Zelazo P,
Moscovitch M, Thompson E, eds. Cambridge handbook
of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press,
2007:81–109.
7. Dakwar E, Levin FR. The emerging role of meditation in
addressing psychiatric illness, with a focus on substance
use disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2009;17:254–67.
8. Gunaratana H. Mindfulness in plain English. Somerville,
MA: Wisdom, 2007.
9. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context:
past, present, and future. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2003;10:
144–53.
10. Ludwig DS, Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness in medicine. JAMA
2008;300:1350–2.
11. Cahn BR, Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP,
and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull 2006;132:180–211.
12. Goleman D. The Buddha on meditation and states of con-
sciousness. In: Shapiro DH Jr, Valsh RN, eds. Meditation,
classic and contemporary perspectives. Piscataway, NJ:
Aldine Transaction, 2008:345–7.
13. Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Attention reg-
ulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn Sci
2008;12:163–9.
14. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, et al. Mindfulness: a pro-
posed operational definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2004;11:
230–41.
15. Chambers R, Gullone E, Allen NB. Mindful emotion reg-
ulation: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev 2009;
29:560–72.
16. Brefczynski-Lewis JA, Lutz A, Schaefer HS, Levinson DB,
Davidson RJ. Neural correlates of attentional expertise in
long-term meditation practitioners. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
2007;104:11483–8.
17. Farb NAS, Segal ZV, Mayberg H, et al. Attending to the
present: meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-
reference. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2007;2:313–22.
18. Watkins E, Teasdale JD. Rumination and overgeneralization
memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic
thinking. J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110:353–7.
19. Linehan MM. Skills training manual for treating borderline
personality disorder. New York: Guilford, 1993.
20. Hayes SC. Acceptance and change: content and context in
psychotherapy. Reno, NV: Context, 1994.
21. Marlatt GA, Witkiewitz K, Dillworth TM, et al. Vipassana
meditation as a treatment for alcohol and drug use disor-
ders. In: Hayes SC, Follette VM, Linehan MM, eds. Mind-
fulness and acceptance: expanding the cognitive-behavioral
tradition. New York: Guilford, 2004:261–87.
22. Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Williams JM, Ridgeway VA, Soulsby
JM, Lau MA. Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major de-
pression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J Consult
Clin Psychol 2000;68:615–23.
23. Wolf DB, Abell N. Examining the effects of meditation tech-
niques on psychosocial functioning. Res Soc Work Pract
2003;13:27–42.
24. Shannahoff-Khalsa DS. Kundalini Yoga meditation tech-
niques for the treatment of obsessive-compulsive and OC
spectrum disorders. Brief Treat Crisis Interv 2003;3:369–82.
25. Marlatt GA, Marques JK. Meditation, self-control and alco-
hol use. In: Stuart RB, ed. Behavioral self-management:
strategies, techniques and outcome. New York: Brunner/
Mazel, 1977:117–53.
26. Linehan MM, Comtois KA, Murray AM, et al. Two-year
randomized controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical be-
havior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal behaviors
and borderline personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry
2006;62:1–10.
27. Fjorback LO, Walch H. Meditation based therapies—a sys-
tematic review and some clinical observations. Religions
2012;3:1–18.
28. Kabat-Zinn J. Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness
meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion, 1994.
29. Teasdale JD, Segal Z, Williams JM. How does cognitive
therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should atten-
tional control (mindfulness training) help? Behav Res Ther
1995;33:25–39.
30. Kisteller JL, Hallett CB. An exploratory study of meditation-
based intervention for binge eating disorder. J Health Psychol
1999;4:357–63.
31. Kozak A. Wild chickens and petty tyrants: 108 metaphors
for mindfulness. Somerville, MA: Wisdom, 2009.
32. Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Biegel GM. Teaching self-care to
caregivers: effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on
the mental health of therapists in training. Train Educ Prof
Psychol 2007;1:105–15.
33. Kornfield J. A path with heart: a guide through the perils
and promises of spiritual life. New York: Bantam, 1993.
34. Gilbert P. Compassion: conceptualizations, research and use
in psychotherapy. New York: Routledge, 2006.
35. Henry WP, Schacht TE, Strupp HH. Patient and therapist in-
troject, interpersonal process, and differential psychother-
apy outcome. J Consult Clin Psychol 1990;58:768–74.
36. Rubia K. The neurobiology of meditation and its clinical effec-
tiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychol 2009;82:1–11.
37. Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cogni-
tion: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci
2010;14:277–90.
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
216 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
38. Manna A, Raffone A, Perrucci MG, et al. Neural correlates
of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in medita-
tion. Brain Res Bull 2010;82:46–56.
39. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard
DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98:676–82.
40. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Thermenos HW, Milanovic S, et al.
Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network
in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of person with
schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:1279–84.
41. Murias M, Webb SJ, Greenson J, Dawson G. Resting state
cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individ-
uals with autism. Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:270–3.
42. Uddin LQ, Menon V. The anterior insula in autism: under-
connected and under-examined. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2009;33:1198–203.
43. Liao W, Zhang Z, Pan Z, et al. Default mode network ab-
normalities in medial temporal lobe epilepsy: a study com-
bining fMRI and DRT. Hum Brain Mapp 2011;32:883–95.
44. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM. Default mode network activ-
ity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psy-
chol 2012;8:49–76.
45. Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP. Increased
amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in
anxiety-prone subjects. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:318–27.
46. Feinstein JS, Stein MB, Paulus MP. Anterior insula reactiv-
ity during certain decisions is associated with neuroticism.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2006;1:136–42.
47. Daniels JK, McFarlane AC, Bluhm RL, et al. Switching be-
tween executive and default mode networks in post trau-
matic stress disorder: alteration in functional connectivity.
J Psychiatry Neurosci 2010;35:258–66.
48. Goenka SN. Mahasatipatthana sutta: the great discourse on
establishing awareness. Seattle, WA: Vipassana Research In-
stitute, 1996.
49. Hanh TN. The miracle of mindfulness. Boston, MA:
Beacon, 1976.
50. Deitrich A. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of
consciousness: the transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Con-
scious Cogn 2003;12:231–56.
51. Lou HC, Norwak M, Kjaer TW. The mental self. Prog Brain
Res 2005;150:197–204.
52. Luders E, Kurth F, Mayer EA, Toga AW, Narr KL, Gaser C.
The unique brain anatomy of meditation practitioners:
alterations in cortical gyrification. Front Hum Neurosci
2012;6:34.
53. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman R, et al. Meditation experi-
ence is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neu-
roreport 2005;16:1983–7.
54. Hozel BK, Ott U, Gard T, et al. Investigation of mindful-
ness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphome-
try. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2008;3:55–61.
55. Luders E, Toga A, Lepore N, Gaser C. The underlying ana-
tomical correlates of long-term meditation: larger hippo-
campal and frontal volumes of grey matter. Neuroimage
2009;45:673–8.
56. Grant JA, Courtemanche J, Duerden EG, Duncan GH,
Rainville P. Cortical thickness and pain sensitivity in Zen
meditators. Emotion 2010;10:43–53.
57. Baerensten KB, Stodklide-Jorgensen H, Sommerlund B,
et al. An investigation of brain processes supporting medita-
tion. Cogn Process 2010;11:57–84.
58. Critchley HD. Neural mechanisms of automatic, affective,
and cognitive integration. J Comp Neurol 2005;495;154–66.
59. Kurth F, Zilles K, Fox PT, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB. A link be-
tween the systems: functional differentiation and integration
within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain
Struct Funct 2010;214:519–34.
60. Mutschler I, Wiekhorst B, Kowalevski S, et al. Functional
organization of the human anterior cingulated cortex. Neu-
rosci Lett 2009;457–60.
61. Damasio AR. The feeling of what happens: body an emo-
tion in the making of consciousness. Orlando, FL: Harcourt,
1994.
62. Niedermeyer E, da Silva FL. Electroencephalography: basic
principles, clinical applications, and related fields. New York:
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2004.
63. Lutz A, Greischar L, Rawlings N, Ricard M, Davidson J.
Long term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma
synchrony during mental practice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
2004;101:16369–73.
64. Sheppard WD. Pretrial EEF coherence as a predictor of se-
mantic processing effects in a lexical decision test. Disserta-
tion Abstr Int 1998;49:B3610.
65. Sheppard WD, Boyer RW. Pretrial EEG coherence as a pre-
dictor of semantic priming effects. Brain Lang 1990;39:37–68.
66. Cardin JA, Carlen M, Meletis K, et al. Driving fast-spiking
cells induces gamma rhythm and control sensory responses.
Nature 2009:459:663–7.
67. Baldeweg T, Spence S, Hirsch SR, Gruzelier J. Gamma-band
electroencephalographic oscillations in a patient with so-
matic hallucinations. Lancet 1998;352:620–1.
68. Leeuwen S, Singer W, Melloni L. Meditation increases the
depth of information processing and improves the allocation
of attention in space. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:133.
69. Lama D, Cutler HC. The art of happiness: a handbook for
living. New York: Riverhead, 2009.
70. Jacobs TL, Epel ES, Lin J, et al. Intensive meditation train-
ing, immune cell telomerase activity, and psychological med-
itators. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011;36:664–81.
71. Blackburn E. Structure and function of telomeres. Nature
1991;350:569–73.
72. Frenck RW, Blackburn EH. The rate of telomere sequence
loss in human leukocytes varies with age. Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A 1998;95:5607–10.
73. Fossel M. Role of cell senescence in human aging. J Anti-
aging Med 2000;3:91–8.
74. Epel ES. Telomeres in a life-span perspective: a new “psy-
chobiomarker”? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009;18:6–10.
75. Cawthon RK, Smith E, O’Brien E, Sivatchenko A, Kerber
RA. Association between telomere length in blood and
mortality in people aged 60 years of older. Lancet 2003;
361:393–5.
76. Njajou OT, Hsueh WC, Blackburn EH, et al. Association
between telomere lengths, specific causes of death, and years
of healthy life in health, aging, and body composition, a
population-based cohort study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med
Sci 2009;64:860–4.
77. Epel ES, Merkin SS, Cawthon R, et al. The rate of leukocyte
telomere shortening predicts mortality from cardiovascular
disease in elderly men. Aging 2008;1:81–8.
78. Nordfjall K, Svenson U, Norrback KF, Adolfsson R, Lenner
P, Roos G. The individual blood cell telomere attrition
rate is telomere length dependent. PLoS Genet 2009;5:
e1000375.
79. Farzaneh-Far R, Lin J, Epel E, Lapham K, Blackburn E.
Telomere length trajectory and its determinants in persons
with coronary artery disease: longitudinal findings from
the heart and soul study. PLoS One 2010;5:e8612.
80. Epel ES, Blackburn EH, Lin J, et al. Accelerated telomere
shortening in response to life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
2004;101:17312–5.
Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation
Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 217
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
81. Epel ES, Lin J, Wilhelm FH, et al. Cell aging in relation to
stress arousal and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Psy-
choneuroendocrinology 2006;31:277–87.
82. Porges SW. Cardiac vagal tone: a physiological index of stress.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995;19:225–33.
83. Carney RM, Blumenthal JA, Stein PK. Depression, heart rate
variability, and acute myocardial infarction. Circulation 2001;
104:2024–8.
84. Seligman MEP. Learned helplessness. Annu Rev Med 1972;23:
407–12.
85. Averill JR. Personal control over aversive stimuli and its re-
lationship to stress. Psychol Bull 1973;80:286–303.
86. Wallston KA, Wallston BS, Smith S, Dobbins CJ. Perceived
control and health. Curr Psychol Res Rev 1987;6:5–25.
87. Vollmayr B, Henn FA. Learned helplessness in the rat:
improvements in validity and reliability. Brain Res Brain
Res Protoc 2001;8:1–7.
88. Mausbach BT, Patterson TL, von Kanel R, et al. Personal
mastery attenuates the effect of caregiving stress on psychi-
atric morbidity. J Nerv Ment Dis 2006;194:132–4.
89. Mausbach BT, von Kanel R, Patterson TL, et al. The mod-
erating effect of personal mastery on the relations between
stress and plamninogen activator inhibitor-1 (PAI-1) antigen.
Health Psychol 2008;27:S172–9.
90. Krause N, Stryker S. Stress and well-being: the buffering role
of locus of control beliefs. Soc Sci Med 1984;18:783–90.
91. Litt MD. Cognitive mediators of stressful experience: self effi-
cacy and perceived control. Cognit Ther Res 1988;12:241–60.
92. Chorpita BF, Barlow DH. 1998. The development of anxi-
ety: the role of control in early development. Psychol Bull
1998;124:3–21.
93. Brody S, Veit R, Rau H. Neuroticism but not cardiovascu-
lar stress reactivity is associated with less longitudinal blood
pressure increase. Pers Individ Dif 1996;20:375–80.
94. Schneider TR. The role of neuroticism on psychological and
physiological stress responses. J Exp Soc Psychol 2004;40:
795–804.
95. Shurgot GR, Knight BG. Influence of neuroticism, ethnic-
ity, familism, and social support on perceived burden in
dementia caregivers: pilot test of the transactional stress
and social support model. J Gerentol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
2005;60B:331–4.
96. Suls J. Affect, stress and personality. In: Forgas JP, ed. Hand-
book of affect and social cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum,
2001:392–409.
97. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine
for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindful-
ness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary
results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1982;4:33–47.
98. Fredrickson BL, Cohn MA, Coffey KA, Pek J, Finkel SM.
Open hearts build lives: positive emotions, induced through
loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal
resources. J Pers Soc Psychol 2008; 5:1045–62.
99. Pace TW, Negi LT, Adame DD, et al. Effect of compassion
meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune, and behav-
ioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocri-
nology 2009;34:87–98.
100. Brand S, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Naranjo JR, Schmidt S. In-
fluence of mindfulness practice on cortisol and sleep in
long-term and short-term meditators. Neuropsychobiology
2012;65:109–18.
101. National Institutes of Health. At http://www.nimh.nih.gov/
statistics/index.shtml
102. Buddhaghosa B; Nyanamoli B, trans. The path of purifica-
tion: Visuddhimagga. Kandy, Sri Lanka: Buddhist Publica-
tion Society, 1991.
103. Piyadhassi Bhikkhu. Vimuttidhamma: from chakra to dham-
machakra. Thailand: Dhamma Publication Fund, 2011.
104. Ingram D. Mastering the core teachings of the Buddha:
an unusually hardcore Dhamma book. London: Aeon,
2008.
105. Wilber K, Engler J, Brown DP. Transformations of con-
sciousness: conventional and contemplative perspectives on
development. Boston, MA: Shambala, 1986.
C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya
218 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013
Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Contenu connexe

Tendances

Mindfulness Implications For Substance Abuse And Addiction
Mindfulness   Implications For Substance Abuse And AddictionMindfulness   Implications For Substance Abuse And Addiction
Mindfulness Implications For Substance Abuse And Addictionpharho
 
Fish d iscovering water meditation as a process of recognition
Fish d iscovering water   meditation as a process of recognitionFish d iscovering water   meditation as a process of recognition
Fish d iscovering water meditation as a process of recognitioncarmodyj
 
Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...
Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...
Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...Dr. Prabhakar Manu
 
A buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousness
A buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousnessA buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousness
A buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousnessteguh.qi
 
Adravya chikitsa
Adravya chikitsaAdravya chikitsa
Adravya chikitsaChaitra Rao
 
Spirituality Training For Palliative Care Fellows
Spirituality  Training For  Palliative  Care  FellowsSpirituality  Training For  Palliative  Care  Fellows
Spirituality Training For Palliative Care FellowsMasa Nakata
 
The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...
The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...
The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...Université de Montréal
 
What is Psychotherapy Integration?
What is Psychotherapy Integration?What is Psychotherapy Integration?
What is Psychotherapy Integration?Maurice Prout
 
research project
research projectresearch project
research projectFang Zhang
 
7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy
7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy
7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapyexquisitemind
 
Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)
Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)
Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)Daryush Parvinbenam
 

Tendances (20)

Buddhism and psychotherapy
Buddhism and  psychotherapyBuddhism and  psychotherapy
Buddhism and psychotherapy
 
Meditation [Dhyan] Versus Relaxation : A Review With Comprehensive Bibliography
Meditation [Dhyan] Versus Relaxation : A Review With Comprehensive BibliographyMeditation [Dhyan] Versus Relaxation : A Review With Comprehensive Bibliography
Meditation [Dhyan] Versus Relaxation : A Review With Comprehensive Bibliography
 
Meditation
MeditationMeditation
Meditation
 
Does Raja Yoga Meditation Bring Out Physiological and Psychological General W...
Does Raja Yoga Meditation Bring Out Physiological and Psychological General W...Does Raja Yoga Meditation Bring Out Physiological and Psychological General W...
Does Raja Yoga Meditation Bring Out Physiological and Psychological General W...
 
Mindfulness Implications For Substance Abuse And Addiction
Mindfulness   Implications For Substance Abuse And AddictionMindfulness   Implications For Substance Abuse And Addiction
Mindfulness Implications For Substance Abuse And Addiction
 
BK-Rajajayoga - An Evidence-Based Mental Silence Type of Meditation
BK-Rajajayoga - An Evidence-Based Mental Silence Type of MeditationBK-Rajajayoga - An Evidence-Based Mental Silence Type of Meditation
BK-Rajajayoga - An Evidence-Based Mental Silence Type of Meditation
 
Fish d iscovering water meditation as a process of recognition
Fish d iscovering water   meditation as a process of recognitionFish d iscovering water   meditation as a process of recognition
Fish d iscovering water meditation as a process of recognition
 
Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...
Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...
Application of Satvavajaya (Psychotherapy) techniques in Madatyaya (Alcoholis...
 
A buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousness
A buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousnessA buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousness
A buddhist perspective of modern psychotherapy &amp; evolution of consciousness
 
Adravya chikitsa
Adravya chikitsaAdravya chikitsa
Adravya chikitsa
 
Spirituality Training For Palliative Care Fellows
Spirituality  Training For  Palliative  Care  FellowsSpirituality  Training For  Palliative  Care  Fellows
Spirituality Training For Palliative Care Fellows
 
Mindfulness for Mental Health Professionals
Mindfulness for Mental Health ProfessionalsMindfulness for Mental Health Professionals
Mindfulness for Mental Health Professionals
 
The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...
The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...
The Gurū-Chelā Relationship Revisited: A Review of the Work of Indian Psychia...
 
Mano vishaya
Mano vishayaMano vishaya
Mano vishaya
 
What is Psychotherapy Integration?
What is Psychotherapy Integration?What is Psychotherapy Integration?
What is Psychotherapy Integration?
 
Brain
BrainBrain
Brain
 
Meditation states and traits eeg, erp and neuroimaging studies (cahn & poli...
Meditation states and traits   eeg, erp and neuroimaging studies (cahn & poli...Meditation states and traits   eeg, erp and neuroimaging studies (cahn & poli...
Meditation states and traits eeg, erp and neuroimaging studies (cahn & poli...
 
research project
research projectresearch project
research project
 
7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy
7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy
7 Principles of Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy
 
Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)
Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)
Meditation as Medication Mastering the Art of Mindfulness (Long Version)
 

En vedette

There is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_intro
There is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_introThere is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_intro
There is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_introElsa von Licy
 
Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'
Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'
Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'Elsa von Licy
 
Les plantes superieures divines et ou diaboliques
Les plantes superieures  divines et ou diaboliquesLes plantes superieures  divines et ou diaboliques
Les plantes superieures divines et ou diaboliquesElsa von Licy
 
Spr ue3.1 poly cours et exercices
Spr ue3.1   poly cours et exercicesSpr ue3.1   poly cours et exercices
Spr ue3.1 poly cours et exercicesElsa von Licy
 
Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...
Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...
Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...Elsa von Licy
 
Pon detailed table-of-contents
Pon detailed table-of-contentsPon detailed table-of-contents
Pon detailed table-of-contentsElsa von Licy
 
Tomberg and anthroposophy
Tomberg and anthroposophyTomberg and anthroposophy
Tomberg and anthroposophyElsa von Licy
 
La lecture-et-les-livres
La lecture-et-les-livresLa lecture-et-les-livres
La lecture-et-les-livresElsa von Licy
 
Loptimismedescho00rzew
Loptimismedescho00rzewLoptimismedescho00rzew
Loptimismedescho00rzewElsa von Licy
 
1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous
1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous
1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vousElsa von Licy
 
Le système corps miroir de guérison la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...
Le système corps miroir de guérison   la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...Le système corps miroir de guérison   la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...
Le système corps miroir de guérison la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...Elsa von Licy
 
Julius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occulta
Julius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occultaJulius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occulta
Julius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occultaElsa von Licy
 
0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite
0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite
0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussiteElsa von Licy
 
Methodes travail etudiants
Methodes travail etudiantsMethodes travail etudiants
Methodes travail etudiantsElsa von Licy
 

En vedette (20)

There is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_intro
There is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_introThere is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_intro
There is no_such_thing_as_a_social_science_intro
 
Prolegomena 4 0
Prolegomena 4 0Prolegomena 4 0
Prolegomena 4 0
 
Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'
Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'
Commento a 'meditazione sui tarocchi'
 
Les plantes superieures divines et ou diaboliques
Les plantes superieures  divines et ou diaboliquesLes plantes superieures  divines et ou diaboliques
Les plantes superieures divines et ou diaboliques
 
Spr ue3.1 poly cours et exercices
Spr ue3.1   poly cours et exercicesSpr ue3.1   poly cours et exercices
Spr ue3.1 poly cours et exercices
 
Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...
Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...
Styles of Scientific Reasoning, Scientific Practices and Argument in Science ...
 
Valentin tomberg
Valentin tombergValentin tomberg
Valentin tomberg
 
K t
K tK t
K t
 
Pon detailed table-of-contents
Pon detailed table-of-contentsPon detailed table-of-contents
Pon detailed table-of-contents
 
Tomberg and anthroposophy
Tomberg and anthroposophyTomberg and anthroposophy
Tomberg and anthroposophy
 
Lefocculte
LefocculteLefocculte
Lefocculte
 
La lecture-et-les-livres
La lecture-et-les-livresLa lecture-et-les-livres
La lecture-et-les-livres
 
Loptimismedescho00rzew
Loptimismedescho00rzewLoptimismedescho00rzew
Loptimismedescho00rzew
 
1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous
1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous
1 methode cerep_pour_augmenter_votre_efficacite_organisez_vous
 
No mans land pdf
No mans land pdfNo mans land pdf
No mans land pdf
 
Le système corps miroir de guérison la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...
Le système corps miroir de guérison   la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...Le système corps miroir de guérison   la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...
Le système corps miroir de guérison la méthode - un cours pratique - martin...
 
Julius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occulta
Julius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occultaJulius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occulta
Julius evola-sugli-strumenti-della-guerra-occulta
 
0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite
0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite
0 methode cerep_avant_propos_un_seul_objectif_votre_reussite
 
Methodes travail etudiants
Methodes travail etudiantsMethodes travail etudiants
Methodes travail etudiants
 
Zen et-heureux
Zen et-heureuxZen et-heureux
Zen et-heureux
 

Similaire à Principles and neurobiological_correlates_of.5

Meditation pnas 2
Meditation pnas 2Meditation pnas 2
Meditation pnas 2cpavian
 
Health Meditation benifits by Alison Dadow
Health Meditation benifits by Alison DadowHealth Meditation benifits by Alison Dadow
Health Meditation benifits by Alison DadowAlison Dadow
 
Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...
Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...
Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...Jonathan Dunnemann
 
Five Popular Theories of Counseling Psychology
Five Popular Theories of Counseling PsychologyFive Popular Theories of Counseling Psychology
Five Popular Theories of Counseling PsychologyShivaniPadole
 
Why meditation
Why meditationWhy meditation
Why meditationsmukhimmel
 
Meditation
MeditationMeditation
Meditationfitango
 
Understanding Holistic Health.PDF
Understanding Holistic Health.PDFUnderstanding Holistic Health.PDF
Understanding Holistic Health.PDFDEEPENDRA MERADEV
 
Surname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMin
Surname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMinSurname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMin
Surname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMinlisandrai1k
 
_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf
_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf
_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdfKnowing Overt
 
ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...
ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...
ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...Jennifer Roman
 
Mind Body Applications In Mental Health Rj
Mind Body Applications In Mental Health RjMind Body Applications In Mental Health Rj
Mind Body Applications In Mental Health Rjburaikyx
 
Mindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity Back
Mindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity BackMindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity Back
Mindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity Backssuserd966ee1
 
Jean watson theory
Jean watson theoryJean watson theory
Jean watson theorySapna Thakur
 
SPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptx
SPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptxSPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptx
SPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptxKunal Verma
 
Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...
Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...
Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...SharpBrains
 
Psychotherapy -Individual & Group
Psychotherapy -Individual & GroupPsychotherapy -Individual & Group
Psychotherapy -Individual & Groupdivya2709
 
Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...
Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...
Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...Family Constellation
 
BARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptx
BARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptxBARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptx
BARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptxmousaderhem1
 

Similaire à Principles and neurobiological_correlates_of.5 (20)

Meditation pnas 2
Meditation pnas 2Meditation pnas 2
Meditation pnas 2
 
Health Meditation benifits by Alison Dadow
Health Meditation benifits by Alison DadowHealth Meditation benifits by Alison Dadow
Health Meditation benifits by Alison Dadow
 
Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...
Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...
Translating Spiritual Ideals into Daily Life: The Eight-Point Program of Pass...
 
Five Popular Theories of Counseling Psychology
Five Popular Theories of Counseling PsychologyFive Popular Theories of Counseling Psychology
Five Popular Theories of Counseling Psychology
 
Why meditation
Why meditationWhy meditation
Why meditation
 
Meditation
MeditationMeditation
Meditation
 
Understanding Holistic Health.PDF
Understanding Holistic Health.PDFUnderstanding Holistic Health.PDF
Understanding Holistic Health.PDF
 
Surname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMin
Surname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMinSurname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMin
Surname 1Student’s NameProfessor’s NameCourse TitleMin
 
Watson theory
Watson theoryWatson theory
Watson theory
 
_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf
_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf
_Mindfulness Meditation_ A Journey to Inner Harmony and Bliss_.pdf
 
ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...
ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...
ADDICTA 2(2) The Importance Of Mindfulness In Preventing Substance-Abuse Rela...
 
Mind Body Applications In Mental Health Rj
Mind Body Applications In Mental Health RjMind Body Applications In Mental Health Rj
Mind Body Applications In Mental Health Rj
 
Mindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity Back
Mindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity BackMindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity Back
Mindfulness Basics- Core Skills For Anxiety and Gaining Your Creativity Back
 
Jean watson theory
Jean watson theoryJean watson theory
Jean watson theory
 
SPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptx
SPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptxSPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptx
SPIRITUALITY & MODERNITY:- GIVE RISE TO MINDFULNESS, WELLBEING & GROWTH.pptx
 
Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...
Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...
Scal­ing up med­i­ta­tion and mind­ful­ness via well­ness pro­grams and biofe...
 
Psychotherapy -Individual & Group
Psychotherapy -Individual & GroupPsychotherapy -Individual & Group
Psychotherapy -Individual & Group
 
Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...
Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...
Yildiz Sethi - Does the Process of Family Constellations Improve Relationship...
 
BARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptx
BARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptxBARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptx
BARBARA DOSEY'S THEORY OF INTEGRAL NURSING(1).pptx
 
Watson theory
Watson theoryWatson theory
Watson theory
 

Plus de Elsa von Licy

Strategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfi
Strategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfiStrategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfi
Strategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfiElsa von Licy
 
Neuropsychophysiologie
NeuropsychophysiologieNeuropsychophysiologie
NeuropsychophysiologieElsa von Licy
 
L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages 184 ko)
L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages   184 ko)L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages   184 ko)
L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages 184 ko)Elsa von Licy
 
C1 clef pour_la_neuro
C1 clef pour_la_neuroC1 clef pour_la_neuro
C1 clef pour_la_neuroElsa von Licy
 
Vuillez jean philippe_p01
Vuillez jean philippe_p01Vuillez jean philippe_p01
Vuillez jean philippe_p01Elsa von Licy
 
Plan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 p
Plan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 pPlan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 p
Plan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 pElsa von Licy
 
Bioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutions
Bioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutionsBioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutions
Bioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutionsElsa von Licy
 
Poly histologie-et-embryologie-medicales
Poly histologie-et-embryologie-medicalesPoly histologie-et-embryologie-medicales
Poly histologie-et-embryologie-medicalesElsa von Licy
 
Atelier.etude.efficace
Atelier.etude.efficaceAtelier.etude.efficace
Atelier.etude.efficaceElsa von Licy
 
A short history of man — progress and decline
A short history of man — progress and declineA short history of man — progress and decline
A short history of man — progress and declineElsa von Licy
 
Valentin tomberg platonic soul
Valentin tomberg platonic soulValentin tomberg platonic soul
Valentin tomberg platonic soulElsa von Licy
 

Plus de Elsa von Licy (20)

Strategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfi
Strategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfiStrategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfi
Strategie Decisions Incertitude Actes conference fnege xerfi
 
Rainville pierre
Rainville pierreRainville pierre
Rainville pierre
 
Neuropsychophysiologie
NeuropsychophysiologieNeuropsychophysiologie
Neuropsychophysiologie
 
L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages 184 ko)
L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages   184 ko)L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages   184 ko)
L agressivite en psychanalyse (21 pages 184 ko)
 
C1 clef pour_la_neuro
C1 clef pour_la_neuroC1 clef pour_la_neuro
C1 clef pour_la_neuro
 
Hemostase polycop
Hemostase polycopHemostase polycop
Hemostase polycop
 
Antiphilos
AntiphilosAntiphilos
Antiphilos
 
Vuillez jean philippe_p01
Vuillez jean philippe_p01Vuillez jean philippe_p01
Vuillez jean philippe_p01
 
Plan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 p
Plan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 pPlan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 p
Plan de cours all l1 l2l3m1m2 p
 
M2 bmc2007 cours01
M2 bmc2007 cours01M2 bmc2007 cours01
M2 bmc2007 cours01
 
Feuilletage
FeuilletageFeuilletage
Feuilletage
 
Chapitre 1
Chapitre 1Chapitre 1
Chapitre 1
 
Biophy
BiophyBiophy
Biophy
 
Bioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutions
Bioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutionsBioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutions
Bioph pharm 1an-viscosit-des_liquides_et_des_solutions
 
Poly histologie-et-embryologie-medicales
Poly histologie-et-embryologie-medicalesPoly histologie-et-embryologie-medicales
Poly histologie-et-embryologie-medicales
 
Atelier.etude.efficace
Atelier.etude.efficaceAtelier.etude.efficace
Atelier.etude.efficace
 
Swa 2015 final
Swa 2015 finalSwa 2015 final
Swa 2015 final
 
A short history of man — progress and decline
A short history of man — progress and declineA short history of man — progress and decline
A short history of man — progress and decline
 
Valentin tomberg platonic soul
Valentin tomberg platonic soulValentin tomberg platonic soul
Valentin tomberg platonic soul
 
Trinosofia
TrinosofiaTrinosofia
Trinosofia
 

Dernier

18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...RKavithamani
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinRaunakKeshri1
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 

Dernier (20)

18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 

Principles and neurobiological_correlates_of.5

  • 1. Principles and Neurobiological Correlates of Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Craig Mehrmann, BS, and Rakesh Karmacharya, MD, PhD Keywords: Buddhism, central executive network, default mode network, diffuse attention, focused attention, insight, intrinsic connectivity network, meditation, mindfulness, salience network T he term meditation encompasses a broad variety of mental-training practices that vary between cultures and traditions, ranging from techniques designed to promote physical health, relaxation, and improved concen- tration, to exercises performed with farther-reaching goals, such as developing a heightened sense of well-being, cul- tivating altruistic behaviors, and, for some, attaining en- lightenment.1 Meditation can be conceptualized as complex emotional and attentional regulatory practices in which men- tal and somatic events are affected by specific mental-training practices.2 Meditation is typically associated with a concur- rent state of heightened vigilant awareness and reduced metabolic activity3 —which lead to improved physical health,4 psychological balance, and emotional stability.5 Not all med- itation practices focus on the training of specific cognitive skills, and in those that do, the methodologies and out- comes often vary. It is therefore essential to be explicit about the type of meditation practice under investigation. Different types of meditation can be classified based on how the practitioner’s attentional processes are regulated and directed.6 In this article we focus on the two most com- mon styles of meditation derived from Buddhist traditions: (1) concentrative, or focused-attention, meditation, and (2) diffuse, or open-monitoring, meditation. Concentrative meditation involves maintaining and continually refocusing attention on a chosen object, such as a body sensation, single point in space, color, object, sound, or affective state such as compassion. Open-monitoring meditation involves developing a present-centered and unattached/neutral mode of observa- tion toward all sensational phenomena, including thoughts. Concentrative and diffuse meditation practices are of- ten combined, whether in a single session or over the course of the practitioner’s training, and are intentionally designed to train specific cognitive processes.6 In many instances, the attentional stance cultivated through meditation not only is characterized by the style of meditation (i.e., concen- trative or diffuse) but also includes the awareness of certain philosophical principles (or metaphysical characteristics) that support practitioners’ ability to impartially perceive their reality. In other words, the attentional stance often integrates the continuous awareness of certain metaphys- ical characteristics that act as a medium through which practitioners may objectively observe their sensory experi- ence. Such characteristics often include the perpetual tran- sience of all phenomena, the illusion of a separate and permanent self or ego, and the understanding that one will never ultimately be satisfied by sense objects—hence the term insight meditation, which refers to a form of diffuse meditation that is practiced with the continual awareness or experience of these philosophical principles, and with an emphasis on experiencing impermanence or change (i.e., the continual arising and passing of phenomena from moment to moment). Western scientific interest in meditation has grown tre- mendously in recent years, reflecting the increased appli- cation of meditation-based modalities in clinical settings. However, the scientific study of meditation has, in large part, been conducted with minimal reference to its religious and spiritual origins.7 Buddhist traditions offer widely ap- plied, extensive, precisely descriptive, and highly detailed theories about concentrative and diffuse forms of medita- tion, which have remained intrinsically unchanged for over 2500 years and are presented in a systematic manner befit- ting a scientific approach.6 Mindfulness-based therapeutic programs are all derivatives of traditional Buddhist medita- tion. The theory and practice of concentrative, diffuse, and insight meditation are at the core of these programs, making these meditation practices the basis for most applications in clinical and psychiatric settings.1,8,9 Mindfulness-based therapeutic programs have become a popular form of therapeutic intervention: they are efficacious; their princi- ples are universal; they are cost-effective; and they are From the Center for Human Genetic Research, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA (Mr. Mehrmann and Dr. Karmacharya); Harvard Medical School and Chemical Biology Program, Broad Institute of Harvard and MIT (Dr. Karmacharya); Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Program, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA (Dr. Karmacharya). Supported, in part, by National Institutes of Health Mentored Clinical Sci- entist Research Career Development Award no. K08MH086846 (RK). Correspondence: Rakesh Karmacharya, MD, PhD, Massachusetts General Hospital, Center for Human Genetic Research, 185 Cambridge St., Boston, MA 02114. Email: karmacharya@mcb.harvard.edu © 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College DOI: 10.1097/HRP.0b013e31828e8ef4 NEUROBIOLOGY Editor: Rakesh Karmacharya, MD, PhD Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 205 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 2. highly adaptable for diverse populations with different cog- nitive abilities.7,10 Moreover, these programs have made training in concentrative and diffuse meditation more ac- cessible by offering what one may consider a nonreligious alternative for individuals who may be unwilling to adopt Buddhist terminology.7 A third style of meditation, known as transcendental meditation (TM), has been extensively researched and en- compasses the remaining forms of meditation practice. TM practice is centered on the repetition of a mantra, and emphasizes the development of a witnessing, thought- free “transcendental awareness” or “pure consciousness” that is described by practitioners to be “the original source of thought.”11 This state is explicitly said to be cultivated in the absence of any concentration or effort and is charac- terized by the experiences of “unboundedness” and “loss of time, space, and body sense,” thus placing TM in a medita- tion category of its own.1,11 TM is not easily comparable and is relatively unrelated to mindfulness-based meditation techniques and therefore will not be discussed here. The theory and practice of meditation are not grounded in mysticism or in religious dogma, rites, rituals, or ceremo- nies, and may rather be accurately described as composing a detailed and precisely descriptive scientific technique. Me- ditation is universal in nature; meditation methodologies are equally applicable to individuals of any religious, cultural, or spiritual background; and the practice of meditation does not require any form of conversion or identification with a religious tradition. Many Westerners, however, are unfamil- iar with the cultural context in which the terminology of Buddhist meditation has developed. This lack of knowl- edge has, in some cases, resulted in misinterpretations of meditation practices as well as in unwarranted religious or dogmatic stigma being attached to them. To help bridge this cultural boundary, we present an introduction to the principles and clinical applications of concentrative and diffuse forms of meditation. We also review relevant scientific studies to date on concentrative and diffuse meditation, and discuss the clin- ical implications of these findings. Following a discussion of the philosophical basis and neurological underpinnings of these practices and how they apply to modern psychiatry, we suggest further directions for scientific research. CONCENTRATIVE AND DIFFUSE MEDITATION Concentrative meditation is often practiced in preparation for practicing diffuse meditation and, as noted earlier, en- tails voluntarily focusing attention on a chosen object in a sustained fashion, such as breath sensations or a visual object, visualized image, sound, or affective state (e.g., compassion).6 To sustain this focus, practitioners must constantly monitor and regulate their quality of attention, thereby naturally developing three regulatory skills: (1) to monitor and re- main vigilant to distractions, thereby enabling practitioners to recognize when and to what degree their attention has wandered from the object of focus, (2) to release distractions from the attentional field without further involvement, and (3) to restore attention promptly to the chosen object of focus.1,6 Concentrative meditation develops the three regulative skills to the point that advanced practitioners have an espe- cially acute ability to notice when the mind has wandered, resulting in an automatic and instantaneous restoration of attention on the chosen object of focus.6 Eventually, atten- tion rests more readily and stably on the chosen object. As concentration continues to develop, the regulative skills are invoked less and less frequently, and soon the ability to sus- tain focus becomes “effortless,” a state commonly known as access concentration.1,12 The achievement of access con- centration is often considered the appropriate time to begin practicing diffuse meditation; however, this relatively high degree of concentration is not absolutely necessary for the beginning stages of diffuse meditation. Diffuse meditation is done with the aim of becoming more objectively aware of sensate experience through the de- velopment of one’s ability to be psychologically present and equanimous with all forms of sensory phenomena. This state involves the nonreactive monitoring of the ongoing stream of experience from moment to moment, without fo- cusing on any explicit object for a prolonged period of time.13 Practitioners aim to familiarize themselves with re- ality at the pure sensate level, without interpretation of, or identification with, sensational phenomena. In the context of meditation, sensate refers to anything that can be men- tally observed, including thoughts. The prelude to diffuse meditation typically involves con- centrative meditation to calm and focus the mind, reduce distractions, and enhance one’s equanimity toward sensa- tional phenomena. For example, the practitioner may use concentrative meditation focusing on breath sensations at the area of the philtrum (the indentation below the nostrils and above the upper lip). Continually monitoring breath sensations in the chosen area develops equanimity (or non- reactivity) toward body sensations and enables the practi- tioner to disengage from, quiet, and eventually stop the internal dialogue of the mind. From here, the practitioner may shift to diffuse meditation even if access concentra- tion has not yet been established, so long as the awareness of breath sensations is maintained with relative continuity. The practitioner would then work at continually disenga- ging from body-mind sensations as quickly as possible until a continual stream of awareness, or access concentration, has been established. Otherwise, as previously described, practitioners would practice concentrative meditation on breath sensations until access concentration has been estab- lished, and then redirect their focus from breath sensations to begin practicing diffuse meditation. Usually, practi- tioners direct their attention to the most subtle (or least per- ceptible) features of experience possible. Open-monitoring meditation enhances one’s faculty of present-centered, nonjudgmental awareness of psychosomatic C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 206 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 3. phenomena, thereby enhancing cognitive flexibility and reappraisal.1 This state is accomplished through nonreac- tive, metacognitive monitoring of body-mind sensations— which gradually increases awareness of “automatic cognitive and emotional interpretations of sensory, perceptual, and endogenous stimuli.”1,13–15 With consistent practice, the practitioner becomes progressively more aware of sub- tle body-mind sensations, lending access to rich features of sensational experience, such as emotional tone, ac- tive cognitive schema, awareness of sensation all over the body (inside and out), degree of phenomenal intensity, and bodily vibrational frequency6 (i.e., the literal, physical expe- rience of the body and mind as “vibrations,” or rapidly os- cillating particular sensational phenomena whose strength [amplitude] and speed [frequency] fluctuate over time). One also develops the ability to experience the arising and passing of phenomena at a high velocity and at very subtle levels6 —that is, the literal and distinct physical or mental ex- perience of the arising and passing of particular phenomena up to 50 times per second. The subtlety of the practi- tioners’ experience is in direct accordance with their con- centrative capacity (the degree of effort exerted to observe the subtlest sensations possible while remaining continu- ously aware of, and equanimous toward, the internal sen- sate experience). Practitioners continue to practice meditation’s concen- trative or diffuse techniques in everyday life, which enables them to maintain a wholesome lifestyle and contributes to their degree of readily accessible concentrative absorption during formal meditation. The degree of concentrative ab- sorption refers to the depth or strength of concentration on a single-pointed focus and, during formal meditation, correlates directly with how well the continuity of aware- ness has been maintained since the previous meditation ses- sion. For example, practitioners may strive to maintain a continuous awareness of breath sensations or of an affec- tive state such as compassion, or try to maintain a continu- ous superficial “body-scan” throughout the day. Advanced practitioners report a sense of physical lightness and in- creased mental and physical vitality as a result of maintaining a concentrated mind.12 This perception may be explained in part by a possible correlation between increased concentra- tion and decreased emotional reactivity, accounting for a more efficient use of energy both during meditation and in everyday life.15,16 Significant progress has been made recently in investi- gating the effects of consistent concentrative and diffuse meditation practice on systems for sensory processing and integration, revealing two temporally distinct neural modes of self-reference: (1) extended self-reference linking subjec- tive experience across time (narrative focus, also known as the default mode) and (2) momentary self-reference centered on the present moment (experiential focus).17 Narrative focus is characterized by relating to the world through a construction of narratives that constitute the I or self, distinguishing the individual self from the selves of others and objects, whereas experiential focus refers to the neuro- psychological experience of the present moment, without ruminative, ego-centered interpretation of sense objects.17 In the context of meditation, it is useful to think of expe- riential focus as the experience of life through the “internal, present-centered, objective mental observer.” To observe means to watch impartially as an outsider. The internal ob- server is that which enables us to experience a sensation without identifying with it, or figuratively “stand next to” a sensation and observe it objectively. Through meditation, one becomes aware of this observer and develops one’s ability to make a continuous and objective observation of one’s sensate reality. Therefore, practicing experiential focus may be described as practicing present-centered awareness by “continually disengaging attentional processes from self- referential elaboration toward sense objects.”17,18 MINDFULNESS-BASED MEDITATION Mindfulness-based therapeutic techniques have been exten- sively incorporated into the psychiatric setting as evidence- based therapeutic programs, such as mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR),9 dialectical behavioral therapy,19 acceptance and commitment therapy,20 relapse-prevention therapy,21 and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy,22 and have been shown to be effective in treating depression,23 anxiety,24 drug and alcohol addiction,7,25 and various person- ality disorders.7,19,26,27 In the clinical setting, these programs are popular interventions for hypertension, stress, and cardio- vascular illness, and in slowing and perhaps stopping or re- versing neurodegeneration and cognitive decline.7,27 Mindfulness may be conceptualized as bringing one’s complete attention to one’s present experience through the nonjudgmental observation of the ongoing stream of all per- ceivable internal and external stimuli as they arise from mo- ment to moment.28 Thus, mindfulness is remembering to pay attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the pres- ent moment, and nonjudgmentally.28 Dakwar and Levin7 have compiled several psychological mechanisms through which mindfulness may aid in psychiatric symptom reduc- tion and behavioral change. It has been shown that an accepting and nonjudgmental mental outlook toward dis- tressing phenomena can reduce associated stress.15 For ex- ample, patients with borderline personality disorder who have learned to observe distressing thoughts and feelings without dissociation show diminished fear response and cessation of avoidance behaviors previously triggered by those stimuli.19 This effect may be attributed to a kind of habituation/desensitization therapy, where a repetitious, but intentionally objective, cognitive exposure toward the distressing stimulus gradually reduces its associated nega- tive significance.7 Training in mindfulness may also assist in recognizing maladaptive coping skills and aid in implementing better Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 207 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 4. ones.7 A nonjudgmental attitude toward phenomena fos- ters cognitive flexibility and reappraisal, and therefore may lead to changes in thought patterns or attitudes about one’s thoughts.1,7 The unattached or neutral appraisal of one’s thoughts and emotions may reduce automatic reactivity to- ward them, thereby enabling those who have learned mind- fulness to shift from maladaptive thought patterns and behaviors to ones that are more adaptive.29 Increased un- attached awareness of behavioral, cognitive, and emo- tional stimuli may improve stress tolerance, the ability to recognize behavioral consequences, and the ability to set healthy motivations into action.4,7,19,30 Mindfulness-based acceptance may also aid in reducing dissociative adaptations toward feelings or thoughts, as seen in borderline personal- ity disoroder.7 Developing one’s faculty of unattached obser- vation and acceptance of distressing thoughts or feelings may reduce the likelihood of maladaptive behavior patterns. For example, borderline patients who practice accepting their chronic feelings of emptiness may engage less fre- quently in self-destructive and self-injurious behaviors that were previously used to distract from those feelings.7,19 As with Buddhist meditation, participants undergoing mindfulness-based therapy sit in a calm, but vigilant, posi- tion and observe their object of attention (e.g., breath, body parts, thoughts) without judgment or rumination and as continuously as possible. However, a mindfulness-based ap- proach to therapy does not necessarily require formal medi- tation in the strict sense.7 The goal of mindfulness-based treatment is to promote mindfulness in the patients accord- ing to their own capacities. For instance, a patient with de- velopmental disabilities, brain damage, severe depression or mania, anxiety, or psychosis may be unable to practice formal sitting meditation. Often, patients may refuse to practice because they do not clearly understand its purpose. Therefore, some strategies offer mindfulness-promoting cognitive exercises that may seem more engaging, less stress- ful, or more practical to some individuals. Such strategies in- clude group discussions, lectures, worksheets, readings, or cognitive exercises such as guided imagery or metaphor to aid in the development of mindfulness-based insights.7 For example, participants may be asked to visualize their thoughts as debris floating over a river, watching their thoughts come and go, giving them as much importance as they would a piece of passing driftwood.31 Participants are also encouraged to develop an unattached relationship to phenomena through intellectual concepts such as the transient nature of sensations and thoughts, helping them to understand that even the most difficult or agitating thoughts and feelings are bound to change. This understand- ing fosters the ability to experience painful thoughts or feelings more presently and calmly, allowing space for con- templation, reevaluation, and the ability to allow them to pass on without further elaboration.7 Patients are also en- couraged to practice mindfulness at home and in everyday life, such as while walking, eating, or talking. If the patient is in a state of severe psychological turmoil, such as psycho- sis or suicidal depression, then providing a quiet, peaceful environment with positive human influence, healthy food, and necessary medications may be the first step in helping them to establish a balanced mind capable of developing the foundations of mindfulness practice. Nevertheless, these modified treatments consider the practice of meditation techniques to be central to their therapeutic efficacy.7,9,10 Mindfulness-based meditation works under the premise that all individuals—health care providers and patients alike—can benefit from objective self-observation. It has been shown that health care professionals who regularly practice mindfulness meditation, particularly those in the psychiatric setting, are better equipped to treat patients. For example, a recent study found that participation in an eight- week mindfulness-based stress reduction program was as- sociated with improved mental health in graduate-level psychology students.32 When compared to a control group, students in the MBSR program reported significant pre/ post declines in perceived stress, negative affect, state and trait anxiety, and rumination, and a significant increase in positive affect and self-compassion. Compassion may be described as a nonpossessive state of loving that is moti- vated by a person’s own sense of well-being with regard to his or her interactions and connectedness with others.33 Developing self-compassion is particularly relevant to the fields of counseling and therapy. It has been purported that in order to have compassion toward one’s client, which is an essential aspect of conducting effective therapy,32–34 a therapist must first have compassion toward him- or herself. Research shows that counselors and therapists lacking in self- compassion are more critical and controlling toward them- selves, and are also more critical and controlling toward their patients, resulting in poorer patient outcomes.32,35 NEUROBIOLOGICAL STUDIES OF MEDIATION Many studies have shown that meditation and meditation- derived therapeutic approaches improve overall physical and mental health.4,10,36 In this section, we present findings from imaging, electroencephalography (EEG), and physiological studies that relate to biological effects observed with meditation. Brain Imaging Intrinsic connectivity networks are large-scale brain net- works that may function either in an active (engaged) or passive (resting) state, suggesting that the human brain is organized into distinct functional networks.37 Significant progress has been made in investigating the effect of consistent meditation practice on systems for sensory processing and inte- gration, revealing two temporally distinct neural modes of self-reference: (1) momentary self-reference centered on the present moment (experiential focus), neurologically corre- lated with the central executive network, and (2) extended C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 208 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 5. self-reference linking subjective experience across time (nar- rative focus), neurologically correlated with the default mode network.17,37,38 The default mode network comprises an integrated sys- tem for autobiographical, self-centered, and social cognitive functions characterized by ruminative, often subconscious, self-referential narrative thought linking subjective experi- ence across time—which generates one’s concept of self or identity.39 The default mode network concept emerged from a body of evidence demonstrating a consistent pattern of de- activation across a distributed network of brain regions dur- ing focused mental tasks; these regions become activated in the absence of such focused mental tasks.39 The brain re- gions include the posterior cingulate cortex (involving au- tobiographical memory and self-referential processes), the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) (involving social- cognitive processes related to self and others), the medial temporal lobe (involved in episodic memory), and the angu- lar gyrus (involved in semantic processing).37 Conversely, the central executive network is activated (and default mode network deactivated) during focused mental tasks and is an- chored in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the posterior parietal cortex.37 There is a third intrinsic con- nectivity network, known as the salience network, acting as a switch or “toggle” between the activation/deactivation of the central executive network and default mode network, depending on the degree of mental engagement. The salience network is anchored in the anterior insula (AI) and the ante- rior cingulate cortex (ACC).37 Abnormal default mode network activity has been impli- cated in a number of psychiatric diseases, including schizo- phrenia,40 autism,41,42 epilepsy,43 depression44 and anxiety disorders,45–47 bipolar disorder,44 attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),44 and Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases.44 In many of these cases, particularly in schizo- phrenia,40 ADHD,44 bipolar disorder,44 and depression/ anxiety disorders,45–47 an inability to “switch off” the hy- peractive default mode network through focusing the mind appears to be a major contributor to the disease.44 A recent study conducted by Whitfield-Gabrieli and colleagues40 used fMRI to examine potential alterations in default mode net- work activity in subjects with schizophrenia. While pointing out that the default mode network is activated during in- ternal, self-referential thought, the authors noted that the normal boundary between internal thought and external perceptions might be blurred by hyperactivity of the default network.40 This suggestion is consistent with psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia whereby patients have an exag- gerated sense of self-reference in the world, as evidenced by paranoia, and a blurring of internal thoughts and external perception, which may give rise to hallucinations.40 In line with these observations, results indicate that the default mode network is hyperactive and hyperconnected in people with schizophrenia and that the strength of connectivity and the defect in appropriate deactivation of the default mode network are correlated with more prominent psychotic symptoms.40 The study also found a significant increase in hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default mode network in immediate family members of schizophrenic individuals as compared to individuals from families with no occurrence of schizophrenia. Another recent fMRI study analyzed working-memory, task-induced modulation of default mode network activity in subjects with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) com- pared to healthy controls.47 The control group showed sig- nificantly stronger connectivity in areas implicated in the salience and executive networks, such as the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right inferior parietal lobule. How- ever, the PTSD group showed stronger connectivity in areas implicated in the default mode network, such as stronger connectivity between the posterior cingulate cortex and the right superior frontal gyrus, and between the medial pre- frontal cortex (MPFC) and the left parahippocampal gyrus. Subjects with PTSD showed a significantly impaired abil- ity to switch from an idling state into a task-oriented state when compared to healthy controls, as implied by hypercon- nectivity in the default mode network during the working- memory task in the PTSD group.47 The different patterns of connectivity suggest significant group differences in the ability to disengage from the default mode network and to engage the central executive network during tasks that require focused attention.47 The psychological characteristics associated with default mode network activation are commonly known distrac- tions in the meditation milieu, such as daydreaming, mind wandering, and projections into the past or future, which has led researchers to investigate the effects of meditation on default mode network activity.52 An fMRI study com- pared differential brain-activation patterns during two dis- tinct modes of perception: narrative focus correlated with default mode network activation, and experiential focus correlated with the central executive network activation, in both novice participants and participants who had at- tended an eight-week course in mindfulness meditation.17 In novices, experiential focus yielded focal reductions in self- referential cortical midline regions associated with narrative focus. In trained participants, experiential focus resulted in more marked and pervasive reductions in the MPFC, in- cluding the rostral subregions of the dorsal MPFC and ven- tral MPFC, suggesting that present-centered experience may rely on suppression of MPFC representations supporting narrative focus. Trained participants also showed marked reductions in the MPFC and increased recruitment of a right lateralized network, comprising the right lateral PFC and viscerosomatic areas such as the insula, secondary so- matosensory cortex, and inferior parietal lobule. Moreover, right insular functional connectivity analysis revealed a strong coupling between the right insula and the VMPFC in novices that was uncoupled in the mindfulness group, sug- gesting viscerosomatic signals are by default associated with Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 209 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 6. activation in the VMPFC.17 This decoupling in the trained mindfulness group was replaced by an increased coupling of the right insula with the DLPFC during formal meditation, which may reflect a neuronal reorganization that supports a continuous, present-centered attentional stance.17 In another fMRI study, brain activity patterns observed during concentrative and diffuse meditation were contrasted in an experiment between expert Buddhist monks and lay novices with an integrated concentrative/diffuse paradigm, where involvement of differential brain activations was an- alyzed between groups during concentrative and diffuse meditation with reference to rest.38 No particular interest was given to identifying previously theorized large-scale brain networks, such as the aforementioned intrinsic con- nectivity network. Striking differences between activity pat- terns were found between the groups. Activity patterns during diffuse meditation resembled the ordinary brain- resting state in monks, but not in controls, suggesting that monks also maintain an open-monitoring attentional stance during nonformal meditation conditions (i.e., in everyday life).38 There may be a meditation-related functional reor- ganization of brain activity patterns in the prefrontal cortex and insula in expert practitioners, reflected by a substan- tially increased reliance on the central executive network and decreased reliance on the default mode network during nonformal meditation conditions.38 Neuronal populations in regions typically associated with self-referential processing during everyday life may have been reallocated to support areas that are involved in metacognition of phenomenal ex- perience; that is, objective, present-centered awareness.13 Several crucial aspects of Buddhist meditation practice are relevant here—all of which relate to transcending concep- tions of a separate self or ego—such as the cultivation of mental states of unconditional love, compassion, and kind- ness, or remaining continually aware of impermanence, or the arising and passing of particular sensational phenomena from moment to moment.8,33,38,48,49 Moreover, this finding is consistent with the goal of meditation, which is to disin- tegrate the experiential psychological distinction between meditation and normative functioning over time.48,49 The study also found that the ACC and DLPFC play an- tagonistic roles in the executive control of attention setting in monks during meditation.38 A negative correlation be- tween ACC activation and DLPFC deactivation during rest was found, possibly suggesting increased reliance upon the salience network when distraction is more likely, such as during resting state.38 This may be a functional reflection of increased reliance on the ACC/salience network and de- creased reliance on the DLPFC/central executive network during rest, and a decreased reliance on the ACC/salience network and increased reliance on the DLPFC/central exec- utive network during concentrative meditation. Note that concentrative meditation is used to establish (or reestablish) concentration when concentration is weak. Predominant ACC activation during rest, as part of the salience network, may be a neurological reflection of continually disengaging from distractors caused by default mode network activa- tion and reengaging with the object of focus during focused mental processing (i.e., toggling between the default mode network and central executive network). However, the DLPFC, as part of the central executive network, is pre- dominantly activated during continual focused attention, which is confirmed by predominant DLPFC activation dur- ing formal concentrative meditation. It would be interest- ing to see if there is a gradual reduction in ACC/salience and default mode network activation with a concurrent, corresponding increase in DLPFC/central executive network activation during a concentrative meditation session in ex- pert participants—and, if so, how this result correlates with the reported strength of concentration during the session. The findings also address the question of whether medita- tive states are associated with a transient deactivation50,51 or activation11,13 of executive brain areas. The findings clearly show that executive areas are activated during meditation.38 Another recent study examined meditation-induced cor- tical gyrification to shed light on anatomical correlates of meditation and meditation-related neuroplasticity through- out the lifespan.52 Gyrification refers to the pattern and degree of cortical folding on the surface of the brain. Par- ticipants consisted of a large sample (n = 100) of long-term insight meditators and age- and sex-matched controls. The degree of cortical gyrification was established by calculat- ing mean curvature with a 3-D version of a well-known gyrification magnetic resonance imaging index across thou- sands of cortices on individual cortical surface models. When applying more liberal significance thresholds (p < 0.05), gyrification was larger in meditators as well as in controls overall.52 However, meditators showed larger gyrification in some of the areas where prior analyses revealed thicker gray matter cortices in meditators as compared to nonmedi- tators.52,53 Regions included the anterior insula,53 left ante- rior temporal gyrus,54,55 left central sulcus and vicinity,52,56 and right parietal operculum, which houses the secondary somatosensory cortex.52,56 Stricter significance thresholds (p < 0.01) revealed five distinct regions where cortical gyr- ification was exclusively larger in meditators: the left pre- central gyrus, left and right anterior insula, right fusiform gyrus, and left cuneus, showing a global maximum in the right AI.52 As previously mentioned, the AI has been shown to play a crucial role in switching between the default mode network and central executive network.37 The current study suggests a correlation between AI gyral complexity and the number of years spent practicing meditation.52 Although the findings of this study should be interpreted with caution since it did not correct for multiple comparisons, previ- ous studies have likewise shown structural differences be- tween meditators and nonmeditators within the AI.53–55,57 Moreover, functional analyses have shown activation of the insula during meditation,52 supporting the notion that C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 210 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 7. the salience network is acting to shift the mind from a state of wandering to a state of present-centered awareness during meditation. The AI has also been described as a “hub” for auto- nomic, affective, and cognitive integration, as it has been shown to integrate a wide array of stimuli, including cogni- tive, socio-emotional, olfactory-gustatory, interoceptive sen- sation, and pain processing.52,58–61 Meditators learn to master their faculties of introspective awareness, emotional control, and self-regulation, as well as techniques oriented toward enhancing wholesome and unconditional mental states.52 Therefore, the AI may be a contributing neural correlate of a fundamental, all-inclusive awareness that acts to shift the mind from a state of self-referential perception of sensation (i.e., identification with sensation) to a state of all- inclusive, present-centered awareness with regard to any possible sensation. This category includes all body sensa- tion, thoughts, beliefs, emotional states—everything that can be observed, which is literally everything but awareness itself. Therefore, the salience network may act as the bridge between the subconscious reactive experience of being iden- tified with sensation, associated with default mode network activation, and an all-inclusive, “transcendent” state of ob- jective, present-centered awareness of sensate experience, contributing to a sense of “oneness” or interconnectedness that is associated with central executive network activation. Electroencephalography Studies EEG coherence is characterized by the synchronous firing of neural networks at a particular frequency. It is theorized that the amplitude of a brain wave is positively correlated with the degree of precision with which cells oscillate and the size of the neural population through which the wave is occurring.62,63 It has been found that increases in EEG coherence correlate with increased brain processing capacity in areas such as attention, working memory, learning, and conscious perception.63–66 Synchronization of oscillatory neu- ral discharges, particularly of the gamma-band frequency (25–70 Hz), appears to play a key role in developing the ability to integrate distributed neural processes into one over- arching network with highly ordered cognitive and affective functions, suggesting that gamma synchrony may also play a crucial role in neuroplasticity.63 In an EEG study, long-term Buddhist meditators were found to possess the ability to induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during meditation, where robust gamma-band oscillations and long-range phase synchrony were found during a nonreferential compassion meditation state.63 The amplitude of activity in some of these practitioners was found to be the highest reported outside of pathological contexts.63,67 This finding suggests that massive distributed neural assemblies were oscillating with high temporal preci- sion during meditation.63 In expert meditators, the transition from basal cognition to the meditation state typically took 5–15 seconds. Time to develop deep meditative states characterized by neural synchronization at high amplitude frequencies was found to be proportional to the size of the neural assembly being stimulated. The goal of medita- tion is to transform normative functioning into a consistent meditative state by disintegrating the distinction between meditation and normative functioning over time—which is reported to occur naturally with consistent practice.8,48,49 In support of this claim, Lutz and colleagues63 found differ- ences in basal gamma activity between novice and expert meditators before meditation, with expert meditators show- ing more gamma activity during resting state. Further testing found that the duration of time spent meditating over the lifespan, and not age, predicted differences in gamma activ- ity during baseline cognitive functioning.63 A recent neurophysiology study investigated the effects of attentional training on the global precedence effect (i.e., the faster detection of targets on a global, rather than on a local, level) in eight highly trained Buddhist monks and nuns and in eight age- and education-matched controls with no previous meditation experience.68 Analysis of reaction times showed a significantly reduced global precedence ef- fect in meditators but not in controls, implying that the meditators had a unique ability to willingly, quickly, and ef- fectively disengage their attention from the dominant global level.68 Analysis of event-related potentials revealed that, due to an enhanced processing of target-level information, the meditators had an acute ability to select the respective target level in which the target number was embedded.68 Moreover, meditators selected target-level information ear- lier in the processing sequence than controls. These results suggest that with mental training, meditators are able to in- crease the speed and accuracy with which they allocate atten- tional resources, thereby increasing the depth and breadth of information processing while reducing response latency to stimuli.68 As described by the methodology involved in concentra- tive and diffuse meditation, these findings support the no- tion that concentrative and diffuse meditation practices result in an increased capacity to process sensory informa- tion by training the practitioner to not “get stuck” on spe- cific stimuli, possibly contributing to the ability to notice fine details more quickly and accurately. To reiterate, con- centrative meditators center their attention on a specific ob- ject of focus for extended periods of time, and when their attention is diverted from the object of focus, they learn to disengage quickly from the distractor and to reengage with the object of focus until access concentration is estab- lished. A similar process may be used to establish access con- centration during diffuse meditation, though instead of maintaining attentional focus on a specific object, the medi- tator practices continually disengaging from sense objects throughout the body/mind as quickly as possible until a con- tinual awareness of the ongoing stream of experience is established. The above results also support the traditional notion that meditation “concentrates” the mind—that is, Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 211 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 8. makes the mind progressively more acutely aware of the details of momentary experience. Physiological Studies It has been known that regular meditation practice can re- duce psychological stress and enhance perceived well- being.8,9,48,49,69 Some attention has been given to the physiological correlates of meditation-derived health bene- fits as a means to understanding the mechanistic links be- tween psychological functioning and physical health. An early physiology study compared the effect of Buddhist meditation on several physiological markers (serum corti- sol, total protein levels, blood pressure, pulse rate, lung vol- ume, and reaction time) between 52 male meditators and 30 age- and sex-matched nonmeditators.4 Measurements of these markers were taken after three and six weeks of practice. At three weeks, results showed significant reduc- tions in serum cortisol level, blood pressure, pulse rate, and reaction time in meditators compared to controls.4 Vi- tal lung capacity, tidal volume, and maximal voluntary ventilation were significantly lower following each medita- tion session than before, indicating a lower need for oxy- gen as a result of meditation.4 In addition, the serum total protein level significantly increased by the six-week time point. Meditators also showed a 23% reduction in reaction time compared to measurements taken before practice, sug- gesting that a high degree of present-centered attentional acuity was developed during practice.4 The control group showed a 7% reduction in reaction time at six weeks, most likely due to task repetition, indicating that meditation practice was not entirely responsible for the reduced reac- tion time in the experimental group. These results are con- sistent with the current conception of meditation as being associated with a concurrent state of heightened vigilant awareness and reduced metabolic activity that elicits im- proved physical health, psychological balance, and emo- tional stability.3,6,15,49 A recent study investigated whether meditation practice is associated with telomerase activity and whether this asso- ciation is at least partly explained by changes in two major contributors to the experience of stress—“perceived control” and “neuroticism.”70 Telomeres are terminal DNA protein complexes consisting of repetitive nucleotide sequences that are located at the ends of chromosomes.71 They are known to ensure genomic stability during cellular replication by pro- tecting the ends of chromosomes from deterioration and from fusing with other chromosomes. However, telomeres shorten under conditions of oxidative stress, as well as with each cell-division cycle. Telomerase is a ribonucleoprotein that counteracts this process by adding DNA to the 3’ end in the telomere regions.71 As cells divide during the aging pro- cess, telomere length steadily decreases and hence is an in- dicator of the biological age of a cell.70,72 Cell division can no longer occur below a critical telomere length, result- ing in a state of senescence.71,73 Therefore, telomere length acts as an effective “psychobiomarker” in predicting physi- cal health and longevity by linking stress and disease.70,74–76 Recent longitudinal studies indicate that telomere length in peripheral blood mononuclear cells can increase over time, suggesting that there may be determinable mechanisms—and therefore possible interventions—that regulate the rate of telomere length alteration.70,77–79 Greater perceived stress, negative affect, and stress-related cardiovascular risk are factors associated with lower telomerase activity that is re- sponsible for protecting against telomere shortening.70,80,81 Individuals who respond with suppressed vagal tone when presented with an acute stressor and who show correspond- ingly increased measures of negative affect and vulnerability to psychological stressors show reduced telomerase activ- ity.70,82,83 Therefore, telomerase activity may play a key role in mediating the relation between psychological stress and disease.70 This controlled, longitudinal study by Jacobs and colleagues70 investigated the effects of a three-month meditation retreat on telomerase activity and on two ma- jor contributors to the experience of stress—perceived con- trol (associated with decreased stress) and neuroticism (associated with increased subjective stress). Perceived con- trol is a marker of stress resilience and refers to a feeling of adequate control that has been associated with reduced psychological stress and more adaptive responses to stress- ful events.70,84–89 For example, an internal locus of control refers to the extent to which individuals believe they can control their external circumstance and has been shown to mediate responses to stressful events by improving cop- ing strategies90,91 and reducing anxiety.70,92 High trait neg- ative affectivity, or neuroticism, refers to chronic feelings of tension, anxiety, lability, or insecurity, and typically ampli- fies the stress response to acute stressors, causing “greater stress vulnerability.”70,93–96 The study by Jacobs and colleagues70 also examined the causal relationship between meaning-finding and affect, with specific regard to how these factors are influenced by meditation. Longitudinal studies have shown that the abil- ity to integrate stressful events into an encompassing sense of meaning or purpose leads to a change in affect.70,97 In the context of meditation, Fredrickson and colleagues98 showed that the amount of time per week spent in “loving- kindness,” or compassion, meditation over a two-month period predicted a cumulative increase in positive affect, contributing to an increase in life satisfaction and reduced depressive symptoms as mediated by one’s degree of per- ceived purpose in life and mindfulness. Jacobs and collea- gues70 found that telomerase activity was significantly greater in retreat participants than in controls at the end of the retreat (p < 0.05). Moreover, increases in perceived control, decreases in neuroticism, and increases in both mindfulness and perceived purpose in life were greater in the retreat group (p < 0.01).70 The data suggest that increases in perceived control and decreases in negative af- fectivity, as influenced by meditation practice, contributed C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 212 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 9. to an increase in telomerase activity. If so, consistent medita- tion could potentially result in increases in telomere length and immune cell longevity throughout the lifespan.70 Hence, the practice of meditation seems to affect cellular processes that are influenced by stress and linked to disease. Another recent study examined the effect of compassion meditation, a form of emotional diffuse meditation, on in- nate immune, neuroendocrine, and behavioral responses to psychological stress, and evaluated the degree to which en- gagement in meditation practice influenced stress reactiv- ity.99 Sixty-one healthy adults were randomized to either six weeks of training in compassion meditation (n = 33) or participation in a health discussion control group (n = 28). These were followed by exposure to a standardized laboratory stressor (Trier Social Stress Test (TSST)) that consists of public speaking followed by mental arithmetic. Physiologic and behavioral responses to the TSST were de- termined by repeated assessments of plasma concentrations of interleukin-6 (IL-6) and cortisol, and by total distress scores on the Profile of Mood States (POMS). Initially, no differences between groups were found on TSST response scores for IL-6, cortisol, or POMS scores. However, in- creased meditation practice as measured by mean number of practice sessions per week was strongly correlated with decreased TSST-induced IL-6 (p = 0.008) and POMS scores (p = 0.014). Because no differences based on group assign- ment were found (meditation versus controls), further anal- ysis was done to compare individuals who spent more time meditating (above the median, high practice) to individuals who spent less time meditating (below the median, low practice) and controls (no practice). High-practice indivi- duals (n = 17) exhibited significantly lower TSST-induced IL-6 and POMS scores compared to low-practice indivi- duals and controls (p = 0.005). These data suggest that en- gagement in compassion meditation may progressively reduce stress-induced immune and behavioral responses relative to one’s degree of experience with meditation.99 Yet another study examined the effects of mindfulness on HPA activity and sleep by comparing morning cortisol samples and also sleep duration and quality between long- and short-term meditators.100 This study was conducted with the specific aim of clarifying existing contradictory conclusions from numerous previous studies on the effects of mindfulness on health by investigating the relationship between traditional Buddhist meditation, mindfulness-based stress reduction, HPA activity, and sleep among long-term practitioners of meditation (Buddhist monks and nuns, and mindfulness teachers) compared to those with no prior med- itation experience (novices). Long-term meditators showed decreased morning cortisol levels that corresponded with length of practice over the lifespan (p < 0.03). Similarly, novices who took an eight-week introductory MBSR course showed reduced morning cortisol levels (p = 0.04) and sub- stantially improved sleep (p = 0.008) and self-attribution of mindfulness (p = 0.001). However, cortisol levels did not change between the beginning and end of individual MBSR sessions. These results support the view that meditation and meditation-derived therapeutic techniques such as MBSR have beneficial effects on biomarkers of stress regulation, such as cortical secretion and quality of sleep, and also seem to improve one’s ability to remain mindful. 35 These results provide support for MBSR as a nonpharmacological inter- vention alongside standard therapies, as well as in nonclinical settings to foster well-being. Moreover, the empirical physio- logical data summarized from this study support the validity of the self-attributed data previously discussed in this section. For example, increased perceived control, reduced negative affectivity, and increased life satisfaction scores would be expected in individuals with improved biomarkers of stress regulation. DISCUSSION Western medical traditions have progressed by way of hy- pothetical deduction, relying heavily upon pharmaceuticals and technology-based interventions to treat or suppress dis- eases caused by pathophysiologic processes. While these have had a substantial impact in improving human health globally, the effectiveness of Western medicine remains limited, espe- cially when treating chronic diseases and mental illness in which stress and the environment play major roles.101 The study and integration of Eastern healing techniques may bring about a complementary approach that will help develop a more comprehensive and holistic attitude toward patient care. Meditation is an ancient Eastern technique that has re- cently been accepted as a form of clinical intervention in the West. The study and practice of Buddhist meditation, along with the increasing use of Buddhist-derived, mindfulness- based therapeutic modalities in the clinical setting, have piqued scientific interest. Here, we have introduced the ba- sic concepts of concentrative, diffuse, and insight medita- tion, as well as some of the relevant clinical applications and scientific studies that suggest significant physiological and neurological correlates of these meditative practices. Neuroscientists have made strides in elucidating some of the basic biological mechanisms underlying the benefits of meditation. Our understanding, however, is rudimentary at this stage. Compelling scientific evidence supports the role of med- itation in promoting health in general20,21,53,70 and in treat- ing numerous clinical and psychiatric disorders.20–30,36,44,47 Buddhist meditation induces what one may consider a low- entropy state of equanimous, vigilant awareness,3 bringing the body to a “hypometabolic state of parasympathetic dom- inance” that serves to rejuvenate the organism’s capacity for resilience and adaptability.7 Long-term Buddhist medita- tion has been correlated with reduced stress response and lower cortisol levels overall,4,36,70 possibly contributing to reduced chronic stress and a restored healthy response to acute stress,4 longevity,70 reduced stress-induced craving, and increased craving tolerance.7 Developed meditators Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 213 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 10. also have the ability to access “highly reinforced” (i.e., deep) meditative states,5,6,16,38,48 which may contribute to cognitive resilience and adaptation in the face of even highly agitating stressors.7 Buddhist meditation has been found to increase cerebral plasticity1,6,36,38 and cerebral blood flow and thickening in brain regions associated with the mental exercises practiced by concentrative and diffuse meditation,15–17,36,38,50–56 suggesting a role in potentially preserving and enhancing brain regions affected by medita- tion.7 This notion is supported by increased proficiency in several cognitive skills associated with consistent concentra- tive and diffuse meditation, such as the highly developed abil- ity to disengage from distractors during concentration and the ability to observe one’s internal states in a non-evaluative way.7–9,33,48,49 Buddhist meditation has been found to increase EEG co- herence through the synchronization of neural discharges, par- ticularly in the gamma-band frequency (25–70 Hz).1,5,6,11,37 It is theorized that a brain wave’s amplitude is positively correlated with the degree of precision with which cells os- cillate and with the size of the neural population through which the wave is occurring.62,63 Increased EEG coherence is correlated with increased brain-processing capacity in areas such as attention, working memory, learning, and conscious perception.63–66 Synchronization of oscillatory neural discharges, particularly of the gamma-band fre- quency, appears to play a key role in developing the ability to integrate distributed neural processes into one overarch- ing network with highly ordered cognitive and affective functions, suggesting that gamma synchrony may also play a crucial role in meditation-induced neuroplasticity.63,67 Recent findings have shown that a gradual, meditation- induced, functional reorganization of brain activity pat- terns occurs between the default mode network, salience network, and central executive network; reliance upon the central executive network and salience network seems to increase, and reliance upon the default mode network seems to decrease.37,38 Abnormal activity in this intrinsic connec- tivity network has been implicated in a number of psychiat- ric diseases (such as ADHD, bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, depression and anxiety disorders, and schizophrenia),37,38,44 and an inability to “switch off” the hyperactive default mode network through focusing the mind appears to be a major contributor to the disease pro- cess,44 suggesting that meditation may aid in reducing symp- toms in these disorders. Future research should consider the state of the anterior insula in individuals with these disor- ders. The AI plays a critical role for the initiation of network switching by the salience network,37 suggesting that AI in- tegrity or connectivity may be compromised in individuals with these disorders. In light of meditation’s effect on this intrinsic connectiv- ity network, meditation may be contraindicated in cases in which there are existing deficits in default mode network function. For example, autistic patients show reduced and abnormal default mode network activation overall, which may be neurologically related to difficulty in structuring so- cial relationships, and they also show an increased reliance on the central executive network,37,41,42 suggesting that meditation may act to solidify or exacerbate existing symp- toms. Therefore, more research is needed to determine how meditation affects large-scale brain networks associated with psychopathology in order to ensure the safe and effec- tive application of meditation in clinical and psychiatric settings. The skills developed through meditation practice have significant implications for the treatment of various psychi- atric disorders. The neural correlates of these skills may be partially explained by mechanisms involved in neuroplasti- city over the lifespan. These findings also support the no- tion that concentrative and diffuse meditation practices aid in behavior modification, in attenuating stress-related illness, and in slowing, and perhaps stopping and rehabili- tating, neurodegeneration and cognitive decline.7 Future meditation research should aid in developing a clearer un- derstanding of how large-scale brain network organization in the human brain produces cognition. Recent evidence suggests that cognitive function depends on a dynamic in- teraction of dissociable large-scale brain networks, such as recently discovered intrinsic connectivity networks.1,37–39 Determining dissociable large-scale brain networks and their cognitive and behavioral correlates in healthy individ- uals would aid in studying psychiatric diseases by providing a reference point for comparison. Developed meditators may provide researchers with a more objective point of ref- erence for healthy brain function than may be found in the general population. Research in the field of neurometacognition has already made progress in understanding the neural correlates of cognition and complex behavior.1,2,6,13,16,34–38 Continued research in this field may eventually give rise to a coherent theoretical framework for the neurobiology of conscious- ness. Meditation research can provide a framework for study that will aid in correlating electrophysiological record- ings with simultaneously activated brain areas and their as- sociated cognitive and behavioral analogues. Research has suggested that long-range phase synchrony may serve as a dynamic mechanism underlying functional interactions in large-scale brain networks.37 Large-scale functional net- works (such as the intrinsic connectivity network) and their cognitive and behavioral correlates may be able to be iden- tified and differentiated through the comparison of concen- trative and diffuse meditation conditions using respectively different fields of experiential awareness (e.g., concentra- tive meditation on breath sensations versus an external vi- sual point, and diffuse meditation on body versus thought/ emotion versus external sensory fields), while simulta- neously recording brain-imaging activity patterns and concurrent electrophysiology. Participants with expert at- tentional control are able to direct their attention to C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 214 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 11. differential fields of awareness at will and continuously, providing a well-defined and highly controlled activation of specific attentional fields and associated functional net- work activity, without the residual activation that may be produced in untrained populations by the lack attentional control and by “cortical noise.”16,37,38 Therefore, expert meditation practitioners may prove to be a valuable popu- lation for investigating a wide variety of phenomena that may aid in defining neural correlates of cognition and also the specific altered states of consciousness, including the neural correlates of attentional stance; mechanisms underly- ing dynamic engagement and disengagement of large-scale networks; how differential large-scale networks cooperate, compete, or coordinate their activity; how knowledge is neurologically constructed; how large-scale network imag- ing correlates with synchronized ocillatory neural activity; and the neural-physiological correlates of specific meditation- induced states of consciousness.37 Buddhist practitioners and literature provide meditation instructions within a comprehensive theoretical framework of the states of con- sciousness that may be experienced through meditation, which may prove useful in guiding and expediting the pro- cess of meditation itself.33,48,49,102–104 Moreover, this pro- cess may provide a strong and invaluable unification of two diametrically opposed methodologies used for the scientific investigation of consciousness—namely, the intel- lectual and deductive approach taken by Western scien- tists and the experiential and inductive approach taken by Eastern spiritual practitioners. The complex cognitive methodology involved in medita- tion presents researchers with unique challenges when de- termining appropriate controls for the specific variables under investigation. Many experiments have failed to con- trol for the type of meditation practice under investigation, making it difficult or impossible to compare results. As pre- viously mentioned, meditation styles can differ significantly in their methodologies and intended results because each style is explicitly designed to train specific cognitive facul- ties. Moreover, the neurological correlates of specific prac- tices differ significantly between populations, such as the differences one may find between monks, lay persons, adults, children, and individuals with psychopathology. Therefore, researchers must be specific about the style of meditation practice and the population under investigation, or else risk causing experimental confounds that produce results with limited or no potential for broader scientific comparison or application. Similarly, while mindfulness-based treat- ments in the clinical setting have been found to be effective, the majority of mindfulness-based treatments rely on other therapeutic modalities in cooperation with meditation, such as cognitive therapy, making it difficult to determine the efficacy of mindfulness practice itself during mindfulness- based treatment. In order to make results derived from mindfulness-based practitioners more comparable to other populations, it may be helpful to determine the effects of mindfulness practice itself through, for example, longitudi- nal studies on individuals who have maintained their prac- tice after the therapy program has been completed. In a similar vein, longitudinal studies on Buddhist meditation are lacking overall and would be of great use in determining neural development and reorganization with meditation ex- perience and in verifying behavioral and experiential modifi- cations over time in relation to constant information (e.g., Buddhist texts). Longitudinal studies would also aid in separating out the specific neurological/behavioral factors affected by meditation itself from those found across indi- viduals who are already likely to practice meditation. Research on how meditation affects neurobiological pathways governing the stress response is lacking overall. Numerous studies have shown, however, that meditation has a normalizing effect on psychobiomarkers of stress reg- ulation and that it produces overall improvements in self- attributed mood scores.8,9,48,49,69 Moreover, the practice of meditation has been shown to positively affect cellular processes that are affected by stress and linked to disease, as is shown by increased telomerase activity with long- term meditation practice.70 The aspects of Buddhist theory that have been scientifi- cally investigated thus far have served mainstream psychol- ogy and cognitive neuroscience well, making Buddhist theory a potential avenue for in-depth and critical scientific investigation. Such investigations may provide a useful and arguably necessary broadening of perspective with regard to defining and treating psychopathology. It has been suggested that the mechanisms underlying psychiatric symp- tom reduction occur at a level of “personality reorganization,” because almost all of the commonly defined personality types—healthy, neurotic, borderline, psychotic, and so on—can be recognized in terms of personality develop- ment and organization over time.105 Therefore, in the context of meditation in the clinical setting, it may be use- ful to think of mental illness in terms of a symptomatic continuum, from most to least pathological, with the aim of progressively reducing symptoms through shifting the mind and body toward a state of natural balance.105 The Western scientific study of meditation has, in large part, been conducted with minimal reference to its religious and spiritual origins, and only a brief overview of the most fundamental and commonly practiced forms of Buddhist meditation has been presented here. However, Buddhism has been successful in maintaining the practice and teach- ing of insight meditation for over 2500 years, and an exten- sive and detailed literature is devoted to theoretical aspects of the stages of meditative experience, or the “process of insight.”102–105 Investigation of these texts reveals a theoretical science with corresponding technical medi- tation instructions that guide the practitioner, in a step- wise manner, through a process of coming out of suffering (or attachment to ego) by gradually deepening his or her knowledge of experiential reality. In short, this process Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 215 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 12. involves a gradual cumulative development of specific fa- culties of awareness that the individual uses to literally heal his or her own body. A detailed synopsis of this process and its implications is beyond the scope of this article. How- ever, a wide body of literature is available for readers who are interested in further exploration.102–105 Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the article. REFERENCES 1. Slagter H, Davidson R, Lutz A. Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plastic- ity. Front Hum Neurosci 2011;5:1–12. 2. Raffone A, Srinivasan N. The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cogn Pro- cess 2010;11:1–7. 3. Young JDE, Taylor E. Meditation as a voluntary hypo- metabolic state of biological estivation. News Physiol Sci 1998;13:149–53. 4. Sudsaung R, Chantanez V, Veluvan K. Effects of Buddhist meditation on serum cortisol and total protein levels, blood pressure, pulse rate, lung volume and reaction time. Physiol Behav 1990;50:543–8. 5. Lutz A, Brefczyski-Lewis J, Johnstone T, Davidson R. Regu- lation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion med- itation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS One 2008;3: e1897. 6. Lutz A, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Meditation and the neuro- science of consciousness: an introduction. In: Zelazo P, Moscovitch M, Thompson E, eds. Cambridge handbook of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2007:81–109. 7. Dakwar E, Levin FR. The emerging role of meditation in addressing psychiatric illness, with a focus on substance use disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2009;17:254–67. 8. Gunaratana H. Mindfulness in plain English. Somerville, MA: Wisdom, 2007. 9. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2003;10: 144–53. 10. Ludwig DS, Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness in medicine. JAMA 2008;300:1350–2. 11. Cahn BR, Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull 2006;132:180–211. 12. Goleman D. The Buddha on meditation and states of con- sciousness. In: Shapiro DH Jr, Valsh RN, eds. Meditation, classic and contemporary perspectives. Piscataway, NJ: Aldine Transaction, 2008:345–7. 13. Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Attention reg- ulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn Sci 2008;12:163–9. 14. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, et al. Mindfulness: a pro- posed operational definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2004;11: 230–41. 15. Chambers R, Gullone E, Allen NB. Mindful emotion reg- ulation: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev 2009; 29:560–72. 16. Brefczynski-Lewis JA, Lutz A, Schaefer HS, Levinson DB, Davidson RJ. Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104:11483–8. 17. Farb NAS, Segal ZV, Mayberg H, et al. Attending to the present: meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self- reference. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2007;2:313–22. 18. Watkins E, Teasdale JD. Rumination and overgeneralization memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110:353–7. 19. Linehan MM. Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford, 1993. 20. Hayes SC. Acceptance and change: content and context in psychotherapy. Reno, NV: Context, 1994. 21. Marlatt GA, Witkiewitz K, Dillworth TM, et al. Vipassana meditation as a treatment for alcohol and drug use disor- ders. In: Hayes SC, Follette VM, Linehan MM, eds. Mind- fulness and acceptance: expanding the cognitive-behavioral tradition. New York: Guilford, 2004:261–87. 22. Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Williams JM, Ridgeway VA, Soulsby JM, Lau MA. Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major de- pression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000;68:615–23. 23. Wolf DB, Abell N. Examining the effects of meditation tech- niques on psychosocial functioning. Res Soc Work Pract 2003;13:27–42. 24. Shannahoff-Khalsa DS. Kundalini Yoga meditation tech- niques for the treatment of obsessive-compulsive and OC spectrum disorders. Brief Treat Crisis Interv 2003;3:369–82. 25. Marlatt GA, Marques JK. Meditation, self-control and alco- hol use. In: Stuart RB, ed. Behavioral self-management: strategies, techniques and outcome. New York: Brunner/ Mazel, 1977:117–53. 26. Linehan MM, Comtois KA, Murray AM, et al. Two-year randomized controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical be- havior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal behaviors and borderline personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;62:1–10. 27. Fjorback LO, Walch H. Meditation based therapies—a sys- tematic review and some clinical observations. Religions 2012;3:1–18. 28. Kabat-Zinn J. Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion, 1994. 29. Teasdale JD, Segal Z, Williams JM. How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should atten- tional control (mindfulness training) help? Behav Res Ther 1995;33:25–39. 30. Kisteller JL, Hallett CB. An exploratory study of meditation- based intervention for binge eating disorder. J Health Psychol 1999;4:357–63. 31. Kozak A. Wild chickens and petty tyrants: 108 metaphors for mindfulness. Somerville, MA: Wisdom, 2009. 32. Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Biegel GM. Teaching self-care to caregivers: effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in training. Train Educ Prof Psychol 2007;1:105–15. 33. Kornfield J. A path with heart: a guide through the perils and promises of spiritual life. New York: Bantam, 1993. 34. Gilbert P. Compassion: conceptualizations, research and use in psychotherapy. New York: Routledge, 2006. 35. Henry WP, Schacht TE, Strupp HH. Patient and therapist in- troject, interpersonal process, and differential psychother- apy outcome. J Consult Clin Psychol 1990;58:768–74. 36. Rubia K. The neurobiology of meditation and its clinical effec- tiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychol 2009;82:1–11. 37. Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cogni- tion: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci 2010;14:277–90. C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 216 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 13. 38. Manna A, Raffone A, Perrucci MG, et al. Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in medita- tion. Brain Res Bull 2010;82:46–56. 39. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98:676–82. 40. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Thermenos HW, Milanovic S, et al. Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of person with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:1279–84. 41. Murias M, Webb SJ, Greenson J, Dawson G. Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individ- uals with autism. Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:270–3. 42. Uddin LQ, Menon V. The anterior insula in autism: under- connected and under-examined. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009;33:1198–203. 43. Liao W, Zhang Z, Pan Z, et al. Default mode network ab- normalities in medial temporal lobe epilepsy: a study com- bining fMRI and DRT. Hum Brain Mapp 2011;32:883–95. 44. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM. Default mode network activ- ity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psy- chol 2012;8:49–76. 45. Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP. Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:318–27. 46. Feinstein JS, Stein MB, Paulus MP. Anterior insula reactiv- ity during certain decisions is associated with neuroticism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2006;1:136–42. 47. Daniels JK, McFarlane AC, Bluhm RL, et al. Switching be- tween executive and default mode networks in post trau- matic stress disorder: alteration in functional connectivity. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2010;35:258–66. 48. Goenka SN. Mahasatipatthana sutta: the great discourse on establishing awareness. Seattle, WA: Vipassana Research In- stitute, 1996. 49. Hanh TN. The miracle of mindfulness. Boston, MA: Beacon, 1976. 50. Deitrich A. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: the transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Con- scious Cogn 2003;12:231–56. 51. Lou HC, Norwak M, Kjaer TW. The mental self. Prog Brain Res 2005;150:197–204. 52. Luders E, Kurth F, Mayer EA, Toga AW, Narr KL, Gaser C. The unique brain anatomy of meditation practitioners: alterations in cortical gyrification. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:34. 53. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman R, et al. Meditation experi- ence is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neu- roreport 2005;16:1983–7. 54. Hozel BK, Ott U, Gard T, et al. Investigation of mindful- ness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphome- try. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2008;3:55–61. 55. Luders E, Toga A, Lepore N, Gaser C. The underlying ana- tomical correlates of long-term meditation: larger hippo- campal and frontal volumes of grey matter. Neuroimage 2009;45:673–8. 56. Grant JA, Courtemanche J, Duerden EG, Duncan GH, Rainville P. Cortical thickness and pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Emotion 2010;10:43–53. 57. Baerensten KB, Stodklide-Jorgensen H, Sommerlund B, et al. An investigation of brain processes supporting medita- tion. Cogn Process 2010;11:57–84. 58. Critchley HD. Neural mechanisms of automatic, affective, and cognitive integration. J Comp Neurol 2005;495;154–66. 59. Kurth F, Zilles K, Fox PT, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB. A link be- tween the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct 2010;214:519–34. 60. Mutschler I, Wiekhorst B, Kowalevski S, et al. Functional organization of the human anterior cingulated cortex. Neu- rosci Lett 2009;457–60. 61. Damasio AR. The feeling of what happens: body an emo- tion in the making of consciousness. Orlando, FL: Harcourt, 1994. 62. Niedermeyer E, da Silva FL. Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2004. 63. Lutz A, Greischar L, Rawlings N, Ricard M, Davidson J. Long term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004;101:16369–73. 64. Sheppard WD. Pretrial EEF coherence as a predictor of se- mantic processing effects in a lexical decision test. Disserta- tion Abstr Int 1998;49:B3610. 65. Sheppard WD, Boyer RW. Pretrial EEG coherence as a pre- dictor of semantic priming effects. Brain Lang 1990;39:37–68. 66. Cardin JA, Carlen M, Meletis K, et al. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and control sensory responses. Nature 2009:459:663–7. 67. Baldeweg T, Spence S, Hirsch SR, Gruzelier J. Gamma-band electroencephalographic oscillations in a patient with so- matic hallucinations. Lancet 1998;352:620–1. 68. Leeuwen S, Singer W, Melloni L. Meditation increases the depth of information processing and improves the allocation of attention in space. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:133. 69. Lama D, Cutler HC. The art of happiness: a handbook for living. New York: Riverhead, 2009. 70. Jacobs TL, Epel ES, Lin J, et al. Intensive meditation train- ing, immune cell telomerase activity, and psychological med- itators. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011;36:664–81. 71. Blackburn E. Structure and function of telomeres. Nature 1991;350:569–73. 72. Frenck RW, Blackburn EH. The rate of telomere sequence loss in human leukocytes varies with age. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998;95:5607–10. 73. Fossel M. Role of cell senescence in human aging. J Anti- aging Med 2000;3:91–8. 74. Epel ES. Telomeres in a life-span perspective: a new “psy- chobiomarker”? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009;18:6–10. 75. Cawthon RK, Smith E, O’Brien E, Sivatchenko A, Kerber RA. Association between telomere length in blood and mortality in people aged 60 years of older. Lancet 2003; 361:393–5. 76. Njajou OT, Hsueh WC, Blackburn EH, et al. Association between telomere lengths, specific causes of death, and years of healthy life in health, aging, and body composition, a population-based cohort study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2009;64:860–4. 77. Epel ES, Merkin SS, Cawthon R, et al. The rate of leukocyte telomere shortening predicts mortality from cardiovascular disease in elderly men. Aging 2008;1:81–8. 78. Nordfjall K, Svenson U, Norrback KF, Adolfsson R, Lenner P, Roos G. The individual blood cell telomere attrition rate is telomere length dependent. PLoS Genet 2009;5: e1000375. 79. Farzaneh-Far R, Lin J, Epel E, Lapham K, Blackburn E. Telomere length trajectory and its determinants in persons with coronary artery disease: longitudinal findings from the heart and soul study. PLoS One 2010;5:e8612. 80. Epel ES, Blackburn EH, Lin J, et al. Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004;101:17312–5. Concentrative, Diffuse, and Insight Meditation Harvard Review of Psychiatry www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org 217 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
  • 14. 81. Epel ES, Lin J, Wilhelm FH, et al. Cell aging in relation to stress arousal and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Psy- choneuroendocrinology 2006;31:277–87. 82. Porges SW. Cardiac vagal tone: a physiological index of stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995;19:225–33. 83. Carney RM, Blumenthal JA, Stein PK. Depression, heart rate variability, and acute myocardial infarction. Circulation 2001; 104:2024–8. 84. Seligman MEP. Learned helplessness. Annu Rev Med 1972;23: 407–12. 85. Averill JR. Personal control over aversive stimuli and its re- lationship to stress. Psychol Bull 1973;80:286–303. 86. Wallston KA, Wallston BS, Smith S, Dobbins CJ. Perceived control and health. Curr Psychol Res Rev 1987;6:5–25. 87. Vollmayr B, Henn FA. Learned helplessness in the rat: improvements in validity and reliability. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 2001;8:1–7. 88. Mausbach BT, Patterson TL, von Kanel R, et al. Personal mastery attenuates the effect of caregiving stress on psychi- atric morbidity. J Nerv Ment Dis 2006;194:132–4. 89. Mausbach BT, von Kanel R, Patterson TL, et al. The mod- erating effect of personal mastery on the relations between stress and plamninogen activator inhibitor-1 (PAI-1) antigen. Health Psychol 2008;27:S172–9. 90. Krause N, Stryker S. Stress and well-being: the buffering role of locus of control beliefs. Soc Sci Med 1984;18:783–90. 91. Litt MD. Cognitive mediators of stressful experience: self effi- cacy and perceived control. Cognit Ther Res 1988;12:241–60. 92. Chorpita BF, Barlow DH. 1998. The development of anxi- ety: the role of control in early development. Psychol Bull 1998;124:3–21. 93. Brody S, Veit R, Rau H. Neuroticism but not cardiovascu- lar stress reactivity is associated with less longitudinal blood pressure increase. Pers Individ Dif 1996;20:375–80. 94. Schneider TR. The role of neuroticism on psychological and physiological stress responses. J Exp Soc Psychol 2004;40: 795–804. 95. Shurgot GR, Knight BG. Influence of neuroticism, ethnic- ity, familism, and social support on perceived burden in dementia caregivers: pilot test of the transactional stress and social support model. J Gerentol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2005;60B:331–4. 96. Suls J. Affect, stress and personality. In: Forgas JP, ed. Hand- book of affect and social cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 2001:392–409. 97. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindful- ness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1982;4:33–47. 98. Fredrickson BL, Cohn MA, Coffey KA, Pek J, Finkel SM. Open hearts build lives: positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. J Pers Soc Psychol 2008; 5:1045–62. 99. Pace TW, Negi LT, Adame DD, et al. Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune, and behav- ioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocri- nology 2009;34:87–98. 100. Brand S, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Naranjo JR, Schmidt S. In- fluence of mindfulness practice on cortisol and sleep in long-term and short-term meditators. Neuropsychobiology 2012;65:109–18. 101. National Institutes of Health. At http://www.nimh.nih.gov/ statistics/index.shtml 102. Buddhaghosa B; Nyanamoli B, trans. The path of purifica- tion: Visuddhimagga. Kandy, Sri Lanka: Buddhist Publica- tion Society, 1991. 103. Piyadhassi Bhikkhu. Vimuttidhamma: from chakra to dham- machakra. Thailand: Dhamma Publication Fund, 2011. 104. Ingram D. Mastering the core teachings of the Buddha: an unusually hardcore Dhamma book. London: Aeon, 2008. 105. Wilber K, Engler J, Brown DP. Transformations of con- sciousness: conventional and contemplative perspectives on development. Boston, MA: Shambala, 1986. C. Mehrmann and R. Karmacharya 218 www.harvardreviewofpsychiatry.org Volume 21 • Number 4 • July/August 2013 Copyright @ 2013 President and Fellows of Harvard College. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.